Open access peer-reviewed chapter - ONLINE FIRST

Emotion Regulation and Cognitive and Social Functioning in Early Development: The Interface between Neurophysiological and Behavioural Perspectives

Written By

Sara Cruz, Mariana Sousa and Vera Mateus

Submitted: 18 September 2023 Reviewed: 12 January 2024 Published: 01 February 2024

DOI: 10.5772/intechopen.1004233

Emotional Intelligence - Understanding and Influencing Emotions IntechOpen
Emotional Intelligence - Understanding and Influencing Emotions Edited by Éric Laurent

From the Edited Volume

Emotional Intelligence - Understanding and Influencing Emotions [Working Title]

Dr. Éric Laurent

Chapter metrics overview

78 Chapter Downloads

View Full Metrics

Abstract

Research on the development of emotions and their function as indicators of psychological adjustment has increased dramatically in recent years. The importance of emotion recognition and regulation for understanding normal development and the development of psychopathology is now undeniable. From an early age, the ability to recognise and modulate emotional responses to cope appropriately with environmental demands is associated with cognitive, behavioural and social development. This chapter aims to critically review the research field of emotion regulation, its relationship to emotional intelligence and its contribution to cognitive and social functioning in young children (up to the age of six), placing a dual emphasis on the role of physiological systems in emotion regulation and on the interface between neurophysiological and behavioural perspectives. Challenges and future directions in emotion regulation research will be discussed.

Keywords

  • emotional intelligence
  • emotion regulation
  • early development
  • childhood
  • neurophysiological correlates

1. Introduction

Over the past decades, there has been a surge of research interest in early childhood emotions. The first years of life appear to be the most optimal period for emotional development because of the brain processes that occur during this time. The brain’s structure, functions and connections are being established in response to environmental stimulation, so emotional experiences that happen early in life will have a profound impact on the development and ability to recognise and manage emotions appropriately [1].

Emotion regulation (ER) is a key element in children’s developmental outcomes, influencing cognition, behaviour and social interactions, and is closely related to emotional intelligence (EI). During childhood, appropriate ER strategies enable children to successfully communicate and cooperate with peers, adults and the environment [2]. These strategies are associated with cognitive functioning, behaviour and empathy processes [3, 4]. In view of the significant importance of emotions, alterations in emotional processing have been proposed as a transdiagnostic mechanism in child psychopathology (see [5] for a review).

The purpose of this chapter is to provide an overview of ER in early childhood (up to the age of six). In what follows, we will define ER and its relationship to EI, as well as explore ER strategies used by young children, discussing their adaptive value and their association with cognitive and behavioural outcomes. We will also examine the role of ER in childhood psychopathology. We will then present a narrative review of the evidence for the neurophysiological mechanisms underlying ER and its relationship to cognition and behaviour. For this purpose, the following terms were used in the literature search: (infant OR infancy OR child*) AND (“emotion* regulation”) AND (mri OR fmri OR fnirs OR nirs OR eeg OR erp OR vagal OR rsa). In addition, a manual search of the reference lists of the selected studies was conducted to identify additional studies that may have been of interest for this review. Finally, we will discuss future challenges and directions for ER research.

Advertisement

2. Emotional intelligence and emotion regulation

Emotional development begins early in life, as infants are already born with the ability to process and express different emotions (e.g. crying) [6]. In the first six years, emotional development is a fundamental aspect that influences cognition, behaviour and social functioning, as it allows children to adapt to different environments (e.g. school) and successfully establish interpersonal relationships throughout their life [7]. Early childhood is an extremely important period for children to learn and manage emotions, with research demonstrating that this period represents a critical window for the development of emotional competencies and self-regulation, with long-term consequences for academic, personal and social outcomes [8].

EI and ER are two research traditions that address emotion management across development. Although relatively independent, their relationship is well-established in the literature. High EI individuals manage their emotions at the earliest point in the emotion trajectory, have a larger repertoire of regulation strategies and are more flexible and successful in regulating their emotions [9]. Understanding emotions will allow their recognition and, consequently, the employment of adequate strategies to deal with different emotional states [10]. Research in early development also corroborates these findings and has shown that children with higher EI are better able to pay attention, are more engaged in school, have more positive relationships and are more empathic [11, 12]. In parallel, children who score higher in EI are also better at regulating aggressive behaviour [13].

EI refers to a set of skills (e.g. perception or regulation of emotions) that allow the recognition and understanding of emotions in oneself and others [14, 15]. These skills are consistent with the term ‘emotional competence’ (EC) employed in the developmental psychology literature [16]. Young children’s EC entails (i) the expression of emotions, (ii) knowledge of emotions in oneself and others and (iii) the ability to regulate their own and others’ emotional expressiveness and experience [17]. The ability to effectively regulate one’s emotional expressions (i.e. emotion regulation) is of great importance, especially in the preschool years, as it plays an important role in development due to its long-term impact on future adjustment [18].

2.1 Emotion regulation in early childhood

ER refers to the child’s ability to modulate emotional responses consciously or unconsciously [19]. It encompasses the intrinsic and extrinsic processes of monitoring, assessing and modifying emotional reactions, particularly concerning their timing and intensity, to achieve personal goals [20, 21]. By modifying the magnitude and/or type of their emotional experience or the emotion-evoking event [22], children can influence their emotions, depending on their valence (positive or negative), along with the situations and time when they are expressed [23]. This ability enables them to effectively navigate and adapt to different environments [24, 25]. According to Gross [22], ER strategies are organised around four temporal stages: (i) an internal or external event triggers an emotional response, (ii) attention is allocated to that event, (iii) an assessment of that event is made and (iv) a response is produced following the emotional experience of that event.

ER abilities grow rapidly in the early years and continue to grow throughout development [26]. In children aged between 1 and 3 years, ER abilities rely heavily on external social support, particularly from their parents and caregivers [27, 28, 29, 30, 31]. Alongside, various processes, such as language or executive functions (e.g. cognitive control), influence the development of ER, enabling children to recognise and label their emotional states, communicate their needs, seek support from others and appropriately respond to environmental demands [21, 27, 32, 33, 34, 35]. The increased awareness of others, supported by the development of the Theory of Mind abilities [36], adds a social dimension to emotions [37].

