Open access peer-reviewed chapter

Modes of Acetylcholine Signaling in the Prefrontal Cortex: Implications for Cholinergic Dysfunction and Disorders

Written By

Matthew Fecik and Lisa M. Savage

Submitted: 04 January 2023 Reviewed: 10 February 2023 Published: 22 May 2023

DOI: 10.5772/intechopen.110462

From the Edited Volume

Acetylcholine - Recent Advances and New Perspectives

Edited by Thomas Heinbockel

Chapter metrics overview

147 Chapter Downloads

View Full Metrics

Abstract

The forebrain cholinergic system is an important mediator of arousal, attention, memory, and other cognitive processes. Cholinergic signaling is typically divided into two patterns, tonic signaling, which involves sustained changes in ambient acetylcholine (ACh) tone over seconds to minutes, and phasic signaling, which involves fast changing, spatially specific release of ACh on a millisecond timescale. There is evidence to suggest unique functional roles for both types of signaling in the prefrontal cortex: phasic release of ACh is thought to be necessary for attentional processes, as well as cue detection, while tonic signaling is thought to be involved in regulating global arousal states and has been shown to increase with general cognitive demand. The differences between these two types of signaling may originate from electrophysiological properties of cholinergic cell types, distinct muscarinic and nicotinic receptor utilization and/or expression, and/or differential hydrolysis of ACh by acetylcholinesterase. This review will summarize the current views on the functional role of each type of signaling, while the contributions of ACh receptors, hydrolysis, and basal forebrain anatomy are examined. Additionally, the implications of these factors in ACh signaling will be examined in terms of cholinergic circuit dysfunction that occurs in neurodegenerative diseases.

Keywords

  • acetylcholine
  • prefrontal cortex
  • attention
  • arousal
  • Alzheimer’s disease
  • alcohol use disorder

1. Introduction

While the role of acetylcholine (ACh) as a neuromodulatory regulator of REM sleep, global arousal states, and consciousness in the prefrontal cortex has been known for quite some time [1, 2, 3], the evidence that ACh also functions as a fast-acting neurotransmitter on a precise spatial and temporal scale to carry out specific cognitive operations has only begun to be explored within the past two decades, with progress accelerating significantly in recent years [4, 5, 6]. Studies elucidating short-duration phasic cholinergic signaling in the cortex have been made possible by the use of techniques for collecting temporally precise data on ACh in vivo, beginning with electrochemical detection [7], and more recently including optogenetics and fiber photometry [8, 9, 10, 11]. Such techniques open exciting new possibilities to advance our understanding of cholinergic neurotransmission in the brain, and importantly, our understanding and treatment of brain disorders such as Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and alcohol use disorder (AUD).

ACh functions in the prefrontal cortex are based on a variety of factors such as cortical cytoarchitecture, receptor utilization, cholinesterase activity, and basal forebrain anatomy. It is therefore necessary to reevaluate our understanding of the literature on disorders of the basal forebrain through the lens of the tonic/phasic ACh signaling distinction. The goal of this review is to define what is currently known about tonic and phasic ACh signaling in the prefrontal cortex, how the heterogeneity between these two types of signaling may arise, review the differences in receptor utilization these two types of transmission require, evaluate the role of acetylcholinesterase activity in this distinction, and review some of the deficits to each type of transmission that occur in both AD and AUD.

Advertisement

2. Modes of ACh signaling and their roles in behavior

2.1 Tonic ACh signaling

Tonic signaling (sometimes referred to as volume transmission) represents the traditional conceptualization of cholinergic transmission in the central nervous system. In this form of signaling, ACh functions as a slow-acting neuromodulator that exerts its effects on timescales of several seconds to minutes or even hours. Neuromodulatory ACh originating from the basal forebrain has been shown to be involved in functions such as the transition between arousal states, REM sleep, and exploratory behavior [2, 12, 13, 14, 15]. Tonic signaling tends to lack both spatial and temporal precision.

Early evidence suggested that “ACh efflux” was not only involved in global arousal states but also directly involved in attentional processes and reward-based learning [16, 17, 18]. An early study using in vivo microdialysis measured ACh efflux in the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) during a 5-choice serial reaction time task (5-CSRTT) and found that in male rats that were well trained on this task, there was increased ACh efflux, an effect that was attenuated in rats not previously trained [19]. Interestingly, they found that ACh efflux was positively correlated with the number of trials completed during the session in which the cue light was illuminated for 5 seconds every trial, compared to 0.5 or 0.25 seconds. The authors contribute that this rise is dependent on the fact that the longer stimulus may elongate the saliency of the food reward and novelty of the task, which has been shown to be dependent on ACh [1820]. However, a follow-up study demonstrated that the increase in ACh efflux was due to the performance of the task alone, as rats whose reinforcement was yoked to another subject’s performance rather than their own did not show the same increase in ACh efflux, despite also receiving the same pattern of rewards and being exposed to the cue stimuli [21]. Later literature would go on to implicate attentional control of behavior, particularly cue detection, as a cognitive function dependent on phasic ACh signaling, not general ACh tone [9, 22, 23].

Understanding of tonic cholinergic signaling came mostly from studies using in vivo microdialysis, in which cholinesterase activity is typically inhibited to ensure a sufficient concentration of ACh for electrochemical detection. This leads to a potential misunderstanding of cholinergic activity in the frontal cortex, with some suggesting that ACh “tone” may even be a methodological artifact [4]. One of the reasons for such a conclusion stems from the seemingly hyperefficient action of AChE, which is the main enzyme involved in the hydrolysis of ACh in the synaptic cleft and therefore the termination of its action. However, it has been shown that a sufficient volume of ACh is able to transiently inhibit AChE [24, 25], leading to further increases in ACh that can potentially spill over into the extracellular fluid and increase cholinergic tone. Such a mechanism would involve a positive feedback loop in which significant quantities of ACh release can inhibit AChE, further increasing the concentration of extracellular ACh. However, this inhibition may be short-lived [24, 25], perhaps on the timescale of milliseconds, and therefore may cause small “pulses” of spillover without hindering fast synaptic phasic signaling at the point of inhibition.

However, the possibility that visual attention tasks require both intact tonic and phasic ACh signaling in the mPFC cannot be ruled out, and it is possible that performance on these tasks requires an alert attentional state governed by tonic signaling, which has been shown to mediate shifts in attention, as well as phasic signaling to encode for specific behavioral epochs and serve as an indicator of cue presentation. Such a mechanism would have been missed by the microdialysis approaches. Thus, the possibility that task performance is reliant on phasic signaling, while behavioral engagement is dependent on the ambient tonic signaling that is perpetually present in the cortex cannot be ruled out and the delineation of the two is a potential area of future research.

2.2 Phasic ACh signaling

Contrasted with tonic ACh signaling, phasic signaling involves much faster time scale circuit-specific release. Phasic signaling is thought to be wired, meaning that it involves the release of ACh from a single cell that innervates a single cortical cell in a highly specific manner [4]. Unlike tonic signaling, it does not involve spillover from the synapse into the extracellular fluid, rather it is constrained to the synapse. This type of signaling occurs on a much faster timescale—likely that of only hundreds of milliseconds. This type of signaling has been implicated in cortical control of attention, specifically cue detection—the cognitive process needed to determine whether a cue that signals a reward is present or not [6].

Based on work using mice, it has been demonstrated that projections originating in the nucleus basalis of Meynert (NbM) and substantia innominata (SI), which form the NbM complex, send their axons to the mPFC and are necessary for cue detection [6]. Particularly, it is thought that these projections are involved in the shift between vigilance and cue detection [6]. This is further demonstrated by the fact that disruption of the mPFC’s cholinergic innervation impairs cue detection, while disrupting projections to the other targets of the NbM, such as the motor cortex, yields no effect on this task [23]. Phasic ACh signaling in this circuit is likely the causal mediator of cue detection, as it has been shown that optogenetic stimulation of the NbM during a cue detection task improved performance during cued trials and increased the false alarm rate during non-cued trials, suggesting that millisecond timescale cholinergic signaling originating in the NbM is involved directly in the encoding of the representation of the cue in the prefrontal cortex in mice [9].

In addition, there is evidence to suggest that the basal forebrain is involved in the formation of stimulus associations in mice. Tu et al. [11] found that cholinergic signaling during associative conditioning can affect the strength of the association formed in a time-dependent manner. Although the NbM is commonly cited as the primary source of cholinergic projections to the prelimbic cortex (PrL), and injection of retrograde tracer into the PrL revealed that the greatest source of cholinergic innervation originated in the horizontal diagonal band (HDB). Therefore, Tu et al. [11] targeted the PrL projections originating in the HDB for optogenetic manipulation and found that stimulation during the unconditioned stimulus impaired associative learning while inhibition facilitated it. Furthermore, they found that optogenetic stimulation during the conditioned stimulus did not affect the strength of associative learning, but inhibition lead to impaired learning. This was further accompanied by fiber photometry data that show that the level of excitation of the PrL correlates with the strength of the memory, such that ACh signaling during the unconditioned stimulus strengthened across sessions. These data suggest that phasic signaling is uniquely sensitive to timing, such that the functional role of the cholinergic projections from the HDB to the PrL region involves specifically timed excitation, further lending evidence for the role of ACh in encoding specific stimulus representations [11].

One of the ways that cholinergic signaling can be achieved on this timescale is through the actions of acetylcholinesterase (AChE), which is an incredibly effective hydrolytic enzyme, making the local regulation of AChE as one of the ways in which a degree of heterogeneity is introduced between tonic and phasic ACh signaling. Local expression of AChE may contribute to some of the anatomical heterogeneity between tonic and phasic signaling and makes it likely that phasic signaling would occur, due to its potent catalytic action. However, this has not been investigated thoroughly and represents a future area of inquiry. As cholinesterase activity is likely one of the most important regulators of spatially and temporally restricted ACh signaling in the prefrontal cortex, our understanding of its spatial distribution in the cortex is of paramount importance.

