Open access peer-reviewed chapter

Integration of Motor, Cognitive, Nutritional, Metabolic, and Epigenetic Influence Variables Related to Early Childhood as a Tool to Promote Child Development at Kindergarten Schools

Written By

Ana Paula Dantas Passos

Submitted: 17 August 2023 Reviewed: 07 September 2023 Published: 03 November 2023

DOI: 10.5772/intechopen.113145

From the Edited Volume

Recent Perspectives on Preschool Education and Care

Edited by Hülya Şenol

Chapter metrics overview

48 Chapter Downloads

View Full Metrics

Abstract

Child development comprises interdependent dimensions which embrace physiological adjustments to disturbances caused by epigenetic modulations of genes in response to physical and social environmental influences, which, in turn, shape the health of children during their development, and reflect on their learning, behavior, and physical and mental well-being through their life span. It is a dynamic process in which children turns from a totally dependent on their caregiver to a human being who responds to one who perceptions in a planned, organized, and independent way. Promoting the conditions for a children’s healthy development depends on knowing how it manifests in different aspects through their development. In the present work, cognition, motricity, nutrition, metabolism, and epigenetics during child development were studied in an integrative and multidisciplinary manner based on the last 40 years of research on child development; with the use of sensitive periods as parameters, whose plasticity is greater than in any other period in life, and which is translated into windows of opportunity for healthy interventions to suggest stimuli according to a specific milestone, democratizing such knowledge, thus making it accessible and functional to parents, teachers, and caregivers of children from zero to 6 years of age.

Keywords

  • child development
  • nutrition
  • motor development
  • cognition
  • epigenetics
  • childhood
  • education

1. Introduction

As a teacher in high school education, my intent to find answers to my questions about behavior and learning difficulties my students used to have made me think about the failure I was experiencing trying to help them. What was missing in my approach? As a biologist, I know that an individual has his genes, the environment can regulate their expression, and the development of each living being has a unique complexity. Then I realized that if I wanted to help my students, I would need to research beyond the answers in the classroom. It was when I began to study more deeply how learning development occurs in humans, including in a comparative aspect, which revealed to me the complexity of the process. Sharing my experiences with a great friend, who also investigated education for a longer time, together with a team, we developed a material in order to experience another way of teaching and learning in the classroom. Working with that material with my students in high school, in some aspects, such as the movements, I observed surprising changes in students that presented learning difficulties because they developed the pleasure to learn and discover. This experience was replicated in public schools in Monteiro Lobato city, in middle school, with equally surprising results. These experiments were carried out in 2007 in high school and in 2011 in middle school and published in an article in 2014 [1].

Continuing with our studies, we decided to investigate more about childhood: how far things I observed in students in high school would be the result of experiences up to 6 years of age? We agreed that I would research the physiological aspects of development. That is when I organized the knowledge about child development in Physiology, under nutrition, cognition, motricity, metabolism, and epigenetics, produced in academic institutions, in order to process all this knowledge into a language easy enough to people not belonging to these institutions could understand it, even those who knew a little about child development. After all, a child’s development depends mainly on the environment that adults around him offer him.

Advertisement

2. Physiology under an integrated approach of systems and definitions

A living system is an open, self-organized system in which there is a continuous exchange of matter and energy with the environment so that life persists. A complex interdependent system that floats and oscillates within limits controls the internal state of plasticity and flexibility. In an attempt to understand this complex system, the biological sciences adopted Descartes’ metaphor, which places the functioning of the world as a machine. Over time, this metaphor has been replaced as a truth, simplifying a complex question and dividing it into parts that add up when they are interconnected, forming a whole greater than the sum of its parts. Studying a living system under this mechanistic reductionism limits its understanding, for there is no single obvious way to divide an organism into organs that are appropriate for a causal analysis of different functions: The organism is a nexus of a large number of weakly determined forces, none is dominant. The separation of causes and effects becomes problematic because organic processes have a historical contingency that hinders universal and generalist explanations [2]. The actions and events that occur since the emergence of the individual are cumulative and influence throughout life: in the way they self-organize, grow, develop, adapt, reproduce, repair, and maintain form and function, age, and die.

Development, in the context of this work, is considered as an unfolding of something already present and somehow pre-formed: The ontogeny of an organism is the consequence of a singular interaction between the genes that it carries, the temporal sequence of the external environments through which it passes during life and random events of molecular interactions between the cells of the individual [2]. Studies, in both humans and animals, reveal that factors that act in early life influence energy balance in the long term [3]. One way to study this energy balance is by measuring the basal metabolism of individuals.

Basal metabolism is the energy expended by cellular and tissue processes to ensure the maintenance of life [4], which is different in adults and children. Basal metabolism in children is proportional to body mass rather than following the coefficient of 0.75 identified in adults. During childhood, basal metabolism mainly corresponds to the activities of the brain, liver, heart, and kidneys. In the newborn, for example, the contribution of the brain to basal metabolism is about 87%. During the first year of life is approximately 53 to 64%. In the case of chemical processes, such as protein synthesis and ion pumping, it is about 2/3 or more of basal metabolism. Significant changes occur in body mass composition during the first year of life. Therefore, the energy cost of growth varies during childhood [5]. When the proportions related to body mass composition stabilize at the end of early childhood (from 0 to 6 years), the basal metabolism coefficient is 0.75, as identified in adults. Before this point, preparing meals for a child is very different compared to an adult: Nutritional needs are very different and vary enormously during the first year according to the specific patterns of growth of the baby and the amount of motor stimulation. In addition, the baby performs various physiological adjustments, adjusts the immune system, and needs adequate nutrition to survive. The type and consistency of food change as the gastrointestinal system matures, becoming capable of metabolizing components and excreting metabolites from complex foods. The introduction of solid foods should occur parallel to the changes that occur in the development of the central nervous system during the first year, which provides a level of readiness for the baby to cope with foods of various textures, from liquid to soft [6]. In addition, human studies and animal experiments demonstrate the crucial role of nutrition in neuronal development: There is strong evidence that the diet of pregnant women, babies, and children has a long-term influence on cognitive development [7].

Considering as a general principle that the central nervous system is very vulnerable when it is developing rapidly, such a period known as a “growth spurt,” nutrition has the greatest effect on brain development during the perinatal period, between the third trimester of pregnancy and the first months of a human baby’s life. During this period of rapid growth, neural events occur according to a well-established schedule, so the effects of nutrition depend on when these events happen [8, 9]. Any change in the basic neuronal structure brought about by nutrition seems to have a long-term effect. Humans do not passively respond to environments in which they are: Cognitive processes affect environment choices and cognition is implicated in food choices [9]. Therefore, an adequate balance of energy and essential nutrients are dietary requirements for proper growth and development during childhood.