By the age of 3, emotion regulation significantly improves, as children can recognise basic emotions in others and adjust their behaviours in social interactions, as observed in an experimental task in which children were asked to assign an emotion to a cartoon character [38]. Behavioural control emerges along with the peak of aggressive behaviours, and young children become able to use effective ER strategies, such as delaying gratification, maintaining focus, using a lower voice and/or reducing motor behaviour, assessed in both social role-play tasks and parent report measures. [33]. Additional evidence, using story completion tasks to elicit emotion-related narratives, suggests that children begin to use antecedent-focused ER strategies (i.e. regulatory strategies that occur before or during emotion processing and are effective in modulating emotional responses, see [39]), such as cognitive reappraisal (e.g. the process of creating positive interpretations or perspectives of a stressful situation to reduce distress, see [40]), at age 3, and become more effective in using this strategy throughout childhood (around age 5) [41]. Between the ages of 4 and 6, children begin to grasp their internal experiences (e.g. memories, fears and wishes) and distinguish real emotional experiences from virtual ones [30, 34, 38, 42]. At the age of 6, they become increasingly able to cope with negative emotions and use distraction strategies (e.g. playing, drawing or singing) to manage them [37]. They also begin to control some maladaptive emotion regulation strategies, such as crying, wailing and shouting [42].

In terms of maladaptive strategies, two strategies have been consistently linked to adjustment problems: suppression and rumination [43, 44, 45]. Suppression involves the inhibition of both emotional expression and thoughts. According to Gross’s model [44, 46, 47], this is a response-focused strategy that restrains emotional expression and temporarily reduces the subjective experience of emotion, but it is ineffective in regulating emotions and physiological arousal over time. In addition, the suppression of unwanted thoughts may paradoxically lead to increased access to these suppressed thoughts [47], resulting in heightened emotional arousal, particularly in the physiological domain, which is considered maladaptive [48]. Rumination is the repetitive focus on the emotional experience, its causes and effects [48, 49]. This strategy is used to further understand and solve problems [50]. However, it can inhibit positive problem-solving [51] and lead to indecision and difficulty in taking action [52].

The benefits of using adaptive ER strategies at an early age are well documented. For example, a systematic review and meta-analysis examined 150 empirical studies on ER abilities (using teacher-, parent-report and task-based assessments) in early childhood and their association with various outcomes [18]. The authors reported that these abilities, assessed around the age of 4, predicted 25 developmental outcomes. Particularly, ER was positively associated with social competence, school engagement and academic performance and negatively associated with internalising problems (e.g. depressive symptoms), peer victimisation and externalising problems (e.g. aggressive behaviour) in early school years (around age 8), later school years (around age 13) and adulthood (around age 38). Other empirical research has shown that ER in pre-schoolers is positively related to social and school engagement (i.e. appropriate interactions with adults, peers and/or materials) and prosociality and negatively related to hyperactivity [53].

The consequences of using suppression or rumination strategies in early childhood are less studied, probably due to the linguistic and cognitive limitations of this age. However, some research has addressed this matter. For example, a study employing a puppet procedure investigated 3- and 4-year-old children’s views about ‘effective’ (e.g. cognitive distraction) and ‘ineffective’ (e.g. rumination and venting) ways of controlling anger, sadness and fear [42]. Compared to adults, children reported that “ineffective” strategies, such as rumination, resulted in greater reductions in negative emotions. The authors suggest that this may be because they have less understanding of or experience with emotion regulation, or because they ‘genuinely consider a strategy like rumination to be effective because they know that emotions sometimes improve with the passage of time’ (p. 11). Other evidence supports the negative impact of maladaptive ER strategies on behaviour and cognition. A study of 193 pre-schoolers aged around 55 months, using teach-report assessments, showed that those who used more maladaptive ER strategies (e.g. anger and frustration) were more likely to engage in aggressive behaviours [54]. Another study involving 119 children aged around 69 months, and using a task-based assessment (i.e. children’s directed attention and concentration to a movie clip), found that the use of suppression hinders their self-control resources [55].

In summary, evidence suggests that young children still develop their emotional awareness and regulation abilities. They often rely on strategies such as seeking comfort from caregivers. These early attempts at emotion regulation can be seen as advantageous, providing children with a sense of security and helping them to cope with overwhelming experiences. In contrast, children who use maladaptive strategies are more likely to experience difficulties in interacting with others and may be more vulnerable to developing psychopathology.

2.2 Emotion regulation as a transdiagnostic factor

Clinical psychopathology has been dominated by a disorder-specific approach to diagnosis and treatment that does not always adequately address comorbidity and symptom overlap between disorders. Furthermore, many risk factors appear to lead to different rather than specific disorders, and the same intervention strategies appear to alleviate these different conditions [56]. These limitations have been highlighted [57], and research has investigated the processes underlying different disorders. Among these, ER has received some attention, as numerous systematic reviews have demonstrated that distinct psychological disorders are related to altered ER, both in adults [43, 58, 59] and children [5]. Due to the apparent role of ER difficulties in different psychopathologies, it has been proposed that it should be considered as a transdiagnostic mechanism [56].

A transdiagnostic framework aims to conceptually understand and explain comorbidity across psychopathology [60]. In line with this, the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) has developed the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) initiative, which provides a translational framework of basic domains of functioning—negative valence, positive valence, cognitive, social processes and arousal/regulatory systems—that underlie human behaviour, from typical to atypical manifestations, and reflect cognition, emotion, motivation and social behaviour processes. Accordingly, clinical conditions emerge from dysfunction in these processes, which predate diagnosis and can be measured through multiple levels of analysis—biological, physiological and behavioural [61, 62]. ER has gained notable importance in the transdiagnostic literature, which suggests that emotion regulation dysfunction not only contributes to distinct conditions (e.g. internalising disorders or conduct problems), but also is comorbid across different forms of psychopathology (see [5] for a review).

Research suggests that difficulties in ER reflect a mechanism underlying the co-occurrence of psychopathology in childhood. A recent meta-analysis of 60 longitudinal studies reported that maladaptive ER strategies (e.g. rumination), assessed using self-report and experimental (e.g., delay of gratification task) measures, are stable, long-term risk factors for developmental psychopathology in children and adolescents [63]. Consistent with this, another meta-analytic review of longitudinal evidence found that appropriate emotion regulation abilities, measured using parent-report questionnaires, at an early age were associated with later lower levels of psychopathology (internalising and externalising problems) [64]. Empirical evidence further supports these findings. Parent-report measures of emotion dysregulation (e.g., irritability) in children aged 3–5 years have been found to predict later mood and externalising disorders in late childhood and adolescence [65, 66].

