Open access peer-reviewed chapter

The First Offer of Alcohol from the Adult Person and Cannabis Use

Written By

Alojz Nociar and Stanislava Šaffová

Submitted: 10 October 2022 Reviewed: 28 October 2022 Published: 22 November 2022

DOI: 10.5772/intechopen.108805

From the Edited Volume

Substance Use Disorder - New Research Perspectives in the Diagnosis, Treatment, and Prognosis

Edited by Patricia Sampedro-Piquero, Román Darío Moreno Fernández and Clara Zancada-Menéndez

Chapter metrics overview

58 Chapter Downloads

View Full Metrics

Abstract

This chapter is based on the data from the national survey on tobacco, alcohol, and drugs among primary and secondary school students aged 15–19 implemented in eight regions of Slovakia during Spring 2018. An unexpected shift was observed in the relationship between the age young people were offered alcohol by an adult and the age when they first experienced its effect. In previous surveys, the mean age of the first offer predated the felt effect of alcohol (“tipsiness”) by about 1 year on average. Our data revealed that following the initial offer of alcohol by an adult, approximately ¾ of participants felt the effect of alcohol later or during the same year (groups 1 and 2); however, in the remaining cases, the effect was felt before alcohol was offered by an adult (group 3). Thus, it appears that one-fifth to one-quarter of participants did not go through the usual ritual of initiation by adults. The analysis showed that this group of participants used cannabis more often than the rest of the sample. Furthermore, there were differences observed among the three groups in reported school attendance, legal and illegal drug use, and bullying. Possible implications of these findings are discussed.

Keywords

  • age of the first offer
  • tipsiness before or after offer of alcohol
  • cannabis use
  • adult persons and drinking initiation

1. Introduction

In research focused on the first exposure to alcohol, the age of the initial contact with this substance is frequently used as a crucial variable potentially associated with future health and psychosocial problems connected to alcohol and drug dependency [1, 2]. However, less is known about the typical situations, contexts, and circumstances in which such first contact and early use of alcohol occur. Alcohol is often offered to young people by an adult in cultures with a permissive approach to this substance (in which it is often legalized and socially accepted). It is interesting that the use of legal drugs in early adolescence is considered as something obvious and even normal even in the countries where legislation does not permit underage drinking and tobacco smoking, and bans are in place for selling these products to minors and adolescents. Since minors do report the use of legal drugs in anonymous surveys, it is likely that alcohol beverages and tobacco products are offered to them by adults, most probably by close family members, relatives, older friends, or siblings. It would therefore seem a common sense to assume that asking about the age of the first offer of alcohol from an adult would be practically the same as asking for the age of the first drink.

Previous research aimed at differentiating between the age of the first contact with any sort of alcohol and the first experience of alcohol-induced changes in mood or psychological state demonstrated that the age of the first drink may be less important as an indicator of the future problems than the age of the first alcohol intoxication [3].

Some studies attempted to explain the use of legal and illegal drugs in early adolescence via specific micro-social conditions, adherence to traditions and different drinking cultures in various European countries is based on the geographical location [4], and other authors investigated whether this phenomenon might be caused by significant sociopolitical changes such as those happening in Europe after the fall of the so-called iron curtain, which divided the West from the Eastern European countries after 1989 [5].

Another research trend focused on the investigation of possible connections between legal and illegal drug use and antisocial behaviors at school (e.g., aggression or bullying). The researchers examined and highlighted the associations between bullying and the use of both legal and illegal drugs in those who perpetrated bullying as well as in the victims and bystanders. Other relevant psychological and social variables were included in their investigation [6, 7, 8]. Ake to boli napriklad tie variables?

Alcohol tends to be used as a socially tolerated drug throughout human life during special occasions and may be a common part of various cultural traditions. During such special occasions (e.g., name days, birthdays, Christmas, or New Year), the family tends to be the primary environment where children and adolescents may be offered alcohol by an important adult, which may model their future alcohol-related behaviors and attitudes (i.e., the social approval of underage drinking). For example, the studies using natural experiments demonstrated that pre-school children who were asked to pretend to act as adults during a birthday celebration started to pretend to be “drunk,” likely imitating what they had previously seen [9].

We tried to find out more about these types of situations through two of the core questions of the TAD (Tobacco, Alcohol, Drugs) questionnaires. The data showed that the percentages of the children and adolescents to whom alcohol beverages were offered by adults during socially approved events seemed to be high and growing year after year as outlined in Figures 1 and 2.

Figure 1.

Champagne wine offered by parents to their children as a new Year’s toast (percentages in 2018).

Figure 2.

Any alcohol offered to teens by adults for the first time (in percentages).

The data clearly demonstrated the annual increases in number of young people to whom alcohol beverages were offered by an adult during a socially approved event, while the average age of the first experience with the three main types of alcohol beverages was relatively constant (around an average of 10 years and growing very slowly)—please refer to Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Tobacco and alcohol used for the first time: 11–14 year olds.

Advertisement

2. Methods

Three TAD questionnaires were used to monitor the level of tobacco, alcohol, and drug use via surveys conducted in regular 4-yearly cycles from 1994 till 2018. The questionnaires were devised to map the impact of the nationwide drug prevention program “School without alcohol, nicotine and drugs” [10]. The questionnaires consisted of [number of questions]. The data from these surveys were processed by statistical package for social sciences (SPSS 20.0).