When cognitive development is disturbed, motor development is often affected. Motor development and cognitive development can be fundamentally interconnected and exhibit equally extended “development schedules” [8]. Motor development in children can be described as a dynamic process in which new forms of movement emerge from intrinsic processes and through interaction with the environment [10]. The assessment of a child’s motor development has been considered one of the most important indicators of possible problems in neurological development [11]. Motor behavior involves more than performing muscle activity, joints, and strength. Adjustments in motor behavior to what the body needs about the environment require perception, planning, decision-making, learning, and discovery of new strategies. Motor development comprises the development of the body, brain, and interaction with the environment [12].

Before moving forward, it is necessary to make a parenthesis to clarify some concepts used here that, sometimes, in many readings, are used as synonyms, such as adaptation and adjustment. According to Schmitd-Nielsen [4], the concept of adaptation refers to behavioral or genetic changes that occur in species over generations, while it adjusts to the changes that occur in the individual, i.e., a single generation adjusts itself to changes in the environment. Therefore, when the word adaptation is being used by authors as a synonym for adjustment, in this work, the term will be corrected into adjustment. The same was observed for the concepts of growth and development. This work follows the definitions given [13]: Growth refers to observable changes in quantity, that is, the increase in body size due to successive cell divisions; processes of change in the level of functioning of the individual as a consequence of their growth. For this reason, the term child development was chosen, not growth, because child development is the specialization of functions over the first 6 years of life.

Advertisement

3. Historical perspective of child development studies

3.1 Preformationism theory

For centuries, childhood was disregarded: Children were like miniatures of fully formed adults. According to Àries [14], people treated children like adults in the Middle Ages. When they were 6 or 7, children were taken to other villages to work as apprentices, wore the same clothes as adults, and went to taverns with their parents. Children under the age of 7 used to receive protection and care. However, people were indifferent to the stimuli provided to children in this age group, such as motor and speech development [14]. According to Ausubel & Sullivan [15], the fundamental thesis of preformationism denies the importance of development in human ontogeny. All characteristics, whether physical, motor, cognitive, or emotional, are pre-existing and pre-formed at birth.

The change in the preformationist view was gradual and began approximately in the 1500s when the world of work showed signs of change. With the invention of the mechanical press, the growth of trade and economic markets, and the emergence of cities and national states, new job opportunities began to emerge: merchants, lawyers, bankers, journalists, government officials, and occupations that required knowledge of reading, writing, and mathematics. Members of a growing middle class began to discredit that a person’s place in society was predetermined by birth. They saw, in children, the possibility of ascending socially, providing their children with the necessary academic education for these new jobs. This new demand for education caused the number of schools in Europe to grow greatly in the sixteenth and seventeenth centuries [16]. The child, then, was no longer seen as a being prepared for the adult’s world, but as someone who needed to be separated from this world to receive an education: The child was seen less as an adult and more as a future adult [14] but not as a child yet.

3.2 The importance of the first years of life

The first to highlight the importance of childhood care was John Locke (1632–1704). In his book “Some Concerns About Education,” from 1693, the author points out that “the smallest or hardly felt an impression in our early childhood is paramount and leaves consequences” [17]; and also highlights the care of the child, which “should be, not what a doctor should do with a sick and agitated child; but what parents, without the help of a doctor, could do to prevent possible diseases and maintain a healthy constitution of their children” [17]. For him, experience is where all knowledge is founded and from where we derive. The environment shapes the mind, and its influence [of the environment] is stronger in the first years of life, a period when one can mold the child for his whole life. According to Locke, many of our thoughts and feelings develop by association, behaviors, by repetition, and we learn by imitation and rewards and punishments. These three factors together would shape character [16, 17].

Although John Locke wrote a philosophy for education and its principles [16] and, in his book, dedicated itself to the physical care he should have with children in the early years, he did not distinguish the stages of child development.

3.3 Child development by stages

Jean Jacques Rousseau (1712–1778) was the first to describe child development as independent of the influences of the environment and as something that happened according to an internal, biological timeline in the eighteenth century. Children were not shaped only by external forces but also grew and learned a lot by themselves following a plan [16]. Rousseau suggested that development unfolded in a series of stages, periods in which the child experienced the world in different ways. That was the difference between a child and an adult: They were not blank canvases to be filled with the teachings of adults, but the patterns of thought and behavior of each child would have unique characteristics of each stage [16, 18]. For Rousseau [18], each child should be treated according to his stage. He also did not believe in the “power of the environment,” especially the social environment, to form a healthy individual. According to Rousseau, instead of teaching the child to think “correctly,” we should allow him to perfect his own abilities and learn by his own means, to rely on his own judgment power [16, 18].

A century after Rousseau, the British naturalist Charles Darwin (1809–1882), after his expedition to distant parts of the world aboard the Beagle, wrote the foundations of what would become the Theory of Evolution, with emphasis on the adaptive value of physical characteristics and behavior for the survival of the individual. During his exploration, Darwin discovered that the prenatal growth of many species was surprisingly similar. From this observation of Darwin, other researchers followed the same general plan for the evolution of the human species, carefully observing all aspects of the child’s behavior, and conducting scientific studies on child development [19].

Influenced by the Theory of Evolution and studies in genetics, Arnold Gesell (1880–1961) was the pioneer in studies on biological maturation and the first to develop tests for child intelligence [16].

According to Gesell, maturation refers to the processes by which development is governed by intrinsic factors, mainly genes, which are chemicals contained in the nucleus of each cell. Genes determine the sequence, timing, and emergence of action patterns in conjunction with environmental factors. For this author, child development is influenced by two main forces: the environment, which would be the external force, and genes, which would be the internal force. For Gesell, babies come into the world with an internal clock, which is the product of 3 million years of biological evolution. Therefore, they are preeminently “wise” about their needs and what they are or are not ready to do. Gesell emphasized that the first year of life is the best time to learn about the child’s individuality and that parents, despite an intuitive sensitivity to the child, needed some theoretical knowledge about child development [16, 20].

3.4 Sensitive periods (windows of opportunity)

In 1891, the term “sensitive periods” was first used by botanist Hugo De Vries (1848–1935) when describing a specific period when poppy flowers were sensitive to the external environment to the point of causing the modification of stamen (male structure of a flower) in the secondary pistil (female structure of a flower). According to De Vries, this sensitive period comprised the seed phase or the first weeks of life of the young plant [21]. Contemporary of De Vries, Maria Montessori (1870–1952), in her book “Il Secreto dell’Infanzia”1936, cites De Vries as the author of the term and uses it to determine the periods that are genetically programmed in time blocks during which the child is especially able to learn certain tasks very well. For example, there are periods for the development of language and the beginning of the use of hands [22]. The concept of “sensitive periods” is a central component of child development for Montessori [16]. According to the author, if the child is prevented from enjoying the experiences at the time when nature planned for him, the “special sensitivity that draws” will disappear, having a disturbing effect on development [23].