Studies investigating experiences of childhood maltreatment also contribute to support the transdiagnostic role of ER in psychopathology. A large meta-analysis of 215 studies using report measures (i.e. questionnaires) documented that ER difficulties (e.g. rumination or suppression) in children who experienced maltreatment were positively associated with psychopathology in preschool children (i.e. internalising, and externalising problems) [67]. Consistent with this, a study using a multilevel modelling approach showed that childhood maltreatment was associated with lower positive effect and higher negative effect, as well as reduced success in ER, as assessed by report instruments [68]. A longitudinal study found that childhood maltreatment was associated with increased emotional reactivity, measured using a task-based experiment (i.e. a visual dot-probe task using affective faces), and the use of suppression and rumination strategies, measured using self-report questionnaires, which mediated the association between maltreatment and psychopathology over time [69].

Further support for the role of ER as a transdiagnostic process comes from studies of children with neurodevelopmental disorders, such as autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) [70]. ASD and ADHD preschool children tend to show marked difficulties in ER, either in parent-report questionnaires or in observational tasks (e.g. frustration-elicited task) [71]. In ASD, ER difficulties have been proposed as a core feature of this condition [72], as it is a strong predictor of symptom severity [73]. Autistic pre-schoolers, compared to typically developing children using observational methods (e.g. free play in an unstructured setting), often show less appropriate ER strategies (e.g. seeking social support) and more maladaptive strategies (e.g. avoidance), as well as increased difficulties in regulating impulsivity to emotional events and more negative effect [74]. Similarly, impulsivity in ADHD children may be related to ER difficulties. For example, a meta-analysis focusing on young children with ADHD has shown that these children have increased difficulties in emotion recognition and exhibit greater emotional lability (i.e. increased shifting of emotions, intense emotional responses), as evidenced by greater behavioural negative emotionality (e.g. anger, frustration) during frustration/challenging tasks [75].

Overall, the evidence points to the importance of adequate ER abilities at an early age and how ER dysfunction may act as a risk factor for the development and maintenance of psychopathology. Promoting effective ER abilities in early childhood not only contributes to enhanced cognitive, behavioural and interpersonal functioning but also serves as an important protective factor against various mental health problems later in life.

Advertisement

3. Neurophysiological correlates of emotion regulation

Emotional functioning develops rapidly in early childhood supported by several neurophysiological mechanisms and structures that mediate the dynamic interplay between emotion, cognition and behaviour. Although less investigated than in adults, a few studies have explored the neural and physiological correlates of ER at early ages.

Neuroimaging research highlights the role of the amygdala and prefrontal cortex (PFC) in ER throughout childhood. For example, a positive association between functional connectivity of the right amygdala-medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and parent-reported emotion regulation scores was found in children aged 4–6 years [76]. In addition, right amygdala-mPFC connectivity was negatively correlated with right amygdala reactivity to facial emotional expressions and parental reports of child negative effect. This finding is consistent with previous research in older children, adolescent and adults, suggesting that the amygdala and mPFC are key neural substrates for emotion processing and regulation across the lifespan [77].

The amygdala has been implicated in emotional reactivity and processing of the stimuli affective arousal and salience [78, 79, 80, 81]. Regions of the PFC, in turn, have been implicated in working memory, inhibitory control, attention and planning [82] processes that are essential for generating and maintaining adaptive ER strategies [80]. In addition, regions of the PFC are often connected to the amygdala, thus contributing to integrate information from the emotion generative and regulatory systems and promote adaptive ER abilities [83]. For example, depressed 4–6-year-old children showed increased activity in the right amygdala when viewing faces of different emotions compared to their non-depressed peers [84]. The authors also found a positive association between amygdala activity and parental reports of children’s negative effect but a negative association with their perceptions of children’s ability to successfully self-regulate their emotions. In another study of children aged 4–7, greater exposure to early life adversity (e.g. serious accident, parental conflict) was significantly associated with weaker amygdala–mPFC functional connectivity, which in turn was associated with more parent-report symptoms of aggressive behaviour and attention problems [85]. Further evidence of the role of these two brain regions derives from studies linking disrupted functioning of the amygdala and related brain circuits to emotional problems in young children with neurodevelopmental disorders. Significantly reduced connectivity between the amygdala and other brain areas important for socio-communicative behaviours (e.g. mPFC, temporal lobe) has been observed in autistic preschool children (mean age of 3.5 years) compared to typically developing peers [86]. Furthermore, this work observed that altered functional connectivity between the amygdala and frontal lobe was associated with greater autistic symptom severity.

The involvement of the PFC has also been confirmed in previous works using functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) to study frustration in childhood, which is particularly relevant to the regulation of emotions when a desired goal is not achieved. Increased bilateral PFC activation was observed in children aged 4–7 years in response to elicited frustration (i.e. receiving negative feedback) in social contexts, with those children who scored higher on parent-report measures of their negative affectivity showing less brain activation in this brain region [87]. Similar findings were reported in a study of 3- to 5-year-old children [88]. Greater activation in the middle PFC was observed in response to winning a prize in a computer game, whereas increased lateral PFC activation was registered in the frustration condition (i.e. losing a desired prize). Furthermore, changes (increases) in PFC activation to a frustration task were observed in children aged between 4- and 6-years following exposure to a conscious ER interpersonal scaffolding condition, in which children interacted with the examiner during a colouring activity designed to deliberately associate physiological sensations and cognitions with emotions [89]. Among children assigned to the interpersonal scaffolding condition, being rated by their parents as having better emotion regulation abilities was associated with greater increases in PFC activation.

Another neurophysiological marker associated with ER has emerged from studies using electroencephalography (EEG) and event-related potentials (ERPs). ERP frontal N2 and P3b responses were examined as indicators of ER in 4–6-year-old children [90]. The results showed a higher amplitude and shorter latency of the N2 component, as well as a larger P3b, when viewing pictures of angry (vs. happy) faces. Children described by their parents as temperamentally more fearful showed an earlier N2 response to angry faces. Frontal N2 (occurring approximately 200–400 ms after stimulus onset) and P3 (observed in adults approximately 300–500 ms after stimulus onset) components are thought to tap into inhibitory control and attentional regulation processes, as well as action and conflict monitoring, reflecting related activity in regions of the PFC, such as the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) [91, 92].

