Open access peer-reviewed chapter

Students with Mathematics Learning Disabilities and Their Ways of Thinking in Fraction Learning

Written By

Suprih Widodo and Trisno Ikhwanudin

Submitted: 29 May 2019 Reviewed: 22 August 2019 Published: 10 October 2019

DOI: 10.5772/intechopen.89307

From the Edited Volume

Learning Disabilities - Neurological Bases, Clinical Features and Strategies of Intervention

Edited by Sandro Misciagna

Chapter metrics overview

643 Chapter Downloads

View Full Metrics

Abstract

This chapter presents the result of research on ways of thinking of students with mathematics learning disabilities in fraction learning. We conducted a class of fraction learning with Lesh translation model. From the class discussion, interview, and students’ work, we then explore the students’ ways of thinking when they learn fraction. In the class, students with mathematics learning disabilities perform two mental acts with corresponding ways of thinking and ways of understanding; those are interpreting and problem-solving. We find some interesting findings and they are: (1) students know the common denominator method in the addition of fractions; however, they incorrectly apply the method; (2) students use the common denominator approach (for fraction addition) in the multiplication of fraction; and (3) in the division of fraction, students mistakenly apply the invert multiply algorithm.

Keywords

  • students with mathematics learning disabilities
  • fraction learning
  • ways of thinking

1. Introduction

We may have heard the case of a student having difficulty in mathematics, but the student does not experience obstacles in other subjects in school. After further observation, it turns out that the IQ of the student was at an average level even above average. For cases like this, the student can be suspected of having dyscalculic symptoms or mathematics learning disabilities (MLD). Based on the results of the study, the number of people with MLD according to Strauss is 5–8% of school-age children [1], while according to Adler, the number of people with dyscalculia is 5–6% of all children [2].

Research on dyscalculia is still ongoing. Researchers, especially in the United Kingdom and the United States continue to conduct studies to study dyscalculia in greater depth. Therefore, the understanding and understanding of dyscalculia will continue to develop. The following are some of the dyscalculia definitions issued by both formal institutions and individual researchers. Definition of dyscalculia issued by the National Center for Learning Disabilities is as follows: dyscalculia is a term related to learning difficulties in mathematics. Although learning barriers differ from person to person, the general characteristics are as follows: difficulty in numerating, learning numbers, and doing mathematical calculations; difficulty in measurement, showing time, counting money, and estimating the number; problematic in mathematical intelligence and problem-solving strategies [3].

In general, dyscalculia is an umbrella term used for various difficulties in learning mathematics, such as developmental dyscalculia, mathematical difficulties, difficulty learning numerical concepts, and difficulties about learning number concepts.

There are many studies that discuss MLD students, with a different research focus: first, the research that focuses on the identification or criteria of MLD students; second, the research that focuses on how MLD students think in learning mathematics; and third, the research that focuses on finding solutions to learning mathematics in MLD students. The detailed of the research focus is as follows:

1.1 Research that focuses on the identification or criteria of MLD students

The study of the identification and criteria of MLD students has been carried out by several researchers, including the following: Geary described dyscalculia as a numerical and arithmetic difficulty caused by brain injury; he uses this term to describe a population of 5–8% of school-age children who have a cognitive disorder that affects their ability to learn concepts or procedures in one or more areas of mathematics [4].

Next the opinions of several experts about the criteria of MLD students will be described:

  • students with an average IQ whose standardized test scores are below the 20th or 25th percentile [4];

  • slower and often make mistakes in processing the representation of numbers, for example, the symbol number “3” and the equivalent of the non-symbol “◆◆◆” [5];

  • make mistakes in comparing and estimating numbers [6];

  • wrong in doing arithmetic calculations [7]; and

  • wrong in solving numbers problems that are very easy, for example, 4 × 5 = 20 [8].