2.3 Tonic vs. phasic signaling: differing viewpoints on the distinction

The presence of such a highly potent catalytic mechanism such as the hydrolysis of ACh by AChE has led some to suggest that tonic ACh signaling in the forebrain is unlikely to affect behavior at all. In this view, the fact that the rate-limiting step of ACh hydrolysis is the diffusion of ACh into the synapse, and not the hydrolytic action of AChE itself, is evidence to suggest that ACh is unlikely to travel distances beyond the synapse and therefore changes in extracellular ACh concentration is unlikely to be a contributor to behavioral events [4]. However, others believe that the tonic/phasic distinction is an oversimplification and that ACh signaling most likely has both fast and slow components that both contribute to behavior. In this view, cholinergic signaling varies as a function of anatomy, receptor subtypes, and ACh hydrolysis—and therefore the concept of ACh tone may still have some functional role in behavior [26].

While the exact middle ground between these two viewpoints is yet to be determined, one study demonstrated both relevant tonic and phasic ACh signaling simultaneously in the PFC and dorsal hippocampus of mice in an attempt to differentiate between their functions. Using electrochemical choline biosensors, Teles-Grilo Ruivo et al. [15] demonstrated that tonic ACh signaling during sleep was highest exclusively during REM sleep that preceded wakefulness. They also demonstrated that tonic signaling was highest as the animal approached a reward in a randomized forced alternation T-maze. Additionally, they found that phasic ACh was associated with the presentation of the reward, with phasic signaling showing response halfwidths significantly shorter than during tonic signaling. Importantly, they demonstrated that both tonic and phasic signaling were highly coordinated between the PFC and the dorsal hippocampus, suggesting that not only does such a distinction in modes of ACh transmission exist in the cortex, but likely can be extended to the hippocampus as well. Should this be the case, the drivers of the distinction are likely to be driven by ubiquitous mechanisms such as cholinesterase activity, or some intrinsic property of basal forebrain anatomy. Ruivo et al. [15] suggest that their results demonstrate that tonic ACh signaling, especially during REM sleep, maybe prepare the mPFC and hippocampus for subsequent alertness and later attentional demands.

Additionally, there is electrophysiological evidence to suggest a dichotomy in cholinergic signaling in the basal forebrain. Unal et al. [27] demonstrated that there are two distinct populations of basal forebrain cholinergic neurons that are distinguished by their electrophysiological properties. Early firing neurons were more excitable, quicker firing, and had more pronounced refractory periods following firing, while later firing neurons were less excitable but more sustained in their ACh release. The authors suggested early firing cells may be involved in phasic signaling and therefore be important for attention, while the late firing cells may be involved in tonic signaling and therefore more important for global arousal states [27]. This suggests that the dichotomy has its roots in electrophysiological correlates.

The above work was later expanded on by Laszlovszky and colleagues [28]. They recorded from cells in mice both in vivo and in vitro and determined that basal forebrain cholinergic neurons took one of two forms, excitable, burst-firing cells (Burst-BFCN) and less excitable, rhythmic cells (Reg-BFCN). They found Burst-BFCN cells to be more numerous than Reg-BFCN in both the NbM and the horizontal limb of the HDB and consisted of two subtypes, ones with regular inter-spike intervals which they refer to as Burst-BFCN-SB and ones with Poisson-like inter-spike intervals which they refer to as Burst-BFCN-PL. They found that Burst-BFCN cells showed cortical synchronicity and fired bursts of action potentials in response to both reward and punishment during an auditory cue detection task. Reg-BFCN cells, on the other hand, were found to have precise spikes after behavioral outcomes, mainly hits, but not false alarms, correct rejections, or misses. Furthermore, there was distinct anatomical heterogeneity amongst these cell types, with Burst-BFCN found in the anterior basal forebrain and Reg-BFCN cells found in the posterior division [28]. These findings offer a unique viewpoint on the current tonic/phasic debate. They contend that such a divide between tonic and phasic signaling does exist and has anatomical and electrophysiological origins. Such an explanation seems to suggest that tonic ACh signaling has a much greater role in cue detection and cognitive operations than would be suggested by Sarter and Lustig [4].

Recently, basal forebrain cholinergic neurons have also been categorized into neurons that express calbindin-D28K (D28K) protein (ChAT D28K+) and those that do not (ChAT D28K-). The expression of D28K across the basal forebrain nuclei ranges significantly. About 40% of ChAT neurons in the VDB co-stain D28K, relative to 30% in the MS, 16% in the HDB, and less than 2% in the NBM [29]. ChAT+ neurons that also stain for D28K have fewer processes and a lower firing frequency. Interestingly, D28K is a Ca2+ binding protein that may function to protect cells from Ca2+-dependent neurodegeneration [29]. This is supported by data that the D28K protein is decreased in cholinergic neurons as a function of aging and in AD [30, 31]. Data support that cholinergic neurons are a heterogeneous population of cells, and understanding the unique profiles of the subpopulations may lead to a better understanding of critical behavioral processes they are involved in.

The location of cholinergic neurons is also a predictor of differential function. Amongst the cholinergic nuclei that project to the cortex, the HDB (rostral BF), and NBM/SI (caudal BF), there is data to support differential function across the anatomical location. Early literature on the NbM to prefrontal cortex circuit suggests a limited degree of axon collateralization between individual projection neurons, which limits the crosstalk between cortical cells and layers [32]. Thus, these circuits seem to be suited for phasic signaling, such that limited collateralization allows for a degree of spatial specificity that is necessary for wired cholinergic transmission in the prefrontal cortex. However, there exists some heterogeneity across species in terms of the projection targets of the NbM, such that its innervation of the prelimbic and infralimbic cortex has been well characterized in mice, but the data seem a bit more nuanced in the case of rats, such that retrograde viral tracing does not show NbM innervation of the prelimbic or infralimbic cortices in this species, rather the HDB is the key region [33]. Likewise, there is data to suggest that the mouse prelimbic cortex receives its primary cholinergic innervation from the HDB as well [11].

Assessing cholinergic activity across the BF anteroposterior axis via calcium imaging and ACh-specific fiber photometry, it has been revealed that the more rostral cholinergic neurons (HDB) are responsive to pupil change, which marks arousal state, reward delivery, and reward omission. In contrast, cholinergic activity in the caudal region (NbM/SI) was more responsive to unconditioned cues, delivery of shock, and cues that predicted shock [34]. It is therefore possible that the projections of the NbM/SI to the cortex may be important for salient cue detection and that the projections from the HDB to the prefrontal cortex are more critical for appetitive arousal states.

Cortical receptor location within the architectural layers of the cortex may also play a key role in the outcome of ACh signaling. In addition to the aforementioned factors, evidence for tonic and phasic ACh as distinct behaviorally relevant modes of neurotransmission come from the differential roles and actions of muscarinic and nicotinic ACh receptors within the cortex [31]. There is some evidence to suggest that fast nicotinic ACh receptor (nAChR) mediated signaling is essential for phasic ACh signaling in the cortex while muscarinic ACh receptors (mAChR) are involved in slow changes in ACh concentration such as during tonic signaling. However, such a distinction may not be so cut and dry, as there seems to be a degree of muscarinic activity needed to perform a cue detection task [35], which suggests that muscarinic receptor activity may be needed to regulate global arousal states and “prime” the prefrontal cortex for phasic cholinergic signaling.

Advertisement

3. The contribution of acetylcholine receptors in modulating tonic and phasic activity

3.1 Nicotinic receptors

Nicotinic ACh receptors (nAChR) in the cortex have been shown to be essential for a number of cognitive functions, such a top-down attentional control of behavior and general working memory [36, 37]. Given their fast-acting, ionotropic responses, nAChRs are thought to be involved in phasic ACh signaling. Furthermore, given their selective laminar expression throughout different regions of the cortex, it stands to reason that nAChRs would be selectively expressed in circuits where fast, wired ACh transmission is the norm, and in such a distribution that promotes spatially restrained and functionally heterogeneous responses to ACh signaling from the basal forebrain. While there is evidence to suggest a role for the utilization of these receptors in phasic signaling, the distinction between the role of nAChR in the two modes of cholinergic neurotransmission seems to be much more nuanced.

Much of the evidence surrounding the role of nAChRs in fast synaptic ACh signaling is electrophysiological. It has been shown that the activation of nAChRs evokes short-latency depolarizing postsynaptic potentials in mouse neocortical pyramidal neurons [38], which has been suggested to function as laminar selectivity relative to muscarinic function. Furthermore, nAChR activation throughout the cortex seems to be layer specific, as Poorthuis et al. [39] showed that nAChR activation leads to inhibition of pyramidal cells in the mPFC in layers II and III, but enhanced excitability of layers V and VI. Interestingly, their data suggest that the response to ACh in this region is dependent on different subunits depending on the layer of the cortex. They demonstrated that pyramidal neurons in layer VI exhibit slow inward currents in the presence of ACh that are absent in β2 subunit deficient mice and were blocked by an antagonist for β2 containing nAChRs and were only occasionally accompanied by α7 mediated currents. Meanwhile, layer V mPFC pyramidal cells showed an attenuated response to ACh in α7 deficient mice as well as following the application of an α7 antagonist, but not in β2 deficient mice, suggesting differential expression of these subunits by layer. They also performed two-photon imaging in cortical pyramidal cells of each layer in β2 and α7 deficient mice and found that nAChR-induced neuronal activation is dependent on the β2 subunit when ACh changes in concentration slowly, suggesting that not only is the β2 subunit utilized during tonic signaling, but also potentially reveals an interesting anatomical phenomena in which layer VI, in which β2 containing nAChRs are most involved in, is involved in tonic signaling, while layer V may not be [39].

The idea that nicotinic receptors are involved in tonic ACh signaling is an interesting one as well, in that it would stand as evidence against the assumption nAChRs mediate phasic signaling exclusively, and that tonic signaling is mostly reliant on muscarinic receptors. In addition to its presumed role in the detection of ambient ACh, β2 subunit deletion has been shown to impair attentional performance in mice [40], suggesting a role of such receptor subtypes in phasic ACh signaling, or at least furthering the idea that attentional performance requires some changes in ambient ACh levels. Interestingly enough, the deletion of this subunit can also upregulate muscarinic receptor excitability to compensate in layer VI of the mPFC, which was also seen following the deletion of the α5 subunit as well [41]. The previously mentioned study by Poorthuis and colleagues [39] notably blocked muscarinic receptors to isolate neuronal responses due to nicotinic activation, meaning that such an increase in excitation was masked, and cholinergic tone that would have been detected primarily by muscarinic receptors may have instead bound to nicotinic receptors in layer VI. An alternative explanation is that such a mechanism may allow for further priming of the cortex to ACh signaling to make up for the reduction caused by subunit deletion, allowing for phasic-dependent cognitive processes to attempt to bounce back even in the presence of less cholinergic signaling.