Another ERP index that has been studied in relation to ER is the late positive potential (LPP). The LPP is indicative of attentional processing of negative emotions and emerges as a potentially relevant neural signature for dysregulation of behaviour and effect [91]. The LPP response typically increases to emotional rather than neutral stimuli, but in contrast, a decrease in the LPP response has been reported following the use of ER reappraisal strategies in adults and older children (see [91, 93] for a review). In accordance with this, emotional content (vs. neutral condition) elicited larger LPP amplitudes in children aged 4–6 years [94, 95], but findings on the effect of reappraisal have been inconsistent. One study found that directed instructions to reappraise unpleasant stimuli did not down-regulate LPP, suggesting that young children may still be developing their ability to use cognitive reappraisal strategies [94]. However, another study found that pre-schoolers registered lower LPP amplitudes following simplified instructions for neutral compared to negative interpretations [96]. These inconsistencies may be related to the nature of the reappraisal task administered to the children and thus require further investigation. On the other hand, frontal EEG activation has also been implicated in emotion processing and regulation, with left frontal asymmetry (FA) (i.e. greater activation in the left hemisphere relative to the right hemisphere) being an indicator of appropriate emotional functioning and more adaptive ER abilities [97, 98]. For example, greater left FA has been found in adults during the performance of an appraisal task involving the visualisation of negative pictures [99]. In 9- to 10-year-old children, greater left FA during cognitive appraisal of a sad video clip was observed in those whose parents also used adaptive cognitive ER strategies and when the children reported greater inhibition of negative emotions [100]. Moreover, task-related FA assessed at 4½ years of age predicted later emotional functioning—greater left FA was associated with parent reports of appropriate ER abilities at 9 years of age [101]. In 4-year-old children, patterns of greater right FA were related to parent reports of more externalising and internalising problems in sociable and shy children, respectively, compared with children who showed greater left frontal activation [102]. However, a recent meta-analysis [103], examining the relationship between self-regulation (ER, effortful control and executive function) and frontal EEG activity during infancy and early childhood (up to 6 years of age), found a significant association only between frontal EEG alpha activity and executive function, but not with ER. Despite the substantial evidence suggesting that EEG frontal asymmetry is a potentially relevant marker of later maladaptive emotional functioning, future research is needed.

Finally, respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA)—computed by measuring the heart rate pattern taking into account the frequency of respiration—is a particularly important neurophysiological mechanism for the development of ER abilities and vulnerability to several forms of psychopathology [104]. RSA reflects the action of the vagus nerve supporting the link between the individual’s physiological functioning and social behaviour [105]. Greater RSA at baseline and RSA withdrawal (i.e. a decrease in RSA during challenging conditions) have been associated with better regulatory abilities and more positive developmental outcomes [106, 107, 108]. Studies show that RSA was associated with the ability of 5- to 6-month-old infants to recover and self-soothe during a Face-to-Face Still-Face paradigm [109, 110]. In addition, 10-month-old infants’ higher vagal withdrawal during an arm restraint procedure was associated with more observed maternal orienting (e.g. looking), but not distracting, regulatory behaviours [111]. RSA also discriminated physiologically between infants (6 months) classified as more or less easily frustrated, as measured by maternal reports of their temperament, and observed behaviour during a task-based experiment (i.e. frustration-elicited task), such that more easily frustrated infants displayed greater RSA and had problems with RSA withdrawal in response to an attention-demanding task [112]. Another study showed that lower RSA recovery (i.e. an index of ER) during a fear-provoking situation at 42 months of age was associated with higher parent reports of externalising problems at 48 months [113]. Children aged 2–5 years who showed higher RSA during a challenging social interaction (i.e. playing with unfamiliar peers) exhibited fewer internalising and externalising problems and better self-regulatory behaviours assessed 6–10 months later [114]. There is also evidence indicating that children’s ability to physiologically regulate themselves is related to parental behaviours and psychopathology (e.g. depression) [113, 114].

In conclusion, neuropsychophysiological research seems to indicate that specific markers may underlie adaptive and maladaptive ER. However, longitudinal studies and multi-method approaches are needed, especially when assessing young children. In this way, the developmental changes in emotion processing and regulation that occur during childhood and the acquisition of related skills (e.g. attention, inhibitory control) can be examined to gain a more comprehensive understanding of ER abilities.

Advertisement

4. Conclusion and future directions

The aim of this chapter was to highlight and discuss the importance of supporting appropriate ER abilities in early childhood. Appropriate emotion regulation strategies at an early age are likely to contribute to higher levels of emotional intelligence, as children can effectively manage, understand and deal with their own emotions [9]. Promoting emotion regulation abilities in early childhood may then lay an important foundation for increased emotional intelligence, fostering emotion recognition and contributing to improved interpersonal relationships and social interactions.

In this chapter, we have presented how the current literature supports the development of adjusted ER abilities in young children. Adaptive ER strategies are associated with long-term cognitive, social and mental health outcomes and have a significant impact on children’s successful development. Furthermore, we have examined current evidence on the role of neuropsychophysiological markers associated with ER abilities and how specific physiological responses underlie adaptive and/or maladaptive strategies. In summary, the literature suggests that promoting and supporting the development of appropriate ER abilities, both behavioural and physiological, in early childhood will enable children to be emotionally competent, deal effectively with personal and interpersonal difficulties as well as work with others in social settings, to the benefit of children, families, educators and society.

However, it is important to recognise that the optimal development of ER abilities occurs in an interpersonal context. Although this topic was not the aim of this chapter, it is important to consider that ER emerges in the socialisation process and that the parent/caregiver-child relationship is crucial at an early age for their optimal regulation. Therefore, future studies should address the dyadic context in the emergence and development of ER. For example, examine how parenting styles (e.g. permissive, or authoritative), or the role of intergenerational transmission, affect the development of ER abilities (e.g. whether children tend to adopt similar strategies as their caregivers/parents). Similarly, future studies should investigate how peer interactions (e.g. friendships and social support networks, e.g. school setting) influence emotion management and regulation in young children. It is also important to investigate how cultural factors may influence the development and effectiveness of ER strategies from an early age, as evidence in adults suggests that culture may lead individuals to regulate their emotions differently [115]. In the same line, future neurophysiological studies should consider the contributions of interbrain synchrony (i.e. spatial and/or temporal coordination of biological and behavioural systems between individuals, [116])—particularly in the context of parent/caregiver-child interaction—to the development of appropriate ER abilities.

Finally, as we live in the digital age, it is increasingly important to examine the impact of technology on ER. At an increasingly young age, children are often exposed to and interact with others through digital devices and social media. Future research should investigate how digital communication and social media may affect the development of ER and EI and whether it differs from face-to-face interactions.

Advertisement

Acknowledgments

This work is funded by national funds through the FCT—The Portuguese Foundation for Science and Technology, I.P., under the Project UIDB/04375/2020.