The researchers identified students with MLD using standardized test results, for example, the Woodcock-Johnson Test of Achievement and the Wide Range Achievement Test, by looking at students who were below the 20th or 25th percentile [9]. Lewis further tightens the criteria for identifying MLD students, which combines the following three criteria:

  • students score below 25th percentile on standardized mathematics tests;

  • the results of observations and interviews revealed that there was no influence of environmental or social factors on students inability in mathematics; and

  • after being given treatment, the effect of the treatment on increasing mathematical ability is very less. To find this out, Lewis made a comparison with a control class whose members were not MLD students [10].

In identifying students with MLD, Lewis [9] suggests that if researchers use self-developed identification instruments, it is also necessary to include the results of standardized measuring instruments as a comparison. The next suggestion is to apply a cutoff under the 10th percentile; observing longitudinal data showing that learning difficulties in mathematics are long-standing, and researchers must distinguish the difficulty of learning mathematics is the result of cognitive or non-cognitive factors. To do this it is recommended to conduct a demographic analysis of the respondents, for example, socioeconomic status, ethnicity, and mother tongue. This can also be done with qualitative methods, such as interviews, questionnaires, observation of students, parents, and teachers, to find out the factors that lead to the low mathematical achievement of students.

1.2 Research that focuses on MLD students’ way of thinking in learning mathematics

The study of how MLD students think in learning mathematics has been carried out by several researchers, including the following:

Lewis states that students with MLD have a different mindset in understanding fractions, she looked at students with MLD does not mean they have deficiencies in understanding the concept of fractions, but there are differences in the way of thinking in understanding fractions [11]. Then Lewis states that students with MLD experience obstacles in learning fractions, especially on the topic of fraction comparison, both fraction comparisons with the same denominator, as well as in fractions comparisons involving fractions of half; in this study Lewis suggested examining students’ understanding of the quantity of fractions [12].

Hunt et al. [13] state that MLD students have obstacles in mastering the concept of fractions by learning part-whole models. Newton et al. [14] state that the main error pattern in understanding fractions in MLD students is the use of traditional algorithms that are wrong.

1.3 Research that focuses on finding solutions for MLD students in learning mathematics

The study of alternative mathematical learning solutions for MLD students has been carried out by several researchers, including the following:

Shin and Bryant state that good fraction teaching by MLD students must involve the following 5 aspects: real objects and visual representations such as pictures and number lines, explicit and systematic learning, various time frames and sets of examples, heuristic strategies, and use real problem [15].

Mazzocco et al. state that visual models can be used as alternatives when helping MLD students understand fractions [16]. Gersten et al. [17] state that in assisting MLD students, practitioners are expected to take the following steps: (a) teach students with diverse teaching examples; (b) directing students to say the thoughts and solutions of a problem; (c) teach students to visualize math problems that they face; (d) teach students with diverse/heuristic strategies; (e) the teacher prepares a partner/discussion partner for MLD students; (f) teach MLD students with explicit instructions; (g) the teacher prepares the correct variety and sequence of examples;

Shin and Bryant [15] state that the use of a computer program, Fun Fraction, can help MLD students solve problem-solving in the form of stories. Virtual manipulation in Fun Fraction helps problem-solving skills because students are assisted by this program in representing the problem stories they are dealing with.

Finally, Tian, Jing, and Siegler, state that the use of an optimal number line model can help MLD students understand fraction size and calculation [18].

In this chapter, we focus on students’ ways of thinking in fractions learning. It is needed as an essential first step toward effective instructional methods. We use the theory of mental act, ways of thinking, and ways of understanding from Harel. Furthermore, we also analyze the error pattern of MLD students when they learn fractions. The results of this study are expected to add to the discourse of educational scholarship, especially on the teaching and learning mathematics in an inclusive setting for students with MLD.

Advertisement

2. Fraction learning

Fractional topics include material in mathematics that is difficult to explain. This is because fraction is one of the topics in mathematics that requires high-level and complex thinking. Definition of fractions according to Clarke et al. [19]:

“Fractions are symbolic-shaped expressions that represent the quotient of two numbers ab(where b is not equal to zero). So all rational numbers expressed in terms ab are fractions, but rational numbers 1.45 are not fractions. Rasonals 1.45 can be called a fraction if written 145100 . So that all rational numbers can be written as fractions, but there are some important fractions that are not rational numbers, for example: ab or ab” (p. 15).