Studies conducted on fish embryos in the early twentieth century showed that there were critical periods when interference with the embryonic development of these animals, such as food deprivation, would have “more serious results” than in any other period [24]. Stockard [24] does not explain the term “critical period” per se, but is the first to use it in a scientific work to explain periods in the development of a species in which interventions would be more or less determinant in the development of the individual and that the processes of maturation in different parts would happen at different rates. This concept was adopted by neurobiology, whose research revealed that different regions of the nervous system of children matured in different periods [25]. The effect of a given stimulus (or experience) on development depends on when it occurs during that development, and in most cases, there is a window of opportunity in which a particular stimulus can influence it. If a certain stimulus must happen at a particular interval of development, this interval is called a “critical period.” However, when a particular stimulus needs to occur at a certain interval, but still influences, even if more leniently, outside that interval, that larger interval is called the “sensitive period.” Although the term “critical period” is more used, most of the effects of stimuli occur in sensitive periods [26].

The concept behind this term is that certain windows are opened to the effect of external experiences, from birth, closing one by one, with increasing age, because of the decline of the plasticity of the brain. There are windows to the development of motor control, vision, feelings, and language. In theory, if the child loses an opportunity, he can no longer develop the neuronal circuit with all its potential for a specific function [13], as described by Montessori. This is because if a neuronal circuit, once formed, is not used, it can be lost and not work properly—it is the principle of “use it or lose it.” One of the advantages of human brain development taking a long time is that many stimulation opportunities and experiences can be promoted to enable fine-tuning of development [26].

3.5 Epigenetics and critical periods

According to the Origin of Development hypothesis, during critical developmental periods (prenatal and postnatal mammals), nutrition and other environmental stimuli influence the pathways through which development will follow. Numerous studies show an association between low birth weight and the incidence of cardiovascular disease, hypertension, type 2 diabetes, and deficiencies in insulin metabolism and serum cholesterol concentrations in adults. From the point of view of epigenetics (the study of heritable traits that are not associated with changes in the sequence of nucleotides – which form genes, the basic physical and functional unit of heredity – but with chemical changes in DNA, which is made up by genes, or regulatory and structural proteins that are linked to it [27, 28]), child development could be defined as “carved experiences into DNA of an organism through one of the main epigenetic mechanisms of change: methylation.” DNA methylation corresponds to the chemical transfer of a methyl group to a CpG sequence (cytosine-phosphate-guanine) in one of the DNA strands. This mechanism is generally related to gene silencing, whether of paternal or maternal origin, during embryogenesis, as well as after birth [29, 30, 31]. The DNA methylation machinery sets the standards during development and possibly during adulthood in response to new signals from the environment, which are renewed when the genome is duplicated in mitosis [32, 33, 34, 35, 36]. Therefore, child development could be defined as a dynamic interaction between the environment and the child and mediated by a series of epigenetic modifications of specific genes that would result in physiological, stable, and persistent cognitive, emotional, and behavioral. Fraga and collaborators [37], studying monozygotic twins, discovered substantial epigenetic differences between them. According to van Ijzendoorn and collaborators [30], this was possible because identical twins were exposed to different environments and, because of this, would no longer be identical. Many studies suggest that childhood trauma may induce depression, anxiety, and post-traumatic stress response by epigenetic regulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, which is responsible for stress response [38]. Epigenetic changes would be the molecular mechanism by which the environment would affect the physiology and behavior of a developing child, and development would incorporate environmentally induced signatures into the epigenome [3037], whose modifications could be transmitted throughout generations. Heijmans and collaborators [39], studying individuals who were exposed to famine during Dutch Hunger Winter in 1944–1945, showed that those individuals, 6 decades later, had less DNA methylation of the imprinted gene IGF-2 (related to insulin response to glucose so to overweight, obesity, and type 2 diabetes [40]), compared to their same-sex unexposed siblings. Early-life environmental conditions can contribute to epigenetic modifications that affect the individuals throughout their life and their next generation.

Advertisement

4. The importance of an integrative approach of child development

Considering development as an unfolding of something already present and somehow preformed, the ontogeny of an organism is the consequence of a singular interaction between the genes it carries, the temporal sequence of the external environments through which it passes during life, and random events of molecular interactions between the cells of the individual [2]. Studies, both in humans and in other animals, reveal that factors that act in early life influence the long-term energy balance [3]. Nutritional physiology often undergoes programmed changes throughout development as individuals mature from birth to adulthood [41]. Considering human beings, movements depend on biological and psychological environments as on conditions of the environment where they live and are built throughout their lives [42], which is no different for any other animal. Therefore, the first years of life are essential for the foundation of later development: The young and the adult are the result of the quality of care they had in childhood.

Child development includes interdependent dimensions that encompass social, emotional, cognitive, psychomotor and patterns of behavior, and nutrition [43]. According to Fonseca [44], for Piaget, all cognitive mechanisms are based on and emerge from the development of motor skills, which occurs in early childhood (from 0 to 6 years of age). Fonseca [44] states that psychomotricity helps the child to acquire sensations, perceptions, and concepts that will allow him to know his own body and, from it, the world around him. Ivanovic and collaborators [45] found that the differences in IQ among Chilean high school students were mainly due to malnutrition in childhood. According to Cadavid Castro [46], it is increasingly clear that the quantity and quality of nutrients received by the child are directly related to their cognitive potential and their mental and emotional health, in addition to reflecting on adult brain functions and their eventual decline with age. Childhood is the crucial phase in which many health-related behaviors are shaped [47], and a child does not choose the environment in which he lives, much less has the skill and knowledge necessary to choose a diet or exercise. According to the World Health Organization, only 3% of cases of type 2 diabetes and obesity were observed in children in the United States in the mid-1970s [43]. Currently, this proportion reaches 45% of cases, with alarming rates in other countries, including Brazil. Children depend on their parents to have a healthy childhood: Parents who know which nutritional and psychomotor factors are important to a child can educate them to be active, with full development of their cognitive and motor abilities. In the future, this child will be able to choose his own path, such as dedicating himself to sports or “books,” for example, and not simply exclude one for not feeling able to perform the other [48].

The passage from a limited motor repertoire of the newborn to complex motor skills and manipulation of the child is among the most dramatic and visible transformations in the human life cycle. Because of this, many researchers believed that these transitions could promote a model of understanding the development of higher cognitive functions or even be underlying higher functions [49].