In addition to the β2 subunit, it seems the α5 nAChR subunit is necessary for attention as well, as deletion of the Chrna5 gene, which encodes for the α5 subunit, impairs attention [42]. It was also demonstrated that the loss of Chrna5 delays cholinergic excitation, such that mPFC layer VI pyramidal cells from Chrna5 deficient mice show attenuated onset kinetics but unaffected response magnitude during optogenetic stimulation of cholinergic afferents in vitro and that enhancing nicotinic receptor affinity pharmacologically can actually restore the typical response to optogenetic stimulation [8]. The authors suggest that α5 subunit containing nAChRs in the PFC may be necessary to define a critical window for cue detection, such that a delay to their onset results in the animal being unable to properly integrate detected stimuli via corticothalamic connections and ends up missing the cue altogether. Another interesting factor that may play into the α5 subunit’s role in phasic ACh signaling in cue detection is its relative rarity, in that its expression is much less prevalent than subunits such as β2 [43], and therefore may be more likely to facilitate spatially constrained wired transmission. However, this is yet to be investigated and represents a considerable gap in our understanding of the role of the α5 subunit in attention.

One further subtype that has been implicated in attention control, and by extension, the role of its dysfunction in cognitive decline, is the α7 nAChR which is composed entirely of α7 subunits. This particular subunit has been highly implicated in the cognitive decline associated with AD [44, 45, 46]. It has been shown that an α7 nAChR agonist was sufficient to enhance learning speed but not filter distracting information, which was instead enhanced by an α4/ β2 agonist, with no effect on learning speed [47]. Additionally, it has been shown that performance on the 5-choice continuous performance task, a measure of attention in rodents, was improved following the administration of encenicline, a partial agonist to the α7 nAChR, in poor-performing rats [48]. Due to the fact the dysfunction of α7 has been demonstrated in Alzheimer’s disease, it is likely that this particular subtype of receptor is necessary for attentional processes in the cortex, in addition to other processes.

3.2 Muscarinic receptors

Muscarinic ACh receptors (mAChR) are thought to be mediators of tonic acetylcholine signaling, due to their longer duration of action via G-protein coupled mechanisms that require second messenger signaling. mAChRs are present in the prefrontal cortex both presynaptically and postsynaptically [49]. Thus, they would seem to be much more suited as receptors of tonic signaling, and likely their longer-lasting duration of action and the fact that a single muscarinic receptor can have amplified second messenger signaling lends itself to the idea that muscarinic receptors are involved in relatively slow timescale changes in ACh concentration in the prefrontal cortex. Similarly, such a mechanism would be suitable for the detection of relatively small concentrations of ACh present in the extracellular fluid during tonic transmission, as opposed to the much larger concentration present at the cholinergic synapse during phasic signaling [26].

However, there may be a degree of specificity as to which muscarinic receptor subtypes are involved in tonic signaling. It has been shown that the Gq-coupled M1 mAChR, but not the M3 or M5, is essential to the response of pyramidal neurons to tonic ACh [35], suggesting a role for only specific Gq-coupled receptors, not all of them. This is interesting, as the location of M1 receptors on the dendritic shaft and spines of cortical pyramidal cells provides a degree of anatomical evidence for volume transmission in the cortex, likely meaning that the M1 receptor’s anatomical distribution is what favors it toward the detection of ambient ACh over long distances due to the fact that cholinergic neurons are not known to make axo-axonic synapses [50]. These findings point to a specialized role of muscarinic receptors in detecting global, seconds-scale changes in ACh concentration indicative of perisynaptic signaling and tonic transmission.

As mentioned previously, muscarinic receptor activation may serve to “prime” the cortex for subsequent phasic signaling in such a way that both forms of signaling are necessary for attentional processes to occur. A likely mechanism is via increases in cortical pyramidal cell excitability, in which muscarinic receptor activation has been shown to alter via the induction of LTD in these cells in the medial prefrontal cortex [51]. This is further supported by findings that mice that are deficient in the inhibitory M2 muscarinic receptor show enhanced attentional performance, despite having impaired object-location learning and spontaneous recognition memory [52], suggesting that while the Gq-coupled muscarinic receptors, such as M1, play an important role in cortical excitability, the activation of Gi-coupled receptors such as M2 may be detrimental to attentional performance. It seems likely that muscarinic receptor activation is needed for cue detection and sustained attentional processes, as a non-specific muscarinic receptor antagonist is sufficient to impair attentional performance on a two-choice visual discrimination task in mice [53]. Likewise, M1 receptor antagonism impairs performance on a divided attention task in rats, unlike antagonism of the M4 receptor which did not [54]. Additionally, the M1-positive allosteric modulator TAK-071 has been shown to rescue attentional performance in rats with basal forebrain cholinergic cell loss [55]. Such studies suggest that while muscarinic receptors may play a role in attentional processes, their role in phasic ACh signaling remains unclear.

While it seems that muscarinic receptors are needed for attentional processes due to their effects on global arousal states, the dichotomy of tonic signaling being dependent solely on muscarinic receptors while phasic signaling is dependent solely on nicotinic receptors is evidently an oversimplification. Likely, the mechanism by which attentional control occurs is reliant on both nAChRs and mAChRs, such that mAChRs are needed to orient the organism toward the object of their attention, while nAChRs, at least the α7 and α5 subtypes, are involved in the response of cortical neurons to short timescale phasic ACh release. Thus, both nAChRs and mAChRs seem necessary for phasic ACh signaling and its reliant cognitive operations such as cue detection. However, mAChRs on their own may be mediators of tonic ACh signaling, and thus a degree of tonic ACh efflux is needed to ready the prefrontal cortex for an orientation toward a specific stimulus. However, more research is needed to delineate the contributions of each receptor system to tonic and phasic signaling in the prefrontal cortex, though the way to do so does not seem immediately straightforward.

Advertisement

4. The role of acetylcholinesterase in tonic and phasic ACh signaling

One of the ways in which tonic and phasic ACh signaling are regulated is through the actions of AChE, the catalytic enzyme which hydrolyzes ACh and is thought to be the main determinant of the duration of cholinergic signaling. As stated before, AChE can hydrolyze ACh nearly as fast as the cell can release it, and thus the high catalytic activity of this enzyme is evidence itself of the presence of phasic ACh signaling in the prefrontal cortex. However, it has also been hypothesized that this is one of the reasons as to why tonic ACh signaling is unlikely to directly contribute to behavior at all. Sarter and Lustig [4] state that the fact that AChE is such an efficient hydrolyzer of ACh makes it unlikely for synaptic spillover to occur, and therefore tonic ACh as measured by in vivo microdialysis may be a methodological artifact due to the cholinesterase inhibitors perfused into the brain during the process. However, it has been shown that inhibition of AChE by an excess of ACh may occur via the formation of a ternary complex [25], which represents one mechanism by which high-ACh concentration can overwhelm AChE and spill over into the extracellular fluid. Therefore, a central role of potentially heterogeneous utilization of AChE during tonic and phasic ACh signaling distinction must be considered.

In the brain, AChE is exclusively expressed as the tailed AChET variant. This subunit is able to form an amphiphilic tetramer known as G4 AChE, which is facilitated by and tethered to proline-rich membrane anchor (PRiMA), anchoring AChE to the presynaptic membrane [56, 57]. Acetylcholinesterase is co-expressed with markers of cholinergic neurons such as choline acetyltransferase (ChAT) in vivo [57], suggesting that AChE is likely expressed in cholinergic cells before undergoing tetramerization via PRiMA in the endoplasmic reticulum and being transported down the axon [58]. PRiMA likely links tailed G4 AChE to the presynaptic membrane via membrane rafts [59]. Interestingly, PRiMA shows robust co-expression with M1 muscarinic receptors, which are located postsynaptically, suggesting that AChE may originate primarily from cholinergic axons but also intrinsically from neurons in the cortex [60]. Additionally, this co-expression may have interesting functional implications, however, it may simply be due to the ubiquity of expression of the M1 receptor in basal forebrain target regions. To date, no studies have looked to colocalize PRiMA with nicotinic receptors, making any potential differences in PRiMA receptors between neurons expressing the two receptor types is unknown but represents a future area of inquiry.

Experimental manipulations to inhibit the endogenous action of AChE have been shown to cause attentional impairments, such as during a five-choice serial reaction time task in healthy rats [61], suggesting that inhibition of AChE in healthy subjects impairs behaviors likely dependent on phasic signaling. AChE knockout mice have been shown to exhibit a variety of motor deficits due to the role of peripheral AChE in muscle contraction and thermoregulation [62]. Farar et al. [63] characterized PRiMA KO mice on a number of motor and behavioral measures. They found that despite only very subtle motor impairments on the rotarod test and the wire task, these mice had a nearly 200–300-fold increase in extracellular ACh concentration in the striatum as measured via in vivo microdialysis during anesthesia, with none of the thermoregulatory impairments seen in mice with a traditional AChE knockout. Additionally, these mice were not impaired on the Morris water maze. Interestingly, they found that the M1 muscarinic receptor was heavily downregulated across all areas of the brain measured, including by approximately 40% in the cortex, with no such decrease in the α7 nAChR or β2-containing nAChRs. One interpretation of these results laid out by the authors is that these data serve as evidence for the hypothesis that AChE is primarily involved in regulating the extracellular ACh concentration, not terminating synaptic transmission [63].

Should that be the case, then it is possible that PRiMA knockout mice may have deficits in phasic ACh signaling, but not tonic signaling. Thus, the hypothesis may be the inverse of what was proposed by these authors, that AChE is primarily utilized at the cholinergic synapses of the prefrontal cortex to rapidly terminate ACh signaling, while ACh “tone” represents ACh that escapes this mechanism and is, therefore, less active during this form of signaling. This may represent a potential mechanism by which attentional and other cognitive impairments occur in disease states that alter AChE and disrupt the balance between ACh release and hydrolysis, such as Alzheimer’s disease, while leaving other functions that are dependent on tonic signaling unimpaired until later in the disease.