In many classes, fractions are taught only in a procedural way. The teacher usually teaches fractions by applying the method of equalizing the denominator, by calculating the Least Common Multiples (LCM). On the other hand, according to Hiebert and Wearne [20], with this procedural method, students will only gain procedural understanding or syntax thinking. Students will not understand the relationship between fractions, in other words, students’ conceptual understanding (semantic thinking) will be weak.

How can students gain a conceptual understanding of fractional material? Riccomini suggests two teaching strategies for better fraction learning; the two strategies are learning fractions by using number lines and the use of diverse representations [21]. The use of number lines and paper folding as representations is also suggested by Wyberg et al. [22].

Several other research results also support the use of diverse representations. Dey and Dey suggest the use of geometry representations; addition, subtraction, multiplication, and division operations can be represented geometrically [23]. Furthermore, Clark and Roche suggest the use of games in fraction learning; the game is done like a monopoly game using a kind of broken board, dice, and involves all students in the class [24].

The use of image representation is suggested by de Castro [25]. The same representation, using colored art drawings was suggested by Scaptura et al. [26]. Fractional learning using technology was suggested by Mendiburo and Hasselbring; they also prove that teaching fractions with technology are as effective as teaching fractions that use physical manipulation [27].

Other researchers, Lesh, Posh, and Behr stated that students gain a better understanding when they can identify and model mathematical concepts through various representations [28]. Furthermore, the Principle and Standards for School Mathematics suggest that students represent their mathematical ideas so that mathematical ideas make sense according to students [29]. One learning model that offers the use of diverse representations is the Lesh Translation Model.

Advertisement

3. Lesh translation model

Lesh Translational Model states that basic mathematical ideas can be represented in 5 ways: real (manipulative) objects, images, real-world contexts, verbal symbols, and written symbols. This model is illustrated by the followingFigure 1:

Figure 1.

Lesh translation model [30].

Lesh Translational Model emphasizes interactions within and between representations. The arrows between one representation and another represent the intermodal translation, while the arrows in one mode represent the translation in the mode itself. This model suggests that a good understanding of mathematical ideas requires experience from various modes (ways) and the experience of making connections between and within these modes of representation. A translation requires interpretation of ideas that differ from one mode to another. This activity with its intellectual relations activity reflects dynamic learning.

Advertisement

4. Mental act, ways of thinking, and ways of understanding

According to Harel [31], human reasoning involves many mental actions such as interpreting, guessing, concluding, proving, explaining, compiling, generalizing, applying, predicting, classifying, searching and solving problems. He states that way of understanding is a certain cognitive product of mental actions carried out by an individual. For example, after seeing the symbol 34, one can interpret (one mental action) to produce meaning for the symbol 34. The resulting interpretation is one’s Ways of Understanding of the symbol 34. This way of understanding can be different depending on the context, and if judged by an observer, can be considered right or wrong. For example, in a context one can interpret the symbol 34 as “3 objects out of 4 objects,” and another person can interpret as “repeated sums: 14+14+14.” Others might be able to produce sophisticated Ways of Understanding such as equivalent classes (3n4n  where n is a non-zero integer) and naive Ways of Understanding, such as “two numbers with a bar between them.”

Ways of Thinking is a cognitive characteristic of the Mental Act. The cognitive characteristics of the Mental Act are inferred from observations of Ways of Understanding (cognitive products of mental actions). For example, a teacher who follows students’ mathematical behavior might conclude that students’ interpretations of mathematical symbols are inflexible, there are absolutely no quantitative views, or for example, students’ interpretations of symbols are flexible and connected with other concepts. Another example, the teacher can conclude that students’ proof of mathematical statements is based on empirical evidence, or based on deductive reasoning [31].