However, the scenario for the promotion of child development around the world is far from the best: At least 200 million children under the age of five cannot develop their cognitive and socio-emotional potential, basically because of nutritional deficiencies, malnutrition, and inadequate stimuli in the first years of life. Each of these related factors leads to a determining effect on the development of the child. But when two or more of these factors are found together, the combined impact of the factors is even more severe [50]. Understanding how all these factors are interrelated (cognition, motricity, nutrition, metabolism, and factors of epigenetic influence) can contribute to the further understanding of the complexity of human development, which is far beyond studying each phase or each aspect separately, but first of all in knowing how each factor interacts with each other throughout life [51, 52, 53, 54]. We have a big problem ahead of us. The kind of problem is defined and, along with it, the seriousness of its long-term consequences. A response is needed from the world and from us [50].

Advertisement

5. Contributions to pedagogical practices with children from 0 to 6 years of age

Nutrition in the early years of life can affect cognitive performance in later ages [755]. The rapid growth of the brain during the last 3 months of gestation and 2 years of life makes it a vulnerable organ to dietary inadequacies during the first years of life and may create future cognitive and motor problems. In addition, micronutrients are essential for metabolism and, in particular, cell division and tissue growth, such as nervous, muscular, and skeletal [56]. A child with learning or motor difficulties does not necessarily suffer from cognitive problems—they can be of a nutritional order [45], as shown in Table 1. It is necessary to provide the child with the correct food, in the necessary quantities, respecting its development, including the digestive system and the microbiota, whose development is completed around 4 years of age [113]. Therefore, it is paramount that the child receives adequate nutrition. It means receiving the correct amount of nutrients in the required period of development, both at home and at school.

NutrientFunctionBehaviorReferences
CalciumBone formation; absorption of iron.Motor and learning difficulties; passivity; apathy.[57, 58, 59, 60, 61, 62, 63]
IronHemoglobin formation (oxygen transportation).Anemia; pallor; weakness; apathy; learning difficulties; attention deficit; tiredness.[62, 64, 65, 66, 67, 68, 69]
IodineFormation of thyroid hormonesMental retardation; learning difficulties; impairment of physical development.[70, 71, 72, 73, 74]
ZincImmunity; iron absorption; memory.Low immunity; growth retardation; tiredness; weakness; learning difficulties.[75, 76, 77, 78, 79, 80]
Vitamin AFormation of pigments of vision; memory (plasticity of the hippocampus); protection of nerve cells against free radicals; immunity.Learning difficulties; low immunity.[81, 82, 83, 84, 85, 86, 87]
Vitamin DCalcium absorption by bones; immunity.Rickets; motor difficulties; learning difficulties; apathy; sadness; low immunity.[71, 88, 89]
Vitamin EAntioxidant; cell division; immunity.Motor difficulties (muscle pain), weakness; learning difficulties.[71, 90]
Vitamin KBlood coagulation; regulation of calcium in bones.Motor difficulties; bruises that appear easily and long-lasting.[91]
Vitamin CAntioxidant; iron absorption; immunity.Weakness; fatigue; apathy; low immunity.[92, 93, 94, 95, 96]
ThiamineFacilitates the use of glucose by the brain.Learning difficulties (abstract thinking); irritability.[82, 97, 98, 99]
Niacin and pyridoxinMetabolism of amino acids; release of glucose in the muscle; modulation of steroid hormones.Weakness; irritability; nervousness; difficulty sleeping; motor difficulties.[82, 91, 100, 101, 102]
RiboflavinCellular respiration; intermediate metabolism; iron absorption.Inflammation of the mouth; thrush; blowtorch.[71, 102, 103]
Folic acidIron absorption; amino acid synthesis.Anemia; dermatitis; weakness; depression; learning and socialization difficulties.[91, 103, 104]
Cobalamin (B12)Cellular respiration; synthesis of some amino acids.Anemia; demyelination; learning difficulties; attention deficit.[71, 91, 102, 105]
ColineCell membrane formation; water balance; immunity.Liver problems; muscle damage; motor difficulties.[105]
ProteinsFormation of enzymes and muscle proteins.Motor difficulties; tiredness, apathy; learning difficulties; attention deficit.[71, 106]
PUFAsFormation of the cell membrane; part of various hormones; myelination; immunity.Learning difficulties; attention deficit; irritability; hormonal disorders; low immunity.[107, 108, 109, 110, 111, 112]

Table 1.

Observable behaviors in children with some nutritional deficiency.

For each nutrient in the table, behaviors can be observed when one of them is deficient. The functions and respective bibliography are also described.

In addition to nutrition, creating a stimulating environment for development is also important to actively engage the child’s mind to strengthen his neuronal network [114]. The play offers broad physical, emotional, cognitive, and social benefits, as it allows: (i) the development of motor skills, communication, creativity and problem-solving, social competence, and resilience; (ii) promotes alternative scenarios for the experience of their social repertoire; and (iii) signals for positive and negative behaviors resulting from the child’s development process. Free play is of vital importance for healthy childhood development recognized by the United Nations as a fundamental right of the child [115]. However, many of them do not receive the benefits of play in its fullness, either because they are pressured by the accelerated lifestyle of parents or because they live in socially vulnerable communities, which interfere directly with academic performance, socialization with other children, and in her relationship with her parents [116].

Detailed knowledge of the mechanisms that control sensitive periods and plasticity, which, in turn, happen during sensitive periods, provides the basis for the development of procedures to help minimize the long-term effects of harmful experiences during early childhood and maximizes the acquisition of motor and cognitive functions once appropriate conditions are restored. Such knowledge can also lead to more effective methods for educating a child so that he can take advantage of the full potential that the nervous system can offer so that the child can learn from his own experiences [117].

The children who come to school are the result of the stimuli they have received so far. An unsatisfactory performance in any activity should not be treated in a simplified way, because several factors are involved in this result and may be signaling other aspects that need to be worked on than academics. Child development is multidisciplinary. Therefore, the approach to poor performance should also be multidisciplinary. In addition, child development depends not only on the maturation of the brain but also on the interactions between the child and the environment around him. Because of this, the observed development results of different children can vary substantially. Learning about the development sequences and the context in which it needs to happen is necessary for understanding possible problems in development to thus plan effective interventions [118]. It is important to plan interventions appropriate to the period of development in which the child is, providing the necessary stimuli so that he can achieve the skills provided for his/her age, instead of being classified based on his/her disabilities. These interventions are simple, as shown in Table 2, where the minimum stimuli required for each age group from 0 to 6 years old are placed.