Advertisement

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. 1. Tierney AL, Nelson CA III. Brain development and the role of experience in the early years. Zero to Three. 2009;30(2):9-13
  2. 2. Harrington EM, Trevino SD, Lopez S, Giuliani NR. Emotion regulation in early childhood: Implications for socioemotional and academic components of school readiness. Emotion. 2020;20(1):48-53. DOI: 10.1037/emo0000667
  3. 3. Calkins SD, Marcovitch S. Emotion regulation and executive functioning in early development: Integrated mechanisms of control supporting adaptive functioning. In: Child Development at the Intersection of Emotion and Cognition. Washington: American Psychological Association; 2010. pp. 37-57. DOI: 10.1037/12059-003
  4. 4. Ornaghi V, Conte E, Grazzani I. Empathy in toddlers: The role of emotion regulation, language ability, and maternal emotion socialization style. Frontiers in Psychology. 2020;11:1-11. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.586862
  5. 5. Aldao A, Gee DG, De Los RA, Seager I. Emotion regulation as a transdiagnostic factor in the development of internalizing and externalizing psychopathology: Current and future directions. Development and Psychopathology. 2016;28(4 pt 1):927-946. DOI: 10.1017/S0954579416000638
  6. 6. Kopp CB, Neufeld SJ. Emotional development during infancy. In: Handbook of Affective Sciences. New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press; 2003. pp. 347-374. (Series in affective science)
  7. 7. Ruba AL, Pollak SD. The development of emotion reasoning in infancy and early childhood. Annual Review of Developmental Psychology. 2020;2(1):503-531. DOI: 10.1146/annurev-devpsych-060320-102556
  8. 8. National Scientific Council on the Developing Child. The Science of Early Childhood Development: Closing the Gap Between What We Know and What We Do. 2007. Available from: www.developingchild.harvard.edu
  9. 9. Peña-Sarrionandia A, Mikolajczak M, Gross JJ. Integrating emotion regulation and emotional intelligence traditions: A meta-analysis. Frontiers in Psychology. 2015;6:160. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00160
  10. 10. Smieja M, Mrozowicz-Wronska M, Kobylinska D. Emotional intelligence and emotion regulation strategies. Studia Psychologiczne. 2011;49(5):55-64. DOI: 10.2478/v10167-010-0040-x
  11. 11. Eggum ND, Eisenberg N, Kao K, Spinrad TL, Bolnick R, Hofer C, et al. Emotion understanding, theory of mind, and prosocial orientation: Relations over time in early childhood. The Journal of Positive Psychology. 2011;6(1):4-16. DOI: 10.1080/17439760.2010.536776
  12. 12. Raver C, Garner P, Smith-Donald R. The roles of emotion regulation and emotion knowledge for children’s academic readiness: Are the links causal? In: School Readiness and the Transition to Kindergarten in the Era of Accountability. Baltimore, MD, US: Paul H Brooks Publishing Co.; 2007. pp. 121-147
  13. 13. Qualter P, Urquijo I, Henzi P, Barrett L, Humphrey N. Ability emotional intelligence and children’s behaviour in the playground. Social Development. 2019;28(2):430-448. DOI: 10.1111/sode.12340
  14. 14. Salovey P, Mayer JD. Emotional intelligence. Imagination, Cognition and Personality. 1990;9(3):185-211. DOI: 10.2190/DUGG-P24E-52WK-6CDG
  15. 15. Mayer JD, Roberts RD, Barsade SG. Human abilities: Emotional intelligence. Annual Review of Psychology. 2008;59(1):507-536. DOI: 10.1146/annurev-psy.59.103006.093646
  16. 16. Denham SA. Emotional competence during childhood and adolescence. In: Handbook of Emotional Development. Cham: Springer International Publishing; 2019. pp. 493-541
  17. 17. Denham SA. Dealing with feelings: How children negotiate the worlds of emotions and social relationships. Cogniţie Creier Comportament. 2007;XI(1):1-48
  18. 18. Robson DA, Allen MS, Howard SJ. Self-regulation in childhood as a predictor of future outcomes: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin. 2020;146(4):324-354. DOI: 10.1037/bul0000227
  19. 19. Bargh JA, Williams LE. The nonconscious regulation of emotion. In: Handbook of Emotion Regulation. New York, NY, US: The Guilford Press; 2007. pp. 429-445
  20. 20. Thompson RA. Emotion regulation: A theme in search of definition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 1994;59(2/3):25. DOI: 10.2307/1166137
  21. 21. Raikes HA, Thompson RA. Attachment security and parenting quality predict children’s problem-solving, attributions, and loneliness with peers. Attachment & Human Development. 2008;10(3):319-344. DOI: 10.1080/14616730802113620
  22. 22. Gross J. In: Gross JJ, editor. Handbook of Emotion Regulation. 2nd ed. Vol. 669. New York, NY, US: The Guilford Press; 2014. p. xviii
  23. 23. McRae K, Gross JJ. Emotion regulation. Emotion. 2020;20(1):1-9. DOI: 10.1037/emo0000703
  24. 24. Eisenberg N. Prosocial behavior, empathy, and sympathy. In: Well-Being: Positive Development across the Life Course. Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; 2003. pp. 253-265
  25. 25. Eisenberg N, HC and VJ. Effortful control and its socio-emotional consequences. In: Gross J, editor. Handbook of Emotion Regulation. Washington, DC, US: Guilford Press; 2007. pp. 287-306
  26. 26. McClelland MM, Cameron CE. Self-regulation in early childhood: Improving conceptual clarity and developing ecologically valid measures. Child Development Perspectives. 2012;6(2):136-142. DOI: 10.1111/j.1750-2011.00191.x
  27. 27. Calkins SD, Perry NB. The development of emotion regulation: Implications for child adjustment. In: Developmental Psychopathology. New York, US: Wiley; 2016. pp. 1-56. DOI: 10.1002/9781119125556.devpsy306
  28. 28. Cole PM. Moving ahead in the study of the development of emotion regulation. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2014;38(2):203-207. DOI: 10.1177/0165025414522170
  29. 29. Morris AS, Silk JS, Steinberg L, Myers SS, Robinson LR. The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Social Development. 2007;16(2):361-388. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00389.x
  30. 30. Thompson RA. Emotion and emotion regulation: Two sides of the developing coin. Emotion Review. 2011;3(1):53-61. DOI: 10.1177/17540739103910380969
  31. 31. Zeman J, Cassano M, Perry-Parrish C, Stegall S. Emotion regulation in children and adolescents. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics. 2006;27(2):155-168. DOI: 10.1097/00004703-200604000-00014
  32. 32. Campos JJ, Frankel CB, Camras L. On the nature of emotion regulation. Child Development. 2004;75(2):377-394. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00681.x
  33. 33. Crowell JA. Development of emotion regulation in typically developing children. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America. 2021;30(3):467-474. DOI: 10.1016/j.chc.2021.04.001