Advertisement

5. Mental act, ways of thinking, and ways of understanding of MLD student

Here are the results of the data analysis from three students with MLD; we found mental acts, ways of understanding, and ways of thinking as follows:

5.1 Problem solving

Here is one example of student work that used mental act problem-solving (Figure 2).

Figure 2.

Mental act problem-solving of MLD student.

In Figure 2, the student solves a problem: a tailor receives 23 m of white cloth with floral motifs to make a handkerchief. Each handkerchief requires 16 m of fabric. How many handkerchiefs can be made?. To solve this problem, the student wrote: 23:16=23×61=4 handkerchiefs. To answer this word problem, the student performs mental act problem-solving by modeling mathematical word problem into fraction division operation. Then he solves the problem of dividing the fraction using the invert multiple algorithm method [32].

A problem-solving approach is a cognitive characteristic of mental act problem-solving. From the results of the analysis of the answers, it was found that 8 students did the problem-solving approach. In the answers above, it appears that students understand the questions and answer them using a problem-solving approach, in the form of an invert multiple algorithm (IMA) strategy in fraction division operations.

The solution is a cognitive product of mental act problem-solving. From the results of the analysis of answers, obtained student answers are examples of the way of understanding solution.

5.2 Interpreting

The second identifiable mental act of MLD students is interpreting. The example of student work is as follows (Figure 3).

Figure 3.

Mental act interpreting of MLD student.

In Figure 3, the student is asked to describe fractions 12 and 25 in two different ways. Students have been able to interpret 12 with two different interpretation, which is the rectangle and triangle picture. In the rectangle picture which is divided into two parts; one part is shaded and the other part is not shaded. In the triangle picture which is divided into two parts; one part is shaded and the other part is not shaded. There is something interesting in the triangle picture, students divide the triangle in the centerline, with a horizontal triangle position. Next, students interpret 25 with pictures of parallelograms and squares, each of which is divided into five parts; two parts are shaded and the other is not shaded.

Diverse interpretation of mathematical symbols is a cognitive characteristic of mental act interpreting (way of thinking). From the analysis of MLD student test result data, it was found that he made a fractional interpretation in the form of images, namely rectangular and circular images, as shown above. Interpretation is a cognitive product of mental act interpreting. From the results of the analysis of MLD student answers, it is an embodiment of the way of understanding interpreting, namely interpretation. The students’ interpretation of the fractions 12 and 25 is a picture of a rectangle, triangle, parallelogram, and square, as shown above.

We summarize these findings in Table 1.

Mental actWay of understandingWay of thinking
Problem-solvingSolutionProblem-solving approach: invert multiply algorithm
InterpretingInterpretationMultiple interpretations (as pictures of the square, rectangle, etc.)

Table 1.

The mental act, way of understanding, and way of thinking.

Advertisement

6. The error pattern of MLD students in fractions learning

Some patterns of errors made by MLD students are as follows:

6.1 Students know the common denominator method in the addition of fractions; however, they incorrectly apply the method

The pattern of mistakes of the three students is wrong in applying the denominator equalization procedure. Here is a picture showing this (Figure 4).

Figure 4.

Example of an error pattern in applying the denominator equalization procedure to the fraction addition operation.

In the questions, participants are asked to solve two fraction addition questions. In the first problem (part a), students are asked to solve questions 13+13=. This question aims to reveal students’ understanding of the fraction addition operation with the same denominator. For this problem, students give the correct answer: 13+13=23 .

In the second problem (part b), students are asked to solve questions 13+12=. This problem aims to reveal students’ understanding of the sum of fractions with different denominators. In this problem, students give answers: 13+12=16+16=26 . Learners already know the procedure to do the denominator in the addition operation of fractions. So when he sees the question 13+12=, he performs the denominator equalization procedure by changing 3 to 6 in the first term and changing 2 to 6 in the second term. However, students do not make numerator changes. So, participants already know the denominator equalization procedure, but do not make adjustments to the numerator. In other words, students mistakenly understand the denominator equalization procedure in fraction addition operations.