AgeNutritionCognitionMotricity
0–2 m.o.BreastfeedingSpeak articulately to 20 cm from the baby, look into the eyes, and touch the baby. (He/she knows the very existence from the interaction with the other).Move the baby (Reflex movements).
3–6 m.o.BreastfeedingTalk articulately, look into the eyes, touch the baby, give him/her objects, assemble stimulating environments, play “peek-a-boo.” (He/she knows the very existence from the interaction with the other and with the environment).Move the baby, hold it vertically, and leave objects for the baby to pick up. (Reflex and voluntary movements).
6–12 m.o.Breastfeeding, Baby lead weaning (BLW), no sugar.Talk articulately, look into the eyes, touch the baby, give him/her objects, read to the baby, and set up stimulating environments. (He/she knows the very existence from the interaction with the other and with the environment).Hold him/her vertically, leave distant objects for the baby to pick up, mount an obstacle (Reflex and voluntary movements).
12–18 m.o.Solid foods, no sugar.Talk, speak at eye level, hug, praise, and allow his/her to explore the environment. (He/she is knowing how the environment works).Leave distant objects for the child to pick up, encourage him/her to walk, mount obstacles, and provide boxes and objects of various sizes. (Voluntary movements and balance).
18–24 m.o.Solid foods, no sugar.Talk, speak at eye level, hug, praise, allow him to explore the environment, create safe situations to him/her to say “no”. (He/she is understanding how it himself/herself works in the environment).Ask to pick up objects, encourage him/her to walk faster and climb, provide boxes and objects of various sizes, and throw ball. (Balance, motor coordination).
2 y.o.Eat as same items as an adultTalk, speak at eye level, hug, praise, allow her to explore the environment, go to different places, leave her with other children, and help him/her cope with frustration. (He/ she is understanding the rules of the environment).Ask to pick up objects, encourage him/her to walk faster and climb, provide boxes and objects of various sizes, throw the ball, and let him/her eat alone. (Balance, motor coordination).
3 y.o.Eat same items as an adultTalk, speak at eye level, hug, praise, allow him/her to explore the environment, go to different places, leave him/her with other children, give space for autonomy, help him/her understand his/her feelings. FREE PLAY. (He/she is understanding the existence of the other).Ask to pick up objects, encourage him/her to run and climb, play ball, jump rope, provide boxes and objects of various sizes, and assemble a sensory mural. FREE PLAY. (Balance, motor coordination).
4 y.o.Eat as same items as an adultTalk, speak at eye level, hug, praise, allow her to explore the environment, go to different places, leave her with other children, give space for autonomy, and ask him/her to tell stories. FREE PLAY. (He/she is understanding the existence of space x time).Ask to help with tasks, encourage him/her to run, climb, play ball, jump rope, provide boxes and objects of various sizes, assemble a sensory wall and obstacles, calm him down when he/she wets the bed. FREE PLAY. (Balance, motor enhancement).
5 y.o.Eat as same items as an adultTalk, speak at eye level, hug, praise, allow her to explore the environment, go to different places, leave him/her with other children, give him/her space for autonomy and to help, ask him/her to tell stories and draw how you feel. FREE PLAY. (He/she is understanding how to be in the world).Ask to help with tasks, encourage him/her to run and climb, play ball, jump rope, provide boxes and objects of various sizes, and assemble a sensory wall and obstacles. FREE PLAY. (Balance, motor enhancement).
6 y.o.Eat as same items as an adultTalk, speak at eye level, hug, praise, allow him/her to explore the environment, go to different places, leave him/her with other children, give him/her space for autonomy, ask him/her to tell stories and draw how he/she feels, give him/her problems to solve, and ask him/her for help. FREE PLAY. (He/she is understanding how to act in the world).Ask to help with tasks, encourage him to run, climb, play ball, and jump rope, provide boxes and objects of various sizes, and mount obstacles and challenges. FREE PLAY. (Motor improvement and independence).

Table 2.

Basic nutritional, cognitive, and motor stimuli needed to provide a healthy environment to child development from 0 to 6 years old.

Advertisement

6. Discussion and conclusions

Child development unfolds along individual paths whose continuous and discontinuous trajectories, as well as a series of significant transitions are shaped by the interrelation between the different vulnerabilities and resilience. The moment when these changes occur is very important: Child development is liable to risks and open to protective measures that influence not only the first years of life but also adulthood. What happens during the first years of life is extremely important, not only because it promotes an indelible mark on the well-being of the adult, but also because it determines how robust or fragile the subsequent stages will be [119]. It is also necessary to consider that there is great variability in children’s behaviors—each one is unique—because both genetic factors and the influence of the environment interact to give rise to the individual organism. The basic patterns are determined by heredity, but the genetic determinants are expressed through interactions with various aspects of the environment: The environment provides the energy, substance, and environment to unfold the potentialities of the individual—no individual develops in a vacuum. Stimuli offered to a child have broad consequences on the behavior and physiology of this adult [120]. Variations in the behavior of the mother, for example, directly influence epigenetic processes, in which genes “turn on and off” according to signs of the environment in critical periods of development, impacting the social environment, considering that the genome acts in the transmission of individual differences in response to stress [121]. Democratizing academic knowledge about child development in the form of a tool that can be used by educators, childhood professionals, and parents would be a way to create the conditions for those who work with early childhood, to have the necessary knowledge to provide the appropriate stimuli and environment to provide the child with healthy and full development, or in time to intervene to remedy the problems that will potentially result in difficulties in later stages of the child’s life [122].

Promoting the conditions for the development of the child to occur in a healthy way depends on knowing how it manifests itself in different aspects throughout child development [20, 118, 123]. Paraphrasing Gesell and Gesell, we need to conserve the best in childhood if we want to save to the world the best in youth [20].

Advertisement

Acknowledgments

I am grateful to Pedro Gandolla for helping me to understand how to help my students, José Eduardo Pereira Wilken Bicudo for supervising me in my Ph.D., José Guilherme Chauí-Berlinck for supporting me in the laboratory, and Bioscience Institute of University of Sao Paulo to provide the conditions to accomplish this research.