A likely mechanism for which AChE is inhibited at the synapse, as represented in Figure 1 is as such: an overabundance of ACh in the synaptic cleft during phasic signaling may lead to inhibition of AChE which allows acetylcholine to spill out of the synapse and into the extracellular fluid. This ambient ACh would still be under the regulation of AChE, but the enzyme’s location on the synaptic membrane would make it more difficult for extracellular acetylcholine to be hydrolyzed, allowing it to accumulate in the extracellular space. Therefore, AChE still regulates ACh tone, albeit mostly indirectly, through its regulation of phasic ACh signaling. Tonic ACh may also be released by non-synaptic terminals, perhaps by specialized basal forebrain cholinergic cells [27, 28], and it is the combination of these two mechanisms that are responsible for ambient ACh fluctuations.

Figure 1.

Proposed mechanism of the regulation of phasic and tonic acetylcholine (ACh) signaling in the prefrontal cortex. A. Phasic acetylcholine signaling by the terminals of early firing basal forebrain neurons at cholinergic synapses leads to the activation of both postsynaptic nicotinic and muscarinic receptors. The termination of cholinergic signaling is mediated by presynaptic membrane-bound acetylcholinesterase (AChE), which hydrolyzes ACh. B. High levels of phasic firing, such as during high attentional demand, lead to an abundance of ACh at the synapse, inhibiting AChE activity and further increasing the concentration of ACh. C. Synaptic spillover of ACh into the extracellular fluid occurs due to transient inhibition of AChE. D. There is also the release of ACh from the terminals of late-firing basal forebrain cholinergic neurons, which further increases ACh tone. Extracellular ACh is still hydrolyzed by membrane-bound AChE. E. Extracellular ACh travels to distal muscarinic and nicotinic receptors within the prefrontal cortex, modulating global arousal states. *created with BioRender.com

Advertisement

5. ACh in brain disorders: circuit dysfunction

Reductions in markers for the cholinergic phenotype, such as the expression of ChAT, the enzyme necessary for ACh synthesis, are present throughout the basal forebrain following both AD and AUD, a consequence that is considered one of the hallmarks of both of these disorders in particular. These cholinergic deficits are present in two of the main basal forebrain circuits, the projections from the NbM complex (NbM, HDB, SI) to the cortical mantle, and the projections from the MS/DB to the hippocampus. In both disorders, there is a suppression in the basal forebrain cholinergic phenotype. In the case of AUD, there is evidence from animal models to suggest that these deficits may not be permanent and can be rescued via the use of voluntary wheel running exercise [64], the actions of neurotrophins [65], and the AChE inhibitor galantamine [66]. In contrast, chronic treadmill exercise or voluntary wheel running has been shown to attenuate age-related reduction of cholinergic fibers in the cortex and hippocampus and improve some learning and memory outcomes but has minor effects on the number of ChAT [47] positive neurons [67, 68, 69]. However, exercise has been shown to improve ACh levels in the hippocampus in an Aβ1–42 peptide rat model [70]. This does suggest that deficits caused by both alcohol-related brain damage and AD may involve a reduction in functional cholinergic neurons, leading to reductions in overall ACh signaling, which can be rescued. It, therefore, seems likely that in both of these disorders there would be a disruption in either, or both, tonic and phasic ACh signaling in the brain.

The drivers of the selective neuropathological vulnerability of cholinergic neurons, across brain disorders, are their large size and extensive projections, which require high metabolic expenditures and trophic factors to maintain the considerable cytoskeletal surface, as well as the machinery for axonal transport over long distances. These morphological properties of cholinergic neurons increase their vulnerability to oxidative stress, neuroinflammation, and altered energy homeostasis that occurs during aging and disease states [71, 72].

Human clinical data have long supported the role of chronic heavy alcohol use leading to premature brain aging, as well as a risk for the development of dementia, including AD [73, 74, 75, 76, 77]. Furthermore, alcohol consumption has been linked to an increased risk of dementia in individuals with a genetic predisposition to AD [78, 79]. Recent data from preclinical studies demonstrate that the consequences of adult or developmental EtOH exposure resemble advanced brain aging or produces accelerated AD-related pathology in transgenic models with AD-related transgenes [80, 81, 82, 83]. Advanced aging, AD, and AUD have some overlapping neuropathological sequelae: upregulation of proinflammatory markers, suppressed hippocampal neurogenesis, suppression of basal forebrain cholinergic phenotype, and altered pro- and mature NT levels—as well as a change in the ratio of Trk to p75NTRs [84, 85, 86, 87, 88]. A common pathway for cholinergic dysfunction in AD and AUD is the disruption of neurotrophins and their receptors (see Figure 2), which may drive additive effects of AD and AUD pathology.

Figure 2.

Changes in neurotrophins and their receptors that modulate cholinergic cell survival. Both Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and alcohol use disorder (AUD) alter the expression and ratio of pro-to-mature nerve growth factor (NGF) and brain-derived growth factor (BDNF), as well as the tropomyosin kinase receptors for NGF (TrkA), BDNF (TrkB), and p75 receptor. The pro-forms of neurotrophins are potent ligands for p75NTR and can induce cell death or loss of cholinergic phenotype when coupled with sortilin. Thus, AUD likely accelerates the pathological sequela of AD. *Created with BioRender.com

5.1 Alzheimer’s disease and cholinergic dysfunction

The increase in AChE activity in AD has been known for some time, as many of the drugs currently available for the treatment of this disease target this enzyme and inhibit its activity [89, 90, 91]. Cholinesterase inhibition has been shown to increase cognitive performance on the Stroop task in human patients with AD, with the degree of inhibition directly correlating with performance [92]. In AD, AChE inhibitors prolong ACh action, as well as increase the uptake of NGF to improve cholinergic neuronal survival [93]. Furthermore, cholinesterase inhibitor therapy in AD improves cognitive performance by increasing the activation of frontal cortical circuits as determined by fMRI studies [94, 95]. However, only a subset of patients with AD is effectively responsive to AChE inhibitors, and cholinergic basal forebrain integrity is a key predictor of treatment success [96].

As mentioned previously, it is likely that AChE is needed for both tonic and phasic ACh signaling, but is a more immediate causal effector of phasic signaling. It is likely that tonic signaling is required to ready the cortex for phasic signaling, and AChE inhibition may also work to increase ACh tone and facilitate cognitive performance. However, there may be a degree of specificity for phasic signaling when it comes to this modulation. Should the hypothesis that phasic signaling is more immediately dependent on AChE than tonic signaling be supported, then modulation of AChE should have more of an effect on phasic-dependent processes. However, since tonic signaling is likely needed to modulate phasic signaling and vice versa, disentangling these two may be difficult experimentally, since the effects that AChE inhibition may have on tonic signaling may occur after that of phasic signaling, but still on a relatively short pharmacological timescale.

Likely, a balance between ACh and AChE may be critical to the phasic ACh signaling needed for sustained attention. Thus, disorders such as AD disrupt attentional processes by dysregulation of AChE, leading to dysfunction of phasic ACh signaling in the cortex. It has been shown that presenilin-1 (PS1), the catalytic component of the γ secretase complex and therefore the formation of amyloid β (Aβ) [97, 98] affects the processing of PRiMA, as was shown by the use of using hamster ovarian cells transfected with AChET and a PS1 conditional knockout mouse. They showed that γ secretase inhibition led to an increase in both PRiMA and AChET and that G4 AChE was increased in the membrane rafts in the PFC of PS1 KO mice, representing a potential mechanism by which AD pathology imparts alterations to AChE function [99]. Furthermore, PS1 transgenic mice show increased AChE activity throughout the entirety of the basal forebrain, prior to any behavioral deficits, suggesting that AChE dysfunction may be one of the first steps in the cascade of pathology associated with AD [100]. This increase in AChE activity is typically the therapeutic target of cholinesterase-inhibiting drugs such as donepezil, one of the most widely used drugs for the treatment of AD.

Romberg et al. [101] tested 3xTgAD mice, a strain with APPswe, PS1M146V, and tauP301L mutations to recapitulate some of the major features of AD in humans, on a 5-choice serial reaction time task (5-CSRTT) and found that while the transgenic mice were able to match wild-type controls in performance early on in the task, they would later begin to show impairments across the duration of the task, specifically as stimulus duration was reduced, signifying deficits in sustaining attention. These deficits were rescued via the administration of donepezil, demonstrating that inhibiting the alterations to typical AChE activity was sufficient to restore similar functioning to wild-type mice, suggesting that this particular task is reliant on AChE to modulate cortical cholinergic activity [101]. It is likely that AD, through its upregulation of AChE activity, perturbs the frontocortical circuitry necessary for attention by disrupting phasic ACh signaling. Similarly, donepezil was able to reduce scopolamine-induced omissions during the 5-CSRTT, demonstrating that inhibiting AChE can compensate for muscarinic receptor antagonism during this task [102].

Nicotinic receptors have been heavily implicated in the pathogenesis of AD, which likely disrupts nicotinic-dependent signaling in the cortex, and as discussed above has been linked to phasic signaling. This dysfunction likely originates with the SK family of Ca2+ sensitive K+ channels, as treatment with a selective agonist of these channels improves the nAChR function [103]. Dysfunction with such circuitry is evident by the fact that impairments in sustained attention have been shown using a mouse model of AD [104], demonstrating likely deficits with phasic cholinergic deficits, which, as mentioned before, is at least partially dependent on the utilization of nAChRs in the prefrontal cortex [42, 48]. Additionally, there is some evidence to suggest a link between Aβ pathology and nicotinic dysfunction, as it has been shown that Aβ prevents nicotine-induced inhibitory signaling, but not excitatory signaling, in PFC pyramidal neurons in vitro [105], suggesting that Alzheimer’s disease pathology may be disrupting the balance of excitation and inhibition necessary for phasic ACh signaling in the PFC. The link between Aβ and nAChR function is further supported by data showing that infusion of Aβ increased α-bungarotoxin autoradiography binding to the α7 nAChR in the frontal cortex exclusively in animals that received weekly attentional stimulation, suggesting that α7 nAChR functionality may be impaired by Aβ, but regular activation of attentional circuitry can activate compensational mechanisms in both the cortex to attempt to restore regular functioning [106].