  34. 34. Thompson RA, Lagattuta KH. Feeling and understanding: Early emotional development. In: Blackwell Handbook of Early Childhood Development. Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishing Ltd; 2006. pp. 317-337. DOI: 10.1002/9780470757703.ch16
  35. 35. Groves NB, Kofler MJ, Wells EL, Day TN, Chan ESM. An examination of relations among working memory, ADHD symptoms, and emotion regulation. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2020;48(4):525-537. DOI: 10.1007/s10802-019-00612-8
  36. 36. LaBounty J, Wellman HM, Olson S, Lagattuta K, Liu D. Mothers’ and fathers’ use of internal state talk with their young children. Social Development. 2008;17(4):757-775. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00450.x
  37. 37. Sabatier C, Cervantes DR, Torres MM, De los Rios OH, Sañudo JP. Emotion regulation in children and adolescents: Concepts, processes and influences. Psicología desde el Caribe. 2017;34(1):75-90
  38. 38. Pons F, Harris PL, de Rosnay M. Emotion comprehension between 3 and 11 years: Developmental periods and hierarchical organization. European Journal of Developmental Psychology. 2004;1(2):127-152. DOI: 10.1080/17405620344000022
  39. 39. Webb TL, Miles E, Sheeran P. Dealing with feeling: A meta-analysis of the effectiveness of strategies derived from the process model of emotion regulation. Psychological Bulletin. 2012;138(4):775-808. DOI: 10.1037/a0027600
  40. 40. Gross JJ, Feldman BL. Emotion generation and emotion regulation: One or two depends on your point of view. Emotion Review. 2011;3(1):8-16. DOI: 10.1177/1754073910380974
  41. 41. Sala MN, Pons F, Molina P. Emotion regulation strategies in preschool children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 2014;32(4):440-453. DOI: 10.1111/bjdp.12055
  42. 42. Dennis TA, Kelemen DA. Preschool children’s views on emotion regulation: Functional associations and implications for social-emotional adjustment. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2009;33(3):243-252. DOI: 10.1177/0165025408098024
  43. 43. Aldao A, Nolen-Hoeksema S, Schweizer S. Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review. 2010;30(2):217-237. DOI: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004
  44. 44. Gross JJ. The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Review of General Psychology. 1998;2(3):271-299. DOI: 10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271
  45. 45. Luca M. Maladaptive rumination as a transdiagnostic mediator of vulnerability and outcome in psychopathology. Journal of Clinical Medicine. 2019;8(3):314. DOI: 10.3390/jcm8030314
  46. 46. Gross JJ, Thompson RA. Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations. In: Handbook of Emotion Regulation. New York, NY, US: The Guilford Press; 2007. pp. 3-24
  47. 47. John OP, Gross JJ. Healthy and unhealthy emotion regulation: Personality processes, individual differences, and life span development. Journal of Personality. 2004;72(6):1301-1334. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00298
  48. 48. Nolen-Hoeksema S, Aldao A. Gender and age differences in emotion regulation strategies and their relationship to depressive symptoms. Personality and Individual Differences. 2011;51(6):704-708. DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.06.012
  49. 49. Watkins ER. Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought. Psychological Bulletin. 2008;134(2):163-206. DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.163
  50. 50. Papageorgiou C. An empirical test of a clinical metacognitive model of rumination and depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2003;27(3):261-273. DOI: 10.1023/A:1023962332399
  51. 51. Hong RY. Worry and rumination: Differential associations with anxious and depressive symptoms and coping behavior. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2007;45(2):277-290. DOI: 10.1016/j.brat.2006.03.006
  52. 52. Ward A, Lyubomirsky S, Sousa L, Nolen-Hoeksema S. Can’t quite commit: Rumination and uncertainty. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2003;29(1):96-107. DOI: 10.1177/0146167202238375
  53. 53. Coelho V, Cadima J, Pinto AI. Child functioning in inclusive preschools: Associations between self-regulation, engagement, prosociality, and hyperactivity. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 2023;86:101518. DOI: 10.16/j.appdev.2023.101518
  54. 54. Helmsen J, Koglin U, Petermann F. Emotion regulation and aggressive behavior in preschoolers: The mediating role of social information processing. Child Psychiatry and Human Development. 2012;43(1):87-101. DOI: 10.1007/s10578-011-0252-3
  55. 55. Gunzenhauser C, von Suchodoletz A. Preschoolers’ use of suppression influences subsequent self-control but does not interfere with verbal memory. Learning and Individual Differences. 2014;32:219-224. DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2014.03.007
  56. 56. Dalgleish T, Black M, Johnston D, Bevan A. Transdiagnostic approaches to mental health problems: Current status and future directions. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2020;88(3):179-195. DOI: 10.1037/ccp0000482
  57. 57. Hofmann SG, Hayes SC. The future of intervention science: Process-based therapy. Clinical Psychological Science. 2019;7(1):37-50. DOI: 10.1177/2167702618772296
  58. 58. Dryman MT, Heimberg RG. Emotion regulation in social anxiety and depression: A systematic review of expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal. Clinical Psychology Review. 2018;65:17-42. DOI: 10.1016/j.cpr.2018.07.004
  59. 59. Sloan E, Hall K, Moulding R, Bryce S, Mildred H, Staiger PK. Emotion regulation as a transdiagnostic treatment construct across anxiety, depression, substance, eating and borderline personality disorders: A systematic review. Clinical Psychology Review. 2017;57:141-163. DOI: 10.1016/j.cpr.2017.09.002
  60. 60. Bruno A, Iannuzzo F, Muscatello MRA. Comorbidity from a categorical to a transdiagnostic-dimensional approach: New perspectives for researchers and clinicians. Clinical Neuropsychiatry. 2023;20:7-8. DOI: 10.36131/cnfioritieditore20230101
  61. 61. Cuthbert BN. The role of RDoC in future classification of mental disorders. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience. 2020;22(1):81-85. DOI: 10.31887/DCNS.2020.22.1/bcuthbert
  62. 62. Cuthbert BN, Insel TR. Toward the future of psychiatric diagnosis: The seven pillars of RDoC. BMC Medicine. 2013;11(1):126. DOI: 10.1186/1741-7015-11-1126