References

  1. 1. Passos APD, Gandolla P. Vídeo pedagógico interativo como prova de conceito para plataforma inteligente de ensino. Revista Iberoamericana de Estudos em Educação. 2014;9(1):205-217
  2. 2. Lewontin RC. The Triple Helyx. Cambridge: Harvard University Press; 2001. 144 p
  3. 3. Rooney K, Ozanne S. Maternal over-nutrition and offspring obesity predisposition: Targets for preventative interventions. International Journal of Obesity. 2011;35(7):883-890
  4. 4. Schmitd-Nielsen K. Fisiologia Animal: adaptação e meio ambiente. São Paulo: Livraria Editora Santos; 1996. 620 p
  5. 5. Butte F, Wong WW, Garza C. Energy cost of growth during infancy. Proceedings of the Nutrition Society. 1989;48:303-312
  6. 6. Bronner YL, Paige DM. Current concepts in infant nutrition. Journal of Nurse-Midwifery. 1992;37(2):43S-58S
  7. 7. Anjos T et al. Nutrition and neurodevelopment in children: Focus on Nutrimenthe project. European Journal of Nutrition. 2013;52(8):1825-1842
  8. 8. Diamond A. Close interrelation of motor development and cognitive development and of the cerebellum and prefrontal cortex. Child Development. 2000;71(1):44-56
  9. 9. Isaacs E, Oates J. Nutrition and cognition: Assessing cognitive abilities in children and young people. European Journal of Nutrition. 2008;47(Suppl. 3):4-24
  10. 10. Kakebeeke TH et al. Neuromotor development in children. Part 3: Motor performance in 3- to 5-year-olds. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 2013;55(3):248-256
  11. 11. Charitou S, Asonitou K, Koutsouki D. Prediction of infant’s motor development. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences. 2010;9:456-461
  12. 12. Adolph K, Robinson SR. Motor development. In: Kuhn D, Siegler RS, editors. Handbook of Child Psychology: Cognition, Perception, and Language. Vol. 2. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.; 2015. pp. 161-213
  13. 13. Gabbard C. Lifelong Motor Development. 5th ed. San Francisco, CA: Pearson Benjamin Cummings; 2008. 496 p
  14. 14. Àries P. Centuries of Childhood: A Social History of a Family Life. New York: Alfred A. Knopf; 1962. 448 p
  15. 15. Ausubel DP, Sullivan EV. Chapter 2. “Reseña histórica de las tendencias teóricas”. El desarrollo infantil. In: 1. Teorías. Los comienzos del desarrollo. Barcelona: Paidós; 1989. pp. 34-62
  16. 16. Crain W. Theories of Development: Concepts and Applications. Harlow: Pearson Education Limited; 2014. 432 p
  17. 17. Locke J. Some Thoughts Concerning Education. London: s.n.; 1693. 352 p
  18. 18. Rousseau J-J. Émile ou de l’Éducation. London: The Temple Press Letchworth; 1762. 250 p
  19. 19. Berk LE. Child Development. 7th ed. Boston: Pearson Education Inc.; 2006. 784 p
  20. 20. Gesell A, Gesell BC. The Normal Child and Primary Education. Boston: Atheneum Press; 1912. 362 p
  21. 21. De-Vrie H. Species and Varieties: Their Origin by Mutation. Chicago: The Open Coutr Pub. Com; 1904. 888 p
  22. 22. Montessori M, Niño E. el secreto de la infancia. Madrid: Editorial Diana; 2004. 215 p
  23. 23. Montessori M. The Absorbent Mind. Madras: The Theosophical Publishing House; 1949. 320 p
  24. 24. Stockard CR. Developmental rate and structural expression: An experimental study of twins, “double monsters” and single deformities, and the interaction among embrionic organs during their origin and development. Developmental Dynamics. 1921;28(2):115-277
  25. 25. Schore AN. Affect Regulation and the Origin for the Self: The Neurobiology of Emotional Development. New York: Routledge; 2016. 700 p
  26. 26. Pinel JPJ. Biopsychology. 8th ed. Boston: Allyn & Bacon; 2011. 608 p
  27. 27. Felsenfeld GA. Brief history of epigenetics. Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology. 2014;6(a018200):1-10
  28. 28. Waterland RA. Nutrition epigenetics. In: Erdman-Jr JW, Macdonald IA, Zeisel SH, editors. Present Knowledge in Nutrition. 10th ed. Iowa: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.; 2012. pp. 14-27
  29. 29. Murgatroyd C, Spengler D. Epigenetics of early child development. Frontiers in Psychiatry. 2011;2(APR):1-15
  30. 30. Van Ijzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Ebstein RP. Methylation matters in child development: Toward developmental behavioral epigenetics. Child Development Perspectives. 2011;5(4):305-310
  31. 31. Waterland RA, Michels KB. Epigenetic epidemiology of the developmental origins hypothesis. Annual Review of Nutrition. 2007;27(1):363-388
  32. 32. Barker DJP et al. Growth in utero and serum cholesterol concentrations in adult life. BMJ. 1993;307:1524-1527
  33. 33. Hales CN et al. Fetal and infant growth and impaired glucose tolerance at age 64. BMJ. 1991;303:1019-1022
  34. 34. Law CM, Shiell AW. Is blood pressure inversely related to birth weight? The strength of evidence from a systematic review of the literature. Journal of Hypertension. 1996;14:935-941
  35. 35. Malkki R et al. Fetal nutrition and cardiovascular disease in adult life. The Lancet. 1993;341:938-941
  36. 36. Osmond C, Simmonds SJ. Early growth and death from cardiovascular disease in women. BMJ. 1993;307:1519-1524
  37. 37. Fraga MF et al. Epigenetic differences arise during the lifetime of monozygotic twins. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2005;102(30):10604-10609
  38. 38. Ni Y et al. Emerging trends in epigenetic and childhood trauma: Bibliometrics and visual analysis. Frontiers in Psychiatry. 2022;13:925273
  39. 39. Heijmans B et al. Persistent epigenetic differences associated with prenatal exposure to famine in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2008;105(44):17046-17049
  40. 40. Lewitt M et al. The insulin-like growth factor system in obesity, insulin resistance and type 2 diabetes mellitus. Journal of Clinical Medicine. 2014;3:1561-1574
  41. 41. Hill RW, Wyse GA, Anderson M. Animal Physiology. 3th ed. Massachusetts: Sinauer Associates, Inc.; 2012. 725 p
  42. 42. Freire P. Pedagogia da autonomia: saberes necessários à prática educativa. São Paulo: Paz e Terra; 1996. 144 p
  43. 43. FAO/WHO. Vitamin and Mineral Requirements in Human Nutrition: Report of a Joint FAO/OMS Expert Consultation. 2nd ed. Bangkok: WHO Library Cataloguing-in-Publication Data; 2004
  44. 44. Fonseca V. Desenvolvimento psicomotor e aprendizagem. Porto Alegre: ARTMED; 2008. 582 p
  45. 45. Ivanovic DM et al. Nutritional status, brain development and scholastic achievement of Chilean high-school graduates from high and low intellectual quotient and socio-economic status. British Journal of Nutrition. 2007;87(1):81