Similarly, within the cortex itself, there are deficits to muscarinic receptors as well, as M1 mAChR’s are being considered as a potential target for treatment [107]. However, work using radioligand labeling of human participants with AD has demonstrated no changes in M1 labeling in the cortex in AD, only in the dentate gyrus [108]. It is possible that despite no changes in M1 expression in the cortex, there are still deficits to its typical function, as postmortem analysis of the brains of individuals with AD showed reduced G-protein coupling of the M1 receptor in the cortex, despite no change in its density [109], mirroring the radioligand results from Scarr et al. [108]. It is possible that impaired function of the M1 receptor in AD serves as an indicator of dysfunctional tonic ACh signaling in the cortex, but as mentioned previously, the relationship between muscarinic receptors and tonic signaling is not one-to-one, and there is likely some contribution of nicotinic receptors to tonic signaling dependent behaviors as well.

5.2 Alcohol use disorder and cholinergic dysfunction

The cholinergic pathology in AUD is similar to that of AD, with some overlap in the effects of binge ethanol exposure during adolescence and age-related cognitive decline [110]. It is therefore likely that alcohol-related damage to the basal forebrain leads to dysfunction of tonic and phasic ACh signaling as well. Acute ethanol exposure in rats has been shown to lead to deficits in sustained attention, demonstrating that phasic ACh signaling in the cortex is likely dysregulated during intoxication [111], though the impairments due to acute ethanol likely have to do with the depressive effect of ethanol throughout the entirety of the brain. However, exposure to binge levels of ethanol in adolescence has been shown to lead to deficits that persist across the lifespan [112, 113, 114]. Adolescent intermittent ethanol (AIE) exposure has been shown to lead to reductions in ChAT immunostaining in the NbM in adulthood, an effect exclusive to rats that are exposed to ethanol in adolescence, but not adulthood [115]. This loss of ChAT has been shown to be rescued by galantamine, an AChE inhibitor [66]. Likely, this type of alcohol exposure leads to an upregulation of cholinesterase activity, leading to overactive hydrolysis of ACh that is making it difficult for precisely time-locked phasic ACh signaling to occur, similar to AD.

However, there is some evidence to suggest that overactive AChE is leading to some of these deficits in ways beyond its disruption of phasic ACh signaling. It has been shown that overactive AChE induces apoptosis in both living mice and cell cultures exposed to ethanol [116], suggesting that it is possible that ethanol may overstimulate AChE activity and lead to an apoptotic cascade. However, it is unclear how this relates to the rescue of alcohol-related deficits to cholinergic phenotype, as it has been shown that ChAT cells in the basal forebrain are not dead following exposure to binge levels of ethanol in adolescence, but rather are entering a quiescent state that can be rescued either via neurotrophins [65], cholinesterase inhibiting drugs [66], or voluntary wheel running exercise [64]. The relation between the quiescent state these cells take, and the apoptotic mechanism described is yet to be ascertained.

Similarly, AIE has been shown to have effects on tonic ACh signaling in the prefrontal cortex. Adolescent alcohol exposure has been shown to attenuate behaviorally relevant acetylcholine efflux in the PFC during a spontaneous alternation task, which was accompanied by reductions in ChAT in the NbM and the medial septum/diagonal band (MS/DB), suggesting that AIE disrupts innervation of the PFC by the basal forebrain, leading to a reduction in cholinergic tone [117]. Likely, ACh tone is needed during this task to induce a state of general arousal in which the animal is actively attenuating to extra-maze cues to determine the arms of the maze it has visited already and to avoid visiting the same arms consecutively [13]. Fernandez and Savage [112] also demonstrated parallel behavioral impairments, as rats exposed to AIE showed deficits on operant attention set-shifting task, a task that has been shown to be dependent on the mPFC [118]. The exact role of phasic ACh signaling during this task is yet to be investigated, but it is possible that the detection of a visual cue to indicate which of the two levers indicates reward is dependent on a similar mechanism to cue detection in the sustained attention task, and the fact that AIE rats are impaired on the shift from spatial side reinforcement to a visual cue determining reinforcement suggests that these two tasks may be dependent on similar mechanisms. However, more work is needed to determine whether phasic acetylcholine signaling is required for attention.

Other models of alcohol-related brain damage have shown similar effects on the cholinergic system and the prefrontal cortex. For example, adult rats either fed a pyrithiamine deficient diet (PTD), given access to ethanol in their drinking bottles (CET), or a combination of both (PTD-CET) were shown to have impaired spontaneous alternation behavior and reduced ACh efflux in the mPFC during this task, which was accompanied by a reduced latency to lever press during a set shift during operant attention set shifting despite no impairment in performance [119]. This suggests that there are tonic ACh signaling deficits in this model, as demonstrated by the decreased ACh efflux, as well as possible phasic signaling deficits, as the increased amount of time needed to make a lever press in PTD, CET, and PTD-CET animals suggests that the time course of ACh signaling in PFC is being disrupted. The deficits seen during spontaneous alternation during PTD have been shown to be rescued by the AChE inhibitor tacrine [120], suggesting a role of AChE overexpression in the pathology seen in this disorder, suggesting that overactive AChE, mirroring what is seen in AD, is responsible for the tonic ACh deficits. Presumably, these deficits would extend to phasic ACh-dependent processes, but they were not tested here. The fact that cholinesterase inhibition has such an effect on tonic signaling is interesting, but not surprising, as while the mechanism proposed within this review suggests that AChE is more important for phasic signaling, it is nevertheless required to regulate ACh tone as well, exerting its actions both directly by hydrolyzing extracellular ACh and indirectly by regulating phasic signaling and synaptic spillover.

Alcohol-related brain damage and AD seem to converge on the cholinergic system, and the projections from the basal forebrain to the prefrontal cortex seem to be a set of circuits that show particular vulnerability to perturbations. Dysfunction of these circuits likely has effects on both tonic and phasic ACh signaling simultaneously, and it seems that, at the moment, it would be conceptually difficult to investigate an experimental manipulation that would affect one type of signaling but not the other. However, it may be possible to determine whether the time course of the impairments seen in these two types of signaling differ, such that perhaps phasic signaling is first affected by the early cholinergic deficits seen in AD, which later expands to tonic signaling deficits later on. This remains to be determined and represents a future direction for research into modes of ACh signaling.

Advertisement

6. Conclusions and future directions

As the distinction between the roles of tonic and phasic ACh signaling becomes clearer, it is important to understand the specific dysfunction that is occurring in both of these types of signaling in the cortex in disorders such as AD and AUD. It is likely that these two disorders have effects on both muscarinic and nicotinic ACh receptors and that these deficits are part of what is driving the dysregulation of the two types of ACh signaling in the cortex. Additionally, the role of AChE in this distinction cannot be overstated, as its role in hydrolyzing synaptic ACh may be one of the most important regulators of phasic ACh, with its inhibition by high ACh concentrations possibly leading to spillover into the extracellular space and therefore augmenting ACh tone that is usually otherwise due to a different set of basal forebrain cholinergic cells.

The availability of fluorescent ACh sensors for fiber photometry allows for the measurement of ACh activity in vivo on a second timescale. Studies in which changes to ACh signaling following alterations to normal AChE activity can be assessed and are needed to determine the exact role of this enzyme in cortical cholinergic synapses, and how its dysfunction relates to some of the deficits seen in common cholinergic disorders. Likewise, the contribution of alterations to typical nAChR and mAChR function in the PFC to phasic ACh signaling could be investigated using photometric methods. Studies looking directly into how tonic and phasic ACh utilize different cellular machinery seem to be elusive for now, but further breakthroughs in the field could lead to new avenues to address this gap in the literature.

Furthermore, reexamining AD and alcohol-related brain damage through the lens of the tonic phasic distinction may allow researchers to hone in on the exact mechanisms by which cholinergic dysfunction occurs in these diseases and therefore develop new treatments. A reconceptualization of these disorders in terms of the mounting evidence that ACh in the prefrontal cortex is phasic in regard to many of the cognitive symptoms seen in AD may lead us to a better understanding of how the current pharmacological treatments for these diseases work and how to improve them. Similarly, updating our understanding of alcohol-related brain damage using this new conceptualization of ACh signaling in the prefrontal cortex may lead the field into novel treatments and ways to prevent or even reverse cholinergic deficits that arise via exposure to heavy amounts of alcohol. Such advancements may be possible with a reevaluation of the way that ACh signaling in the cortex contributes to behavior.