6.2 Students use the common denominator approach (for fraction addition) in the multiplication of fraction

The second error pattern is very interesting, namely, students apply the denominator equalization procedure in multiplication operations. Here is a picture showing this (Figure 5).

Figure 5.

Example of error pattern applying the denominator equalization procedure to multiplication operations.

In the problem, students are asked to solve questions 45×13=.This problem aims to reveal students’ understanding of fraction multiplication. In this problem, students give answers: 45×13=1215×515=6015÷5=43=113. There is an interesting thing, students apply the denominator equalization procedure (supposed to be the sum operation) on the fraction multiplication operation. So when he saw the problem 45×13=, he did the procedure of equating the denominator in the first syllable by changing 5 to 15 and in the second syllable changing 3 to 15. There were other interesting things done by students. He only did the multiplication, namely: 1215×515=6015. He then divides 6015 by 5 to produce 43 fractions. The interesting thing is that students apply the denominator equalization procedure in fraction multiplication operations.

6.3 In the division of fraction, students mistakenly apply the invert multiply algorithm

The third error pattern is very interesting, namely, students turn the first syllable in a fraction division operation. Here is a picture showing this (Figure 6):

Figure 6.

Example of the first syllable error pattern in a fraction division operation.

In the second problem (part b), students are asked to solve questions 94÷35=. This question aims to reveal students’ understanding of fraction distribution operations. In this problem, students seem to already know the procedure of division operations on fractions. But there is an interesting thing, students use the method of multiplying with the inverse (invert multiply algorithm), but what is reversed is not the second term, but the first term. Consider the following illustration of student answers: 94÷35=49×35=415. So that the answers obtained are reversed, the answer should be 154, students get 415.

Advertisement

7. Discussion

MLD students solve fractions problem procedurally, they apply common denominator approach, drawing a picture, direct multiplied strategy, and invert multiply algorithm in solving fractions problems. They cannot practice the other strategies like using a benchmark or residual which demands the ability to infer and explain. Therefore, we conclude MLD students only perform two mental acts, which are problem-solving and interpreting. They could not develop other mental acts like explaining or inferring.

Some interesting findings when MLD students solve fractions problem are: (1) they know the procedure of common denominator approach in fraction addition operation, however, they mistakenly apply the procedure; (2) in multiplication and divisions operation, they are familiar with the procedure, however, they mistakenly apply the procedure. The two finding is in line with Newton et al. research, they revealed that the main pattern of error in fraction understanding on MLD students is the use of traditional false algorithms [14]. These findings also in accordance with the research of Mazzocco et al., which show that the difficulties in fraction learning are still felt by MLD students until they are in grade 8 [16]. Other researchers also had the same research result, which stated that MLD students make a mistake in performing arithmetic calculations [7].

Another previous research explained that students with MLD have a different ways of thinking in understanding fractions. Lewis considered that the MLD students did not mean to have a lack of understanding of fractions; however, they had different ways of thinking in understanding fractions [11]. We find that MLD students have different ways of thinking in understanding fractions addition operation; they differently understand the common denominator approach, they do not multiply the numerator by the same number with the denominator.

The other research findings deduced that adolescent MLD students are experiencing difficulties in fraction comparison subjects, either fractions comparisons with the same denominator or in fractions comparisons involving a half fraction [12]. Lewis suggested to investigating younger MLD students as the subject. We involved younger students with MLD in our research, a similar result is found, that is MLD students have difficulties in solving fractions comparison problems [33].

In our finding, partitioning activities, which are beneficial for regular students, but not necessarily helpful to MLD students; this may happen because MLD students do not follow a developmental pattern like their regular peers. In accordance with our findings, Lewis explained that partitioning activity was probably the root of understanding the quantity of fractions in regular students; MLD students may not follow this pattern of development [10].