  46. 46. Cadavid-Castro MA. Inteligencia, alimentación y nutrición en la niñez: revisión. Perspectivas en Nutrición Humana. 2009;11:187-201
  47. 47. Malik K. Human development report. In: The Rise of the South: Human Progress in a Diverse World. New York: s.n.; 2013. p. 92573
  48. 48. Chauí-Berlinck JG, Bicudo JE, Silva M. Para garimpar mais que ouro - uma análise do desempenho olímpico brasileiro. Revista da Biologia. 2014;12(2):16-21
  49. 49. Thelen E, Kelso JAS, Fogel A. Self-organizing systems and infant motor development. Developmental Review. 1987;7(1):39-65
  50. 50. Jolly R. Early childhood development: The global challenge. The Lancet. 2007;369(9555):8-9
  51. 51. Marino E, Pluciennik GA. Primeiríssima infância, da gestação aos três anos: percepções e práticas da sociedade brasileira sobre a fase inicial da vida. São Paulo: Fundação Maria Cecília Souto Vidigal; 2013
  52. 52. Marmot M, Allen JJ. Social determinants of health equity. American Journal of Public Health. 2014;104(Suppl. 4):517-519
  53. 53. Scrimshaw NS. Malnutrition, brain development, learning, and behavior. Nutrition Research. 1998;18(2):351-379
  54. 54. Young ME. Do desenvolvimento da primeira infância ao desenvolvimento humano: investindo no futuro de nossas crianças. São Paulo: Fundação Maria Cecília Souto Vidigal; 2010. 457 p
  55. 55. Stevenson J. Dietary influences on cognitive development and behaviour in children. The Proceedings of the Nutrition Society. 2017;65(4):361-365
  56. 56. Benton D. Neurodevelopment and neurodegeneration: Are there critical stages for nutritional intervention? Nutrition Reviews. 2010;68(Suppl. 1):S6-S10
  57. 57. Allen L, Kerstetter JE. Calcium. In: Caballero B, Allen LH, Prentice A, editors. Encyclopedia of Human Nutrition. 2nd ed. Vol. I. Oxford: Elsevier Inc.; 2005. pp. 253-259
  58. 58. Goulding A. Calcium. In: Mann J, Truswell S, editors. Essentials of Human Nutrition. 2nd ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2002. pp. 129-140
  59. 59. Hallberg L et al. Calcium: Effect of different amounts on nonheme- and heme-iron absorption in humans. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 1991;53(1):112-119
  60. 60. Hidalgo C, Núñez MT. Calcium, iron and neuronal function. IUBMB Life. 2007;59(4-5):280-285
  61. 61. Ilich JZ et al. Nutrition in bone health revisited: A story beyond calcium. Journal of the American College of Nutrition. 2000;19(6):715-737
  62. 62. Miranda M et al. Reducing iron deficiency anemia in Bolivian school children: Calcium and iron combined versus iron supplementation alone. Nutrition. 2014;30(7-8):771-775
  63. 63. Weaver CM. Calcium. In: Erdman J Jr, Macdonald I, Zeisel SH, editors. Present Knowledge in Nutrition. 10th ed. Iowa: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.; 2012. pp. 434-446
  64. 64. Beard J. Iron deficiency alters brain development and functioning. The Journal of Nutrition. 2003;133:1468-1472
  65. 65. Domellöf M. Iron requirements, absorption and metabolism in infancy and childhood. Current Opinion in Clinical Nutrition and Metabolic Care. 2007;10(3):329-335
  66. 66. Draper A. Child development and iron deficiency. In: The Oxford Brief. Washington DC: USAID, Opportunities for Micronutrient Interventions, and Partnership for Child Development; 1997
  67. 67. Fuglestad AJ et al. Iron deficiency after arrival is associated with general cognitive and behavioral impairment in post-institutionalized children adopted from Eastern Europe. Maternal and Child Health Journal. 2013;17(6):1080-1087
  68. 68. Lozoff B et al. Behavior of infants with iron-deficiency anemia. Child Development. 1998;69(1):24-36
  69. 69. Shafir T et al. Effects of iron deficiency in infancy on patterns of motor development over time. Human Movement Science. 2006;25(6):821-838
  70. 70. Gordon N. Iron deficiency and the intellect. Brain & Development. 2003;25:3-8
  71. 71. Gropper SS, Smith JL, Groff JL. Advanced Nutrition and Human Metabolism. 5th ed. Belmont: Wadsworth, Cengage Learning; 2009. 600 p
  72. 72. Houston R. Iodine: Physiology, dietary sources and requirements. In: Caballero B, Allen LH, Prentice A, editors. Encyclopedia of Human Nutrition. 2nd ed. Vol. III. Oxford: Elsevier Inc.; 2005. pp. 66-73
  73. 73. Gordon R, Skeaff S, Gray A. Iodine supplementation improves cognition in mildly iodine-deficient children. Iodine and Cognition. 2009;90:3
  74. 74. Thomson C. Iodine. In: Mann J, Truswell S, editors. Essentials of Human Nutrition. 2nd ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2002. pp. 166-171
  75. 75. Aneja SUT et al. Impact of zinc supplementation on mental and psychomotor scores of children aged 12 to 18 months: A randomized, doubled-blind trial. The Journal of Pediatrics. 2005;146:506-511
  76. 76. Bhatnagar S, Taneja S. Zinc and cognitive development. British Journal of Nutrition. 2007;85(S2):S139
  77. 77. Black MM et al. Infants: Impact of zinc supplementation, birth weight. Pediatrics. 2004;113(5):1297-1305
  78. 78. Castillo-Durán C et al. Effect of zinc supplementation on development and growth of Chilean infants. The Journal of Pediatrics. 2001;138(2):229-235
  79. 79. Lonnerdal B. Zinc and health: Current status and future directions dietary factors influencing zinc absorption 1. The Journal of Nutrition. 2000;130:1378-1383
  80. 80. Mott DD, Dingledine R. Unraveling the role of zinc in memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2011;108(8):3103-3104
  81. 81. Bhan MK, Sommerfelt H, Strand T. Micronutrient deficiency in children. The British Journal of Nutrition. 2001;85(Suppl 2):S199-S203
  82. 82. Bourre JM. Effects of nutrients (in food) on the structure and function of the nervous system: Update on dietary requirements for brain. Part 1: Micronutrients. The Journal of Nutrition, Health & Aging. 2006;10(5):377-385
  83. 83. Faber M, Venter SL, Benadé AJS. Increased vitamin A intake in children aged 2-5 years through targeted home-gardens in a rural South African community. Public Health Nutrition. 2002;5(1):11-16
  84. 84. Semba RD. The role of vitamin A and related retinoids in immune function. Nutrition Reviews. 1998;56(1 Pt 2):S38-S48
  85. 85. Solomon NW. Competitive interaction of iron and zinc in the diet: Consequences for human nutrition. The Journal of Nutrition. 1986;116(6):927-935
  86. 86. West KP Jr. Dietary vitamin-A deficiency: Effects on growth, infection, and mortality. Food and Nutrition Bulletin. 1991;13(2):1-12