References

  1. 1. Jones BE. Arousal and sleep circuits. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2020;45(1):6-20. DOI: 10.1038/s41386-019-0444-2
  2. 2. Poulet JFA, Crochet S. The cortical states of wakefulness. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience. 2018;12:64. DOI: 10.3389/fnsys.2018.00064
  3. 3. Perry E, Walker M, Grace J, Perry R. Acetylcholine in mind: A neurotransmitter correlate of consciousness? Trends in Neurosciences. 1999;22(6):273-280. DOI: 10.1016/s0166-2236(98)01361-7
  4. 4. Sarter M, Lustig C. Forebrain cholinergic signaling: Wired and phasic, not tonic, and causing behavior. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2020;40(4):712-719. DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1305-19.2019
  5. 5. Parikh V, Bangasser DA. Cholinergic Signaling dynamics and cognitive control of attention. Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences. 2020;45:71-87. DOI: 10.1007/7854_2020_133
  6. 6. Sarter M, Lustig C, Berry AS, Gritton H, Howe WM, Parikh V. What do phasic cholinergic signals do? Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2016;130:135-141. DOI: 10.1016/j.nlm.2016.02.008
  7. 7. Bruno JP, Gash C, Martin B, Zmarowski A, Pomerleau F, Burmeister J, et al. Second-by-second measurement of acetylcholine release in prefrontal cortex. The European Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;24(10):2749-2757. DOI: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.05176.x
  8. 8. Venkatesan S, Lambe EK. Chrna5 is essential for a rapid and protected response to Optogenetic release of endogenous acetylcholine in prefrontal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2020;40(38):7255-7268. DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1128-20.2020
  9. 9. Gritton HJ, Howe WM, Mallory CS, Hetrick VL, Berke JD, Sarter M. Cortical cholinergic signaling controls the detection of cues. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2016;113(8):E1089-E1097. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1516134113
  10. 10. Jing M, Li Y, Zeng J, Huang P, Skirzewski M, Kljakic O. et al. An optimized acetylcholine sensor for monitoring in vivo cholinergic activity. Nature Methods. 2020;17(11): 1139-1146. DOI: 10.1038/ s41592-020-0953-2
  11. 11. Tu G, Halawa A, Yu X, Gillman S, Takehara-Nishiuchi K. Outcome-locked cholinergic Signaling suppresses prefrontal encoding of stimulus associations. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2022;42(20):4202-4214. DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1969-21.2022
  12. 12. Giovannini MG, Bartolini L, Kopf SR, Pepeu G. Acetylcholine release from the frontal cortex during exploratory activity. Brain Research. 1998;784(1-2):218-227. DOI: 10.1016/s0006-8993(97)01161-x
  13. 13. Lalonde R. The neurobiological basis of spontaneous alternation. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2002;26(1):91-104. DOI: 10.1016/s0149-7634(01)00041-0
  14. 14. Sarter M, Bruno JP. Cortical cholinergic inputs mediating arousal, attentional processing and dreaming: Differential afferent regulation of the basal forebrain by telencephalic and brainstem afferents. Neuroscience. 2000;95(4):933-952. DOI: 10.1016/s0306-4522(99)00487-x
  15. 15. Teles-Grilo Ruivo LM, Baker KL, Conway MW, Kinsley PJ, Gilmour G, Phillips KG, et al. Coordinated acetylcholine release in prefrontal cortex and Hippocampus is associated with arousal and reward on distinct timescales. Cell Reports. 2017;18(4):905-917. DOI: 10.1016/j.celrep.2016.12.085
  16. 16. Himmelheber AM, Sarter M, Bruno JP. Operant performance and cortical acetylcholine release: Role of response rate, reward density, and non-contingent stimuli. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research. 1997;6(1):23-36. DOI: 10.1016/s0926-6410(97)00014-1
  17. 17. Himmelheber AM, Sarter M, Bruno JP. Increases in cortical acetylcholine release during sustained attention performance in rats. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research. 2000;9(3):313-325. DOI: 10.1016/s0926-6410(00)00012-4
  18. 18. Inglis FM, Fibiger HC. Increases in hippocampal and frontal cortical acetylcholine release associated with presentation of sensory stimuli. Neuroscience. 1995;66(1):81-86. DOI: 10.1016/0306-4522(94)00578-s
  19. 19. Passetti F, Dalley JW, O'Connell MT, Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. Increased acetylcholine release in the rat medial prefrontal cortex during performance of a visual attentional task. The European Journal of Neuroscience. 2000;12(8):3051-3058. DOI: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2000.00183.x
  20. 20. Acquas E, Wilson C, Fibiger HC. Conditioned and unconditioned stimuli increase frontal cortical and hippocampal acetylcholine release: Effects of novelty, habituation, and fear. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1996;16(9):3089-3096 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8622138
  21. 21. Dalley JW, McGaughy J, O'Connell MT, Cardinal RN, Levita L, Robbins TW. Distinct changes in cortical acetylcholine and noradrenaline efflux during contingent and noncontingent performance of a visual attentional task. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2001;21(13):4908-4914 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11425918
  22. 22. Sarter M, Lustig C. Cholinergic double duty: Cue detection and attentional control. Current Opinion in Psychology. 2019;29:102-107. DOI: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.12.026
  23. 23. Parikh V, Kozak R, Martinez V, Sarter M. Prefrontal acetylcholine release controls cue detection on multiple timescales. Neuron. 2007;56(1):141-154. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.08.025
  24. 24. Massoulie J, Pezzementi L, Bon S, Krejci E, Vallette FM. Molecular and cellular biology of cholinesterases. Progress in Neurobiology. 1993;41(1):31-91. DOI: 10.1016/0301-0082(93)90040-y
  25. 25. Hofer P, Fringeli UP. Acetylcholinesterase kinetics. Biophysical Structure Mechanism. 1981;8(1-2):45-59. DOI: 10.1007/BF01047105
  26. 26. Disney AA, Higley MJ. Diverse spatiotemporal scales of cholinergic signaling in the neocortex. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2020;40(4):720-725. DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1306-19.2019
  27. 27. Unal CT, Golowasch JP, Zaborszky L. Adult mouse basal forebrain harbors two distinct cholinergic populations defined by their electrophysiology. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2012;6:21. DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2012.00021
  28. 28. Laszlovszky T, Schlingloff D, Hegedus P, Freund TF, Gulyas A, Kepecs A, et al. Distinct synchronization, cortical coupling and behavioral function of two basal forebrain cholinergic neuron types. Nature Neuroscience. 2020;23(8):992-1003. DOI: 10.1038/s41593-020-0648-0
  29. 29. Li X, Yu H, Zhang B, Li L, Chen W, Yu Q , et al. Molecularly defined and functionally distinct cholinergic subnetworks. Neuron. 2022;110(22):3774-3788 e3777. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2022.08.025
  30. 30. Ahmadian SS, Rezvanian A, Peterson M, Weintraub S, Bigio EH, Mesulam MM, et al. Loss of calbindin-D28K is associated with the full range of tangle pathology within basal forebrain cholinergic neurons in Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiology of Aging. 2015;36(12):3163-3170. DOI: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2015.09.001
  31. 31. Riascos D, Nicholas A, Samaeekia R, Yukhananov R, Mesulam MM, Bigio EH, et al. Alterations of Ca(2)(+)-responsive proteins within cholinergic neurons in aging and Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiology of Aging. 2014;35(6):1325-1333. DOI: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.12.017
  32. 32. Price JL, Stern R. Individual cells in the nucleus basalis--diagonal band complex have restricted axonal projections to the cerebral cortex in the rat. Brain Research. 1983;269(2):352-356. DOI: 10.1016/0006-8993(83)90145-2
  33. 33. Chaves-Coira I, Martin-Cortecero J, Nunez A, Rodrigo-Angulo ML. Basal forebrain nuclei display distinct projecting pathways and functional circuits to sensory primary and prefrontal cortices in the rat. Frontiers in Neuroanatomy. 2018;12:69. DOI: 10.3389/fnana.2018.00069
  34. 34. Robert B, Kimchi EY, Watanabe Y, Chakoma T, Jing M, Li Y, et al. A functional topography within the cholinergic basal forebrain for encoding sensory cues and behavioral reinforcement outcomes. eLife. 2021;10. DOI: 10.7554/eLife.69514
  35. 35. Gulledge AT, Bucci DJ, Zhang SS, Matsui M, Yeh HH. M1 receptors mediate cholinergic modulation of excitability in neocortical pyramidal neurons. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29(31):9888-9902. DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1366-09.2009
  36. 36. Wallace TL, Bertrand D. Importance of the nicotinic acetylcholine receptor system in the prefrontal cortex. Biochemical Pharmacology. 2013;85(12):1713-1720. DOI: 10.1016/j.bcp.2013.04.001
  37. 37. Bloem B, Poorthuis RB, Mansvelder HD. Cholinergic modulation of the medial prefrontal cortex: The role of nicotinic receptors in attention and regulation of neuronal activity. Frontiers in Neural Circuits. 2014;8:17. DOI: 10.3389/fncir.2014.00017
  38. 38. Hedrick T, Waters J. Acetylcholine excites neocortical pyramidal neurons via nicotinic receptors. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2015;113(7):2195-2209. DOI: 10.1152/jn.00716.2014
  39. 39. Poorthuis RB, Bloem B, Schak B, Wester J, de Kock CP, Mansvelder HD. Layer-specific modulation of the prefrontal cortex by nicotinic acetylcholine receptors. Cerebral Cortex. 2013;23(1):148-161. DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhr390
  40. 40. Guillem K, Bloem B, Poorthuis RB, Loos M, Smit AB, Maskos U, et al. Nicotinic acetylcholine receptor beta2 subunits in the medial prefrontal cortex control attention. Science. 2011;333(6044):888-891. DOI: 10.1126/science.1207079
  41. 41. Tian MK, Bailey CD, De Biasi M, Picciotto MR, Lambe EK. Plasticity of prefrontal attention circuitry: Upregulated muscarinic excitability in response to decreased nicotinic signaling following deletion of alpha5 or beta2 subunits. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31(45):16458-16463. DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3600-11.2011
  42. 42. Bailey CD, De Biasi M, Fletcher PJ, Lambe EK. The nicotinic acetylcholine receptor alpha5 subunit plays a key role in attention circuitry and accuracy. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30(27):9241-9252. DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2258-10.2010