  63. 63. Cavicchioli M, Tobia V, Ogliari A. Emotion regulation strategies as risk factors for developmental psychopathology: A meta-analytic review of longitudinal studies based on cross-lagged correlations and panel models. Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopatholog. 2023;51(3):295-315. DOI: 10.1007/s10802-022-00980-8
  64. 64. Compas BE, Jaser SS, Bettis AH, Watson KH, Gruhn MA, Dunbar JP, et al. Coping, emotion regulation, and psychopathology in childhood and adolescence: A meta-analysis and narrative review. Psychological Bulletin. 2017;143(9):939-991. DOI: 10.1037/bul0000110
  65. 65. Paulus FW, Ohmann S, Möhler E, Plener P, Popow C. Emotional dysregulation in children and adolescents with psychiatric disorders. A narrative review. Frontiers in Psychiatry. 2021;12:628252. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.628252
  66. 66. Vogel AC, Jackson JJ, Barch DM, Tillman R, Luby JL. Excitability and irritability in preschoolers predicts later psychopathology: The importance of positive and negative emotion dysregulation. Development and Psychopathology. 2019;31(3):1067-1083. DOI: 10.1017/S0954579419000609
  67. 67. Miu AC, Szentágotai-Tătar A, Balázsi R, Nechita D, Bunea I, Pollak SD. Emotion regulation as mediator between childhood adversity and psychopathology: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review. 2022;93:102141. DOI: 10.1016/j.cpr.2022.102141
  68. 68. Ion A, Bîlc MI, Pițur S, Pop CF, Szentágotai-Tătar A, Miu AC. Childhood maltreatment and emotion regulation in everyday life: An experience sampling study. Scientific Reports. 2023;13(1):7214. DOI: 10.1038/s41598-023-34302-9
  69. 69. Weissman DG, Bitran D, Miller AB, Schaefer JD, Sheridan MA, McLaughlin KA. Difficulties with emotion regulation as a transdiagnostic mechanism linking child maltreatment with the emergence of psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology. 2019;31(3):899-915. DOI: 10.1017/S0954579419000348
  70. 70. England-Mason G. Emotion regulation as a transdiagnostic feature in children with neurodevelopmental disorders. Current Developmental Disorders Reports. 2020;7(3):130-138. DOI: 10.1007/s40474-020-00200-2
  71. 71. Ros R, Graziano PA. A transdiagnostic examination of self-regulation: Comparisons across preschoolers with ASD, ADHD, and typically developing children. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology. 2020;49(4):493-508. DOI: 10.1080/15374416.2019.1591280
  72. 72. Berkovits L, Eisenhower A, Blacher J. Emotion regulation in young children with autism Spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2017;47(1):68-79. DOI: 10.1007/s10803-016-2922-2
  73. 73. Fenning RM, Baker JK, Moffitt J. Intrinsic and extrinsic predictors of emotion regulation in children with autism Spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2018;48(11):3858-3870. DOI: 10.1007/s18803-018-3647-1
  74. 74. Mazefsky CA, Herrington J, Siegel M, Scarpa A, Maddox BB, Scahill L, et al. The role of emotion regulation in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2013;52(7):679-688. DOI: 10.1016/j.jaac.2013.05.006
  75. 75. Graziano PA, Garcia A. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and children’s emotion dysregulation: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review. 2016;46:106-123. DOI: 10.1016/j.cpr.2016.04.011
  76. 76. Gaffrey MS, Barch DM, Luby JL, Petersen SE. Amygdala functional connectivity is associated with emotion regulation and amygdala reactivity in 4-to 6-year-olds. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2021;60(1):176-185. DOI: 10.1016/j.jaac.2020.01.024
  77. 77. Gabard-Durnam LJ, Flannery J, Goff B, Gee DG, Humphreys KL, Telzer E, et al. The development of human amygdala functional connectivity at rest from 4 to 23 years: A cross-sectional study. NeuroImage. 2014;95:193-207. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.038
  78. 78. Adolphs R. The social brain: Neural basis of social knowledge. Annual Review of Psychology. 2009;60:693-716. DOI: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163514
  79. 79. Pessoa L. Emotion and cognition and the amygdala: From “what is it?” to “what's to be done?”. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48(12):3416-3429. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.06.038
  80. 80. Sebastian CL, Ahmed SP. The neurobiology of emotion regulation. In: Beech AR, Carter AJ, Mann RE, Rotshtein P, editors. The Wiley Blackwell Handbook of Forensic Neuroscience. 1st ed. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons; 2018. DOI: 10.1002/9781118650868.ch6
  81. 81. Todd RM, Evans JW, Morris D, Lewis MD, Taylor MJ. The changing face of emotion: Age-related patterns of amygdala activation to salient faces. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2011;6(1):12-23. DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsq007
  82. 82. Fuster JM. The Prefrontal Cortex. 4th ed. San Diego: Elsevier; 2008
  83. 83. Berboth S, Morawetz C. Amygdala-prefrontal connectivity during emotion regulation: A meta-analysis of psychophysiological interactions. Neuropsychologia. 2021;153:107767. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2021.107767
  84. 84. Gaffrey MS, Barch DM, Singer J, Shenoy R, Luby JL. Disrupted amygdala reactivity in depressed 4-to 6-year-old children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2013;52(7):737-746. DOI: 10.1016/j.jaac.2013.04.009
  85. 85. Park AT, Leonard JA, Saxler PK, Cyr AB, Gabrieli JD, Mackey AP. Amygdala–medial prefrontal cortex connectivity relates to stress and mental health in early childhood. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2018;13(4):430-439. DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsy017
  86. 86. Shen MD, Li DD, Keown CL, Lee A, Johnson RT, Angkustsiri K, et al. Functional connectivity of the amygdala is disrupted in preschool-aged children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2016;55(9):817-824. DOI: 10.1016/j.jaac.2016.05.020
  87. 87. Camacho MC, Williams EM, Ding K, Perlman SB. Multimodal examination of emotion processing systems associated with negative affectivity across early childhood. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2021;48:100917. DOI: 10.1016/j.dcn.2021.100917
  88. 88. Perlman SB, Luna B, Hein TC, Huppert TJ. fNIRS evidence of prefrontal regulation of frustration in early childhood. NeuroImage. 2014;85:326-334. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.057
  89. 89. Grabell AS, Huppert TJ, Fishburn FA, Li Y, Hlutkowsky CO, Jones HM, et al. Neural correlates of early deliberate emotion regulation: Young children’s responses to interpersonal scaffolding. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2019;40:100708. DOI: 10.1016/j.dcn.2019.100708