According to Brousseau, the appearance of learning obstacle in mathematics can be caused by three obstacles, namely ontogenic obstacle (mental learning readiness), didactical obstacle (obstacle from teacher instruction or teaching material), and epistemological obstacle (students’ knowledge which has limited application context) [34]. In the context of Brousseau theory, the three error patterns of the MLD students in fractions learning is prone to the type of epistemological obstacle, that is MLD students already know fractions concept, however, they have limited application context to the other fractions problems [35].

Advertisement

8. Conclusion

We found only two mental acts with corresponding WoU and WoT, namely problem-solving and interpreting. On the analysis of MLD students, it was found an interesting thing in the mental act problem solving, i.e., the student knew the common denominator approach in the operation of fraction addition, but the practice is still wrong. The same thing is also found in multiplication and division operation. Surprisingly, students use the common denominator approach in the fraction multiplication. In the division of fraction, students mistakenly apply the invert multiply algorithm.

The results of this study can be used by the teachers as a guideline when teaching fractions to students. Future research is recommended to analyze the error patterns of MLD students with other topics in mathematics, such as geometry.

References

  1. 1. Strauss V. Trying to figure out why math is so hard for some, theories abound: Genetics, gender, how It’s taught. The Washington Post. 2003;2003
  2. 2. Adler B. What Is Dyscalculia. Cognitive Center in Sweden: Sweden; 2001
  3. 3. National Center for Learning Disabilities. The State of Learning Disabilities. New York: NCLD; 2014
  4. 4. Geary DC. Mathematics and learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2004;37(1):4-15
  5. 5. Piazza M, Facoetti A, Trussardi AN, Berteletti I, Conte S, Lucangeli D, et al. Developmental trajectory of number acuity reveals a severe impairment in developmental dyscalculia. Cognition. 2010;116:33-41
  6. 6. Mazzocco MMM, Feigenson L, Halberda J. Impaired acuity of the approximate number system underlies mathematical learning disability (dyscalculia). Child Development. 2011;82:1224-1237
  7. 7. Geary DC, Hoard MK, Byrd-Craven J, DeSota MC. Strategy choices in simple and complex addition: Contributions of working memory and counting knowledge for children with mathematical disability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2004;88:121-151
  8. 8. Mazzocco MMM, Devlin KT, McKenney SJ. Is it a fact? Timed arithmetic performance of children with mathematical learning disabilities (MLD) varies as a function of how MLD is defined. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2008;33:318-344
  9. 9. Lewis KE, Fisher MB. Taking stock of 40 years of research on mathematical learning disability: Methodological issues and future directions. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education. 2016;47(4):338-371
  10. 10. Lewis KE. Beyond error patterns: A sociocultural view of fraction comparison errors in students with mathematical learning disabilities. Learning Disability Quarterly. 2016b;1(14):1-14
  11. 11. Lewis KE. Difference not deficit: Reconceptualising mathematics learning disabilities. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education. 2014;45(3)
  12. 12. Lewis KE. Understanding mathematical learning disabilities as developmental difference: A fine-grained analysis of one student’s partitioning strategies for fractions. Journal of Education and Development. 2016;39(4):812-857
  13. 13. Hunt JH et al. Initial understandings of fraction concepts evidenced by students with mathematical learning disabilities and difficulties: A framework. Learning Disability Quarterly. 2016;13(1):1-13
  14. 14. Newton et al. An examination of the ways that students with learning disabilities solve fraction computation problems. The Elementary School Journal. 2014;115(1):1-21
  15. 15. Shin M, Bryant DP. Improving the fraction word problem solving of students with mathematics learning disabilities: Interactive computer application. Remedial and Special Education. 2016;38(2):76-86