  43. 43. Dani JA. Neuronal nicotinic acetylcholine receptor structure and function and response to nicotine. International Review of Neurobiology. 2015;124:3-19. DOI: 10.1016/bs.irn.2015.07.001
  44. 44. Ren JM, Zhang SL, Wang XL, Guan ZZ, Qi XL. Expression levels of the alpha7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptor in the brains of patients with Alzheimer's disease and their effect on synaptic proteins in SH-SY5Y cells. Molecular Medicine Reports. 2020;22(3):2063-2075. DOI: 10.3892/mmr.2020.11253
  45. 45. Ma KG, Qian YH. Alpha 7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptor and its effects on Alzheimer's disease. Neuropeptides. 2019;73:96-106. DOI: 10.1016/j.npep.2018.12.003
  46. 46. De Jaco A, Bernardini L, Rosati J, Tata AM. Alpha-7 nicotinic receptors in nervous system disorders: From function to therapeutic perspectives. Central Nervous System Agents in Medicinal Chemistry. 2017;17(2):100-108. DOI: 10.2174/1871524916666160729111446
  47. 47. Azimi M, Oemisch M, Womelsdorf T. Dissociation of nicotinic alpha7 and alpha4/beta2 sub-receptor agonists for enhancing learning and attentional filtering in nonhuman primates. Psychopharmacology. 2020;237(4):997-1010. DOI: 10.1007/s00213-019-05430-w
  48. 48. Hayward A, Adamson L, Neill JC. Partial agonism at the alpha7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptor improves attention, impulsive action and vigilance in low attentive rats. European Neuropsychopharmacology. 2017;27(4):325-335. DOI: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2017.01.013
  49. 49. Bubser M, Byun N, Wood MR, Jones CK. Muscarinic receptor pharmacology and circuitry for the modulation of cognition. Handbook Expression in Pharmacology. 2012;208:121-166. DOI: 10.1007/978-3-642-23274-9_7
  50. 50. Yamasaki M, Matsui M, Watanabe M. Preferential localization of muscarinic M1 receptor on dendritic shaft and spine of cortical pyramidal cells and its anatomical evidence for volume transmission. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30(12):4408-4418. DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5719-09.2010
  51. 51. Caruana DA, Warburton EC, Bashir ZI. Induction of activity-dependent LTD requires muscarinic receptor activation in medial prefrontal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31(50):18464-18478. DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4719-11.2011
  52. 52. Romberg C, Bartko S, Wess J, Saksida LM, Bussey TJ. Impaired object-location learning and recognition memory but enhanced sustained attention in M2 muscarinic receptor-deficient mice. Psychopharmacology. 2018;235(12):3495-3508. DOI: 10.1007/s00213-018-5065-7
  53. 53. Dillon GM, Shelton D, McKinney AP, Caniga M, Marcus JN, Ferguson MT, et al. Prefrontal cortex lesions and scopolamine impair attention performance of C57BL/6 mice in a novel 2-choice visual discrimination task. Behavioural Brain Research. 2009;204(1):67-76. DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.04.036
  54. 54. Nicklas PR, Kiefer ML, Whalen MA, Stewart MT, Mosura DE, Bennett EM, et al. Muscarinic M1, but not M4, receptor antagonism impairs divided attention in male rats. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior. 2021;205:173184. DOI: 10.1016/j.pbb.2021.173184
  55. 55. Kucinski A, Phillips KB, Koshy Cherian A, Sarter M. Rescuing the attentional performance of rats with cholinergic losses by the M1 positive allosteric modulator TAK-071. Psychopharmacology. 2020;237(1):137-153. DOI: 10.1007/s00213-019-05354-5
  56. 56. Xie HQ , Leung KW, Chen VP, Chan GK, Xu SL, Guo AJ, et al. PRiMA directs a restricted localization of tetrameric AChE at synapses. Chemico-Biological Interactions. 2010;187(1-3):78-83. DOI: 10.1016/j.cbi.2010.02.018
  57. 57. Perrier AL, Massoulie J, Krejci E. PRiMA: The membrane anchor of acetylcholinesterase in the brain. Neuron. 2002;33(2):275-285. DOI: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00584-0
  58. 58. Dobbertin A, Hrabovska A, Dembele K, Camp S, Taylor P, Krejci E, et al. Targeting of acetylcholinesterase in neurons in vivo: A dual processing function for the proline-rich membrane anchor subunit and the attachment domain on the catalytic subunit. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29(14):4519-4530. DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3863-08.2009
  59. 59. Xie HQ , Liang D, Leung KW, Chen VP, Zhu KY, Chan WK, et al. Targeting acetylcholinesterase to membrane rafts: A function mediated by the proline-rich membrane anchor (PRiMA) in neurons. The Journal of Biological Chemistry. 2010;285(15):11537-11546. DOI: 10.1074/jbc.M109.038711
  60. 60. Perrier NA, Kherif S, Perrier AL, Dumas S, Mallet J, Massoulie J. Expression of PRiMA in the mouse brain: Membrane anchoring and accumulation of 'tailed' acetylcholinesterase. The European Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;18(7):1837-1847. DOI: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02914.x
  61. 61. Balachandran RC, Sieg ML, Tran CTQ , Clancy BM, Beaudin SA, Eubig PA. Cholinergic and dopaminergic interactions alter attention and response inhibition in Long-Evans rats performing the 5-choice serial reaction time task. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior. 2018;175:160-173. DOI: 10.1016/j.pbb.2018.10.006
  62. 62. Duysen EG, Stribley JA, Fry DL, Hinrichs SH, Lockridge O. Rescue of the acetylcholinesterase knockout mouse by feeding a liquid diet; phenotype of the adult acetylcholinesterase deficient mouse. Brain Research. Developmental Brain Research. 2002;137(1):43-54. DOI: 10.1016/s0165-3806(02)00367-x
  63. 63. Farar V, Mohr F, Legrand M, Lamotte d'Incamps B, Cendelin J, Leroy J, et al. Near-complete adaptation of the PRiMA knockout to the lack of central acetylcholinesterase. Journal of Neurochemistry. 2012;122(5):1065-1080. DOI: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2012.07856.x
  64. 64. Vetreno RP, Bohnsack JP, Kusumo H, Liu W, Pandey SC, Crews FT. Neuroimmune and epigenetic involvement in adolescent binge ethanol-induced loss of basal forebrain cholinergic neurons: Restoration with voluntary exercise. Addiction Biology. 2020;25(2):e12731. DOI: 10.1111/adb.12731
  65. 65. Hall JM, Gomez-Pinilla F, Savage LM. Nerve growth factor is responsible for exercise-induced recovery of Septohippocampal cholinergic structure and function. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2018;12:773. DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00773
  66. 66. Crews FT, Fisher R, Deason C, Vetreno RP. Loss of basal forebrain cholinergic neurons following adolescent binge ethanol exposure: Recovery with the cholinesterase inhibitor Galantamine. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2021;15:652494. DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2021.652494
  67. 67. Ke HC, Huang HJ, Liang KC, Hsieh-Li HM. Selective improvement of cognitive function in adult and aged APP/PS1 transgenic mice by continuous non-shock treadmill exercise. Brain Research. 2011;1403:1-11. DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.05.056
  68. 68. Teglas T, Nemeth Z, Koller A, Van der Zee EA, Luiten PGM, Nyakas C. Effects of Long-term moderate intensity exercise on cognitive Behaviors and cholinergic forebrain in the aging rat. Neuroscience. 2019;411:65-75. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2019.05.037
  69. 69. Xu L, Long J, Su Z, Xu B, Lin M, Chen Y, et al. Restored presynaptic synaptophysin and cholinergic inputs contribute to the protective effects of physical running on spatial memory in aged mice. Neurobiology of Disease. 2019;132:104586. DOI: 10.1016/j.nbd.2019.104586
  70. 70. Shamsipour S, Sharifi G, Taghian F. An 8-week Administration of Bifidobacterium bifidum and lactobacillus plantarum combined with exercise training alleviates neurotoxicity of Abeta and spatial learning via acetylcholine in Alzheimer rat model. Journal of Molecular Neuroscience. 2021;71(7):1495-1505. DOI: 10.1007/s12031-021-01812-y
  71. 71. Mattson MP, Magnus T. Ageing and neuronal vulnerability. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 2006;7(4):278-294. DOI: 10.1038/nrn1886
  72. 72. Wu H, Williams J, Nathans J. Complete morphologies of basal forebrain cholinergic neurons in the mouse. eLife. 2014;3:e02444. DOI: 10.7554/eLife.02444
  73. 73. Tomasi D, Wiers CE, Manza P, Shokri-Kojori E, Michele-Vera Y, Zhang R, et al. Accelerated aging of the amygdala in alcohol use disorders: Relevance to the dark side of addiction. Cerebral Cortex. 2021;31(7):3254-3265. DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhab006
  74. 74. Woods AJ, Porges EC, Bryant VE, Seider T, Gongvatana A, Kahler CW, et al. Current heavy alcohol consumption is associated with greater cognitive impairment in older adults. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research. 2016;40(11):2435-2444. DOI: 10.1111/acer.13211
  75. 75. Zahr NM, Pohl KM, Saranathan M, Sullivan EV, Pfefferbaum A. Hippocampal subfield CA2+3 exhibits accelerated aging in alcohol use disorder: A preliminary study. Neuroimage Clinic. 2019;22:101764. DOI: 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101764
  76. 76. Rao R, Draper B. Alcohol-related brain damage in older people. Lancet Psychiatry. 2015;2(8):674-675. DOI: 10.1016/S2215-0366(15)00215-1
  77. 77. Grant BF, Chou SP, Saha TD, Pickering RP, Kerridge BT, Ruan WJ, et al. Prevalence of 12-month alcohol use, high-risk drinking, and DSM-IV alcohol use disorder in the United States, 2001-2002 to 2012-2013: Results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on alcohol and related conditions. JAMA Psychiatry. 2017;74(9):911-923. DOI: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.2161
  78. 78. Anttila T, Helkala EL, Viitanen M, Kareholt I, Fratiglioni L, Winblad B, et al. Alcohol drinking in middle age and subsequent risk of mild cognitive impairment and dementia in old age: A prospective population based study. BMJ. 2004;329(7465):539. DOI: 10.1136/bmj.38181.418958.BE
  79. 79. Kivipelto M, Rovio S, Ngandu T, Kareholt I, Eskelinen M, Winblad B, et al. Apolipoprotein E epsilon4 magnifies lifestyle risks for dementia: A population-based study. Journal of Cellular and Molecular Medicine. 2008;12(6B):2762-2771. DOI: 10.1111/j.1582-4934.2008.00296.x
  80. 80. Barnett A, David E, Rohlman A, Nikolova VD, Moy SS, Vetreno RP, et al. Adolescent binge alcohol enhances early Alzheimer's disease pathology in adulthood through Proinflammatory Neuroimmune activation. Frontiers in Pharmacology. 2022;13:884170. DOI: 10.3389/fphar.2022.884170
  81. 81. Hoffman JL, Faccidomo S, Kim M, Taylor SM, Agoglia AE, May AM, et al. Alcohol drinking exacerbates neural and behavioral pathology in the 3xTg-AD mouse model of Alzheimer's disease. International Review of Neurobiology. 2019;148:169-230. DOI: 10.1016/bs.irn.2019.10.017