  90. 90. Lewis MD, Todd RM, Honsberger MJ. Event-related potential measures of emotion regulation in early childhood. Neuroreport. 2007;18(1):61-65. DOI: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e328010a216
  91. 91. Dennis TA. Neurophysiological markers for child emotion regulation from the perspective of emotion–cognition integration: Current directions and future challenges. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2010;35(2):212-230. DOI: 10.1080/87565640903526579
  92. 92. Lewis MD, Lamm C, Segalowitz SJ, Stieben J, Zelazo PD. Neurophysiological correlates of emotion regulation in children and adolescents. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2006;18(3):430-443. DOI: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.3.430
  93. 93. Hajcak G, MacNamara A, Olvet DM. Event-related potentials, emotion, and emotion regulation: An integrative review. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2010;35(2):129-155. DOI: 10.1080/87565640903526504
  94. 94. DeCicco JM, Solomon B, Dennis TA. Neural correlates of cognitive reappraisal in children: An ERP study. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2012;2(1):70-80. DOI: 10.1016/j.dcn.2011.05.009
  95. 95. Hua M, Han ZR, Chen S, Yang M, Zhou R, Hu S. Late positive potential (LPP) modulation during affective picture processing in preschoolers. Biological Psychology. 2014;101:77-81. DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.06.006
  96. 96. Hua M, Han ZR, Zhou R. Cognitive reappraisal in preschoolers: Neuropsychological evidence of emotion regulation from an ERP study. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2015;40(5):279-290. DOI: 10.1080/875656641.2015.1069827
  97. 97. Fox NA. If it’s not left, it’s right: Electroencephalograph asymmetry and the development of emotion. The American Psychologist. 1991;46(8):863-872. DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.46.8.863
  98. 98. Fox NA. Dynamic cerebral processes underlying emotion regulation. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 1994;59(2-3):152-166. DOI: 10.2307/1166143
  99. 99. Choi D, Sekiya T, Minote N, Watanuki S. Relative left frontal activity in reappraisal and suppression of negative emotion: Evidence from frontal alpha asymmetry (FAA). International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2016;109:37-44. DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2016.09.018
  100. 100. Meza-Cervera T, Tucker A, Liu R, Bell MA. Child emotion inhibition mediates the effect of parent's adaptive cognitive emotion regulation on child frontal EEG asymmetry during reappraisal. Developmental Psychobiology. 2022;64(8):e22339. DOI: 10.1002/dev.22339
  101. 101. Hannesdóttir DK, Doxie J, Bell MA, Ollendick TH, Wolfe CD. A longitudinal study of emotion regulation and anxiety in middle childhood: Associations with frontal EEG asymmetry in early childhood. Developmental Psychobiology. 2010;52(2):197-204. DOI: 10.1002/dev.20425
  102. 102. Fox NA, Schmidt LA, Calkins SD, Rubin KH, Coplan RJ. The role of frontal activation in the regulation and dysregulation of social behavior during the preschool year. Development and Psychopathology. 1996;8(1):89-102. DOI: 10.1017/S0954579400006982
  103. 103. Hofstee M, Huijding J, Cuevas K, Deković M. Self-regulation and frontal EEG alpha activity during infancy and early childhood: A multilevel meta-analysis. Developmental Science. 2022;25(6):e13298. DOI: 10.1111/desc.13298
  104. 104. Beauchaine TP. Respiratory sinus arrhythmia: A transdiagnostic biomarker of emotion dysregulation and psychopathology. Current Opinion in Psychology. 2015;3:43-47. DOI: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.01.017
  105. 105. Porges SW. The polyvagal perspective. Biological Psychology. 2007;74(2):116-143. DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.06.009
  106. 106. Blandon AY, Calkins SD, Keane SP, O'Brien M. Individual differences in trajectories of emotion regulation processes: The effects of maternal depressive symptomatology and children's physiological regulation. Developmental Psychology. 2008;44(4):1110-1123. DOI: 10.1037/0012-1649.44.4.1110
  107. 107. Gentzler AL, Santucci AK, Kovacs M, Fox NA. Respiratory sinus arrhythmia reactivity predicts emotion regulation and depressive symptoms in at-risk and control children. Biological Psychology. 2009;82(2):156-163. DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.07.002
  108. 108. Graziano P, Derefinko K. Cardiac vagal control and children's adaptive functioning: A meta-analysis. Biological Psychology. 2013;94(1):22-37. DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.04.011
  109. 109. Ham J, Tronick ED. Infant resilience to the stress of the still-face: Infant and maternal psychophysiology are related. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2006;1094:297-302. DOI: 10.1196/annals.1376.038
  110. 110. Suurland J, van der Heijden KB, Smaling HJ, Huijbregts SC, van Goozen SH, Swaab H. Infant autonomic nervous system response and recovery: Associations with maternal risk status and infant emotion regulation. Development and Psychopathology. 2017;29(3):759-773. DOI: 10.1017/S0954579416000456
  111. 111. Perry NB, Calkins SD, Bell MA. Indirect effects of maternal sensitivity on infant emotion regulation behaviors: The role of vagal withdrawal. Infancy. 2016;21(2):128-153. DOI: 10.1111/infa.12101
  112. 112. Calkins SD, Dedmon SE, Gill KL, Lomax LE, Johnson LM. Frustration in infancy: Implications for emotion regulation, physiological processes, and temperament. Infancy. 2002;3(2):175-197. DOI: 10.1207/S15327078IN0302_4
  113. 113. Tsotsi S, Borelli JL, Backer M, Veragoo N, Abdulla N, Tan KH, et al. Preschoolers’ emotion reactivity and regulation: Links with maternal psychological distress and child behavior problems. Development and Psychopathology. 2023;35(3):1079-1091. DOI: 10.1017/S0954579421000936
  114. 114. Hastings PD, Nuselovici JN, Utendale WT, Coutya J, McShane KE, Sullivan C. Applying the polyvagal theory to children's emotion regulation: Social context, socialization, and adjustment. Biological Psychology. 2008;79(3):299-306. DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.07.005
  115. 115. Ford BQ , Mauss IB. Culture and emotion regulation. Current Opinion in Psychology. 2015;1(3):1-5. DOI: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2014.12.004
  116. 116. Holroyd CB. Interbrain synchrony: On wavy ground. Trends in Neurosciences. 2022;45(5):346-357. DOI: 10.1016/j.tins.2022.02.002

Written By

Sara Cruz, Mariana Sousa and Vera Mateus

Submitted: 18 September 2023 Reviewed: 12 January 2024 Published: 01 February 2024