  16. 16. Mazzocco M et al. Limited knowledge of fraction representations differentiates middle school students with mathematics learning disability (dyscalculia) versus low mathematics achievement. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2013;115(2):371-387
  17. 17. Gersten R, Chard D, Jayanthi M, Baker S, Morphy P, Flojo J. Mathematics Instruction for Students with Learning Disabilities or Difficulty Learning Mathematics: Asynthesis of the Intervention Research. Portsmouth, NH: RMC Research Corporation, Center on Instruction; 2008
  18. 18. Tian J, Siegler RS. Fractions learning in children with mathematics difficulties. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2016;50(6):614-620
  19. 19. Clarke C, Fisher W, Marks R, Ross S, Zbiek RS. Developing Essential Understanding of Rational Numbers for Teaching Mathematics in Grades 3-5. Reston, VA: NCTM; 2010
  20. 20. Hiebert J, Wearne D. Procedures over concepts: The acquisition of decimal number knowledge. In: Hiebert DJ, editor. Conceptual and Procedural Knowledge: The Case of Mathematics. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1986. pp. 199-223
  21. 21. Riccomini PJ. Core Issues of Math: Number Sense and Fraction. Kansas MTSS Symposium. 2010. Available from: www.kansasmtss.org/2010Symposium/Riccomini_Number_Sense_and_Fractions
  22. 22. Wyberg T, Whitney SR, Cramer KA, Monson DS, Leavitt S. Unfolding fraction multiplication: Helps students understand an important algorithm by using a piece of paper and a number line. Mathematics Teaching in The Middle School. 2012;17(5):289-293
  23. 23. Dey K, Dey R. Teaching arithmetic of fractions using geometry. Journal of Mathematics Education. 2010;3(2):170-182
  24. 24. Clarke D, Roche A. The power of a single game to address a range of important ideas in fraction learning. In: The Australasian Mathematical Psychology Conference 2010, Australia. p. 2010
  25. 25. de Castro BV. Cognitive models: The missing link to learning fraction multiplication and division. Asia Pacific Education Review. 2008;9(2):101-112
  26. 26. Scaptura C, Suh J, Mahaffey G. Masterpieces to mathematics: Using art to teaching fraction, decimal, and percent equivalents. Mathematics Teaching in The Middle School. 2007;13(1):24-28
  27. 27. Mendiburo M., Hasselbring T. Technology’s Impact on Fraction Learning: An Experimental Comparison of Virtual and Physical Manipulative. SREE Conference Abstract Template; 2011
  28. 28. Lesh R, Post T, Behr M. Representations and translations among representations in mathematics learning and problem solving. Problem of representation in the teaching and learning of mathematics. In: Javier C, editor. Hillsdale. NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1987. pp. 33-40
  29. 29. National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. Principles and Standards for School Mathematics. Reston, VA: NCTM; 2000
  30. 30. Cramer K. Using a translation model for curriculum development and classroom instruction. In: Lesh R, Doerr H, editors. Beyond Constructivism: Models and Modeling Perspectives on Mathematics Problem Solving, Learning, and Teaching. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2003. pp. 449-464
  31. 31. Harel G. What is mathematics? A pedagogical answer to a philosophical question. In: Gold B, Simons RA, editors. Proof and Other Dilemmas: Mathematics and Philosophy. Washington, DC: MAA; 2008. pp. 265-290
  32. 32. Zembat IO. An alternative route to teaching fraction division: Abstraction of common denominator algorithm. International Electronic Journal of Elementary Education. 2015;7(3):399-422
  33. 33. Ikhwanudin T, Suryadi D. How students with mathematics learning disabilities understands fraction: A case from the Indonesian inclusive school. International Journal of Instruction. 2018;11(3):309-326. DOI: 10.12973/iji.2018.11322a
  34. 34. Brousseau G. Theory of Didactical Situations in Mathematics. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers; 2002
  35. 35. Ikhwanudin T, Prabawanto S, Wahyudin. The error pattern of students with mathematics learning disabilities in the inclusive school on fractions learning. International Journal of Learning, Teaching and Educational Research. 2019;18(3):75-95. DOI: 10.26803/ijlter.18.3.5

Written By

Suprih Widodo and Trisno Ikhwanudin

Submitted: 29 May 2019 Reviewed: 22 August 2019 Published: 10 October 2019