  87. 87. West KP. Vitamin A deficiency disorders in children and women. Food and Nutrition Bulletin. 2003;24(Suppl. 4):S78-S90
  88. 88. Harms LR et al. Vitamin D and the brain. Best Practice & Research. Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism. 2011;25(4):657-669
  89. 89. Norman AW, Henry HL. Vitamin D. In: Erdman J Jr, Macdonald I, Zeisel S, editors. Present Knowledge in Nutrition. 10th ed. Iowa: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.; 2012. pp. 199-213
  90. 90. Traber MG. Vitamin E. In: Caballero B, Allen L, Prentice A, editors. Encyclopedia of Human Nutrition. 2nd ed. Vol. IV. Oxford: Elsevier Inc.; 2005. pp. 283-397
  91. 91. Berdanier CD, Zempleni J. Advanced Nutrition: Macronutrients, Micronutrients, and Metabolism. Boca Ratón: CRC Press; 2009. 592 p
  92. 92. Fox MK et al. Sources of energy and nutrients in the diets of infants and toddlers. Journal of the American Dietetic Association. 2006;106(1 Suppl. 1):S28-S42
  93. 93. Lane DJR, Richardson DR. The active role of vitamin C in mammalian iron metabolism: Much more than just enhanced iron absorption! Free Radical Biology and Medicine. 2014;75:69-83
  94. 94. Maggini S, Wenzlaff S, Hornig D. Essential role of vitamin C and zinc in child immunity and health. Journal of International Medical Research. 2010;38(2):386-414
  95. 95. Singh M. Role of micronutrients for physical growth and mental development. Indian Journal of Pediatrics. 2004;71(1):59-62
  96. 96. Taras H. Nutrition and student performance at school. The Journal of School Health. 2005;75(6):199-213
  97. 97. Bellisle F. Effects of diet on behaviour and cognition in children. British Journal of Nutrition. 2004;92(suppl. 2):S227-S232
  98. 98. Bettendorf L. Thiamin. In: Erdman J Jr, Macdonald I, Zeisel S, editors. Present Knowledge in Nutrition. 10th ed. Iowa: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.; 2012. pp. 261-279
  99. 99. Thurnham DI: Thiamin. In: Caballero B, Allen L H, Prentice A. Encyclopedia of Human Nutrition. Vol. VI. 2nd ed Oxford: Elsevier Inc., 2005. pp. 263-277
  100. 100. Bender DA. Vitamin B6. In: Caballero L, Allen LH, Prentice A, editors. Encyclopedia of Human Nutrition. 2nd ed. Vol. IV. Oxford: Elsevier Inc.; 2005. pp. 359-367
  101. 101. Silva VR, Russell KA, Gregory JF. Vitamin B6. In: Erdeman J Jr, Macdonald I, Ziesel S, editors. Present Knowledge in Nutrition. 10th ed. Iowa: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.; 2012. pp. 307-320
  102. 102. Truswell S, Milne R. The B vitamins. In: Mann J, Truswell S, editors. Essentials of Human Nutrition. 2nd ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2002. pp. 209-230
  103. 103. Bender DA. Nutritional Biochemistry of the Vitamins. 2nd ed. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2003. 514 p
  104. 104. Bailey LB, Caudill MA. Folate. In: Erdeman J Jr, Macdonaldo I, Ziesel S, editors. Present Knowledge in Nutrition. 10th ed. Iowa: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.; 2012. pp. 321-342
  105. 105. Zeisel SH, Carbin KD. Choline. In: Erdeman J Jr, Macdonald I, Zeisel SH, editors. Present Knowledge in Nutrition. 10th ed. Iowa: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.; 2012. pp. 405-418
  106. 106. Grantham-McGregor S, Baker-Henningham H. Review of the evidence linking protein and energy to mental development. Public Health Nutrition. 2005;8(7A):1191-1201
  107. 107. Calder PC. Dietary fatty acids and the immune system. Lipids. 1999;34(Suppl. 1):S137-S140
  108. 108. Innis SM. Fatty acids and early human development. Early Human Development. 2007;83(12):761-766
  109. 109. Lassek WD, Gaulin SJC. Sex differences in the relationship of dietary fatty acids to cognitive measures in American children. Frontiers in Evolutionary Neuroscience. 2011;3(November):5
  110. 110. Uauy R et al. Term infant studies of DHA and ARA supplementation on neurodevelopment: Results of randomized controlled trials. The Journal of Pediatrics. 2003;143(Suppl. 4):S17-S25
  111. 111. Yaqoob P, Shaikh SR. The nutritional and clinical significance of lipid rafts. Current Opinion in Clinical Nutrition and Metabolic Care. 2010;13(2):156-166
  112. 112. Youdim KA, Martin A, Joseph JA. Essential fatty acids and the brain: Possible health implications. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience. 2000;18(4-5):383-399
  113. 113. Butte N et al. Energy requirements during pregnancy based on total energy expenditure and energy deposition. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2004;79:1078-1087
  114. 114. Rushton S, Juola-Rushton A, Larkin E. Neuroscience, play and early childhood education: Connections, implications and assessment. Early Childhood Education Journal. 2010;37(5):351-361
  115. 115. Nijhof SL et al. Healthy play, better coping: The importance of play for the development of children in health and disease. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2018;95:421-429
  116. 116. Ginsburg KR. The importance of play in promoting healthy child development and maintaining strong parent-child bond. Pediatrics. 2007;119(1):182-191
  117. 117. Knudsen E. Sensitive periods in humans. In: Squire L et al., editors. Fundamental Neuroscience. 3rd ed. Oxford: Academic Press; 2008. 521 p
  118. 118. Sheridan MD. From Birth to Five Years: Children’s Developmental Progress. 3rd ed. New York: Taylor & Francis; 2007. 112 p
  119. 119. Shonkoff JP, Phillips DA. From Neurons to Neighborhoods: The Science of Early Childhood Development. Washington, D.C.: National Academy Press; 2002. 612 p
  120. 120. Levine S. Estimulação na Infância. In: American S, editor. Psicobiologia: as bases biológicas do comportamento. São Paulo: Editora Perspectiva; 1970. pp. 103-109
  121. 121. Schor AN. Affect Regulation and the Origin of the Self: The Neurobiology of Emotional Development. New York: Routledge; 2016. 700 p
  122. 122. Passos APD. Integração de variáveis motoras, cognitivas, nutricionais, metabólicas e de influência epigenética relativas à Primeira Infância como uma ferramenta para investigação do Desenvolvimento Infantil [thesis]. Sao Paulo: Bioscience Institute: University of Sao Paulo; 2019
  123. 123. Payne VG, Isaacs LD. Human Motor Development: A Lifespan Approach. 8th ed. New York: McGraw-Hill Inc.; 2012. 608 p

Written By

Ana Paula Dantas Passos

Submitted: 17 August 2023 Reviewed: 07 September 2023 Published: 03 November 2023