  82. 82. Huang D, Yu M, Yang S, Lou D, Zhou W, Zheng L, et al. Ethanol alters APP processing and aggravates Alzheimer-associated phenotypes. Molecular Neurobiology. 2018;55(6):5006-5018. DOI: 10.1007/s12035-017-0703-3
  83. 83. Tucker AE, Alicea Pauneto CDM, Barnett AM, Coleman LG Jr. Chronic ethanol causes persistent increases in Alzheimer's tau pathology in female 3xTg-AD mice: A potential role for lysosomal impairment. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2022;16:886634. DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.886634
  84. 84. Fernandez GM, Lew BJ, Vedder LC, Savage LM. Chronic intermittent ethanol exposure leads to alterations in brain-derived neurotrophic factor within the frontal cortex and impaired behavioral flexibility in both adolescent and adult rats. Neuroscience. 2017;348:324-334. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.02.045
  85. 85. Do Carmo S, Kannel B, Cuello AC. The nerve growth factor metabolic pathway dysregulation as cause of Alzheimer's cholinergic atrophy. Cell. 2021;11(1). DOI: 10.3390/cells11010016
  86. 86. Iulita MF, Bistue Millon MB, Pentz R, Aguilar LF, Do Carmo S, Allard S, et al. Differential deregulation of NGF and BDNF neurotrophins in a transgenic rat model of Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiology of Disease. 2017;108:307-323. DOI: 10.1016/j.nbd.2017.08.019
  87. 87. Pentz R, Iulita MF, Mikutra-Cencora M, Ducatenzeiler A, Bennett DA, Cuello AC. A new role for matrix metalloproteinase-3 in the NGF metabolic pathway: Proteolysis of mature NGF and sex-specific differences in the continuum of Alzheimer's pathology. Neurobiology of Disease. 2021;148:105150. DOI: 10.1016/j.nbd.2020.105150
  88. 88. Perovic M, Tesic V, Mladenovic Djordjevic A, Smiljanic K, Loncarevic-Vasiljkovic N, Ruzdijic S, et al. BDNF transcripts, proBDNF and proNGF, in the cortex and hippocampus throughout the life span of the rat. Age (Dordrecht, Netherlands). 2013;35(6):2057-2070. DOI: 10.1007/s11357-012-9495-6
  89. 89. Vecchio I, Sorrentino L, Paoletti A, Marra R, Arbitrio M. The state of the art on acetylcholinesterase inhibitors in the treatment of Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Central Nervous System Diseases. 2021;13:11795735211029113. DOI: 10.1177/11795735211029113
  90. 90. Marucci G, Buccioni M, Ben DD, Lambertucci C, Volpini R, Amenta F. Efficacy of acetylcholinesterase inhibitors in Alzheimer's disease. Neuropharmacology. 2021;190:108352. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2020.108352
  91. 91. Srivastava S, Ahmad R, Khare SK. Alzheimer's disease and its treatment by different approaches: A review. European Journal of Medicinal Chemistry. 2021;216:113320. DOI: 10.1016/j.ejmech.2021.113320
  92. 92. Bohnen NI, Kaufer DI, Hendrickson R, Ivanco LS, Lopresti BJ, Koeppe RA, et al. Degree of inhibition of cortical acetylcholinesterase activity and cognitive effects by donepezil treatment in Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry. 2005;76(3):315-319. DOI: 10.1136/jnnp.2004.038729
  93. 93. Moss DE, Perez RG. Anti-neurodegenerative benefits of acetylcholinesterase inhibitors in Alzheimer's disease: Nexus of cholinergic and nerve growth factor dysfunction. Current Alzheimer Research. 2021;18(13):1010-1022. DOI: 10.2174/1567205018666211215150547
  94. 94. Petrella JR, Prince SE, Krishnan S, Husn H, Kelley L, Doraiswamy PM. Effects of donepezil on cortical activation in mild cognitive impairment: A pilot double-blind placebo-controlled trial using functional MR imaging. AJNR. American Journal of Neuroradiology. 2009;30(2):411-416. DOI: 10.3174/ajnr.A1359
  95. 95. Li W, Antuono PG, Xie C, Chen G, Jones JL, Ward BD, et al. Changes in regional cerebral blood flow and functional connectivity in the cholinergic pathway associated with cognitive performance in subjects with mild Alzheimer's disease after 12-week donepezil treatment. NeuroImage. 2012;60(2):1083-1091. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.077
  96. 96. Muller P, Vellage AK, Schmicker M, Menze I, Grothe MJ, Teipel SJ, et al. Structural MRI of the basal forebrain as predictor of cognitive response to galantamine in healthy older adults-a randomized controlled double-blinded crossover study. Alzheimers Dementia (N Y). 2021;7(1):e12153. DOI: 10.1002/trc2.12153
  97. 97. Bagaria J, Bagyinszky E, An SSA. Genetics, functions, and clinical impact of Presenilin-1 (PSEN1) gene. International Journal of Molecular Sciences. 2022;23(18). DOI: 10.3390/ijms231810970
  98. 98. Bustos V, Pulina MV, Ledo J. Amyloidogenic and anti-amyloidogenic properties of presenilin 1. Advances in Pharmacology. 2021;90:239-251. DOI: 10.1016/bs.apha.2020.09.010
  99. 99. Garcia-Ayllon MS, Campanari ML, Montenegro MF, Cuchillo-Ibanez I, Belbin O, Lleo A, et al. Presenilin-1 influences processing of the acetylcholinesterase membrane anchor PRiMA. Neurobiology of Aging. 2014;35(7):1526-1536. DOI: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.01.147
  100. 100. Jazi R, Lalonde R, Qian S, Strazielle C. Regional brain evaluation of acetylcholinesterase activity in PS1/A246E transgenic mice. Neuroscience Research. 2009;63(2):106-114. DOI: 10.1016/j.neures.2008.11.002
  101. 101. Romberg C, Mattson MP, Mughal MR, Bussey TJ, Saksida LM. Impaired attention in the 3xTgAD mouse model of Alzheimer's disease: Rescue by donepezil (Aricept). The Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31(9):3500-3507. DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5242-10.2011
  102. 102. Lindner MD, Hogan JB, Hodges DB Jr, Orie AF, Chen P, Corsa JA, et al. Donepezil primarily attenuates scopolamine-induced deficits in psychomotor function, with moderate effects on simple conditioning and attention, and small effects on working memory and spatial mapping. Psychopharmacology. 2006;188(4):629-640. DOI: 10.1007/s00213-006-0556-3
  103. 103. Proulx E, Power SK, Oliver DK, Sargin D, McLaurin J, Lambe EK. Apamin improves prefrontal nicotinic impairment in mouse model of Alzheimer's disease. Cerebral Cortex. 2020;30(2):563-574. DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhz107
  104. 104. Driscoll LL, Carroll JC, Moon J, Crnic LS, Levitsky DA, Strupp BJ. Impaired sustained attention and error-induced stereotypy in the aged Ts65Dn mouse: A mouse model of down syndrome and Alzheimer's disease. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2004;118(6):1196-1205. DOI: 10.1037/0735-7044.118.6.1196
  105. 105. Chen GJ, Xiong Z, Yan Z. Abeta impairs nicotinic regulation of inhibitory synaptic transmission and interneuron excitability in prefrontal cortex. Molecular Neurodegeneration. 2013;8:3. DOI: 10.1186/1750-1326-8-3
  106. 106. Viel TA, Caetano AL, Albuquerque MS, Araujo MS, Buck HS. Chronic infusion of amyloid-beta peptide and sustained attention altered alpha7 nicotinic receptor density in the rat brain. Current Alzheimer Research. 2012;9(10):1210-1220. DOI: 10.2174/156720512804142930
  107. 107. Dwomoh L, Tejeda GS, Tobin AB. Targeting the M1 muscarinic acetylcholine receptor in Alzheimer's disease. Neuronal. Signals. 2022;6(1):NS20210004. DOI: 10.1042/NS20210004
  108. 108. Scarr E, McLean C, Dean B. Higher levels of different muscarinic receptors in the cortex and hippocampus from subjects with Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Neural Transmission (Vienna). 2017;124(3):273-284. DOI: 10.1007/s00702-016-1625-3
  109. 109. Tsang SW, Lai MK, Kirvell S, Francis PT, Esiri MM, Hope T, et al. Impaired coupling of muscarinic M1 receptors to G-proteins in the neocortex is associated with severity of dementia in Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiology of Aging. 2006;27(9):1216-1223. DOI: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.07.010
  110. 110. Reitz NL, Nunes PT, Savage LM. Adolescent binge-type ethanol exposure in rats mirrors age-related cognitive decline by suppressing cholinergic tone and hippocampal neurogenesis. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2021;15:772857. DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2021.772857
  111. 111. Givens B. Effect of ethanol on sustained attention in rats. Psychopharmacology. 1997;129(2):135-140. DOI: 10.1007/s002130050173
  112. 112. Spear LP. Consequences of adolescent use of alcohol and other drugs: Studies using rodent models. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2016;70:228-243. DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.07.026
  113. 113. Nunes PT, Kipp BT, Reitz NL, Savage LM. Aging with alcohol-related brain damage: Critical brain circuits associated with cognitive dysfunction. International Review of Neurobiology. 2019;148:101-168. DOI: 10.1016/bs.irn.2019.09.002
  114. 114. Sey NYA, Gomez AA, Madayag AC, Boettiger CA, Robinson DL. Adolescent intermittent ethanol impairs behavioral flexibility in a rat foraging task in adulthood. Behavioural Brain Research. 2019;373:112085. DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2019.112085
  115. 115. Vetreno RP, Broadwater M, Liu W, Spear LP, Crews FT. Adolescent, but not adult, binge ethanol exposure leads to persistent global reductions of choline acetyltransferase expressing neurons in brain. PLoS One. 2014;9(11):e113421. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0113421
  116. 116. Sun W, Chen L, Zheng W, Wei X, Wu W, Duysen EG, et al. Study of acetylcholinesterase activity and apoptosis in SH-SY5Y cells and mice exposed to ethanol. Toxicology. 2017;384:33-39. DOI: 10.1016/j.tox.2017.04.007
  117. 117. Fernandez GM, Savage LM. Adolescent binge ethanol exposure alters specific forebrain cholinergic cell populations and leads to selective functional deficits in the prefrontal cortex. Neuroscience. 2017;361:129-143. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.08.013
  118. 118. Birrell JM, Brown VJ. Medial frontal cortex mediates perceptual attentional set shifting in the rat. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2000;20(11):4320-4324 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10818167
  119. 119. Kipp BT, Nunes PT, Savage LM. Sex differences in cholinergic circuits and behavioral disruptions following chronic ethanol exposure with and without thiamine deficiency. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research. 2021;45(5):1013-1027. DOI: 10.1111/acer.14594
  120. 120. Roland JJ, Levinson M, Vetreno RP, Savage LM. Differential effects of systemic and intraseptal administration of the acetylcholinesterase inhibitor tacrine on the recovery of spatial behavior in an animal model of diencephalic amnesia. European Journal of Pharmacology. 2010;629(1-3):31-39. DOI: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2009.12.005

Written By

Matthew Fecik and Lisa M. Savage

Submitted: 04 January 2023 Reviewed: 10 February 2023 Published: 22 May 2023