The surveys were carried out among primary school pupils from grades 5 to 9, aged 10–15 (TAD1). TAD1 questionnaires assessed 30 day, 12 months, and lifetime prevalence of drug use, together with the items on early start of drug use and family environment. The items on aggression and bullying at school were used from 2010 until 2018 [11].

A similar survey (TAD2) was carried out in secondary schools among 16–19 year olds. Apart from the traditional scheme of epidemiological information on drugs, bullying, and aggression, TAD2 contained items assessing the presence of alcohol dependence symptoms, namely screening CAGE (Cut down, Annoyed, Guilty, Eye-opener) and shortened ADS (Alcohol Dependence Scale—see [12, 13]).

Finally, TAD3 questionnaire was used asking for primary and secondary school teachers’ own use of legal drugs such as tobacco and alcohol, knowledge about illegal drugs, as well as their attitudes toward and willingness to take part in school prevention programs.

All the above surveys were approved by the Ministry of Education of the Slovak republic, and the data collection was implemented by the Ministry of Health’ network of the Slovak Office of Public Health in the whole country under the coordination of the first author.

From 1998, TAD2 questionnaire contained two items to capture at least some information related to the early start of the individual experience with alcohol; the questions remained unchanged in all TAD2 versions until 2018. They were as follows:

Did it happen, that adult person offered you to drink alcohol beverage?

1□ No.

2□ Yes → If yes, 1st time it had happened, when I was about:........years old.

Have you ever felt, that you were somewhat “tipsy” (or “half drunk”)?

1□ No.

2□ Yes → If yes, 1st time it had happened, when I was about:........years old.

The last one from the seven anonymous surveys, from which the data for the purpose of this chapter were derived, was conducted in 2018 among 15–19 year olds (n = 4042; 2194 boys and 1848 girls) from ninth grades of the primary schools and from four grades of the secondary schools of all types from grammar and vocational schools in the Slovak republic [14].

2.1 Design and data

All TAD questionnaires were administered anonymously, and respondents received their paper-pencil versions, provided their answers without any personal data, which might be used to identity an individual, class, or school. Only the information about the year of birth and gender was required as obligatory to enable comparisons between boys and girls of certain age. Every respondent had right to refuse to take part in survey.

After completing, respondents returned filled questionnaires in sealed envelopes without any mark, except for the mark written by research assistants to the big envelope with encrypted code of class and school, assigned by survey coordinator. In the seven research waves, 32,814 primary school pupils, 29,375 secondary school students, and finally, 10,180 teachers from both types of schools participated in the surveys.

All three TAD questionnaires were used across the eight main regions of Slovakia, with their samples being self-weighted for gender, school, class, region, and teaching language, taking into account also minorities as the part of the selection criteria.

The samples were created by a stratified proportional random sampling from ninth grades of primary and first to fourth grades of secondary schools, from five types of schools (primary, secondary grammar schools lasting 4 and 8 years, then secondary specialized schools with and without maturity exams, i.e., those lasting from 4 to 5 years, and the secondary specialized schools lasting 3 years only), selected proportionally from eight main regions of the whole country.

The sampling unit was school, and within each selected secondary school, four classes were randomly selected by research assistants from the first to the fourth grades; and one class from each of the existing ninth grades in selected primary schools, because part of the cohort of 15–16 year olds was still in primary schools, while approximately ¾ of them were in the secondary schools, predominantly in the first grade.

The scope of selection was defined according to the requirement for reliability (95%) and preciseness (2%), with respect to the existing numbers of primary school pupils and secondary school students still attending schools in Slovakia and with respect to the age range to be covered, that is, from 15 to 16 to 18–19 year olds.

2.2 Results

At the beginning, we counted overall means of ages for offer and for effect in the same way as before, that is, for the whole sample. But during TAD2 data entries in 2018, we noticed frequent inconsistencies with previous results such as a younger age of the first experience of tipsiness than the age of alcohol offer. Table 1 outlines the average ages for the first offer and the effect felt after any drinking of alcohol for TAD surveys from 1998 until 2018.

Year of TAD
survey
Mean
age
1998
n = 548
2002
n = 3678
2006
n = 4915
2010
n = 3112
2014
n = 2252
2018
n = 1751
Alcohol offered by adult person for the first time13,3712,8813,4813,6813,8914,15
Effect of alcohol felt for the first time (“tipsiness”)14,2114,5114,6214,5214,5515,08

Table 1.

Mean ages of the first offer of alcohol by an adult person and the age when the effect of alcohol was felt (“tipsiness”).

In previous surveys, the mean age at the two points mapped by TAD2 was calculated, as it is indicated in the table above. Averages for the whole dataset showed that the first offer of alcohol came earlier, while “tipsiness” felt subjectively for the first time was observed about 1 year later. However, closer inspection of the data in part of the cases had shown that the “tipsiness” occurred later or during the same year, but in some smaller, but still in substantial proportion of the cases, this effect of alcohol was reported as felt before any alcohol beverage was offered by an adult person.

In other words, approximately one-fifth to one-quarter of the adolescents did not go through the usual ritual of alcohol drinking initiation by adults; they appeared to try alcohol in their own way, not adhering to any symbolic adult permissions.

Thus, three groups were identified within this whole sample in respect to the age of alcohol beverage offer and the age of the pharmacological effect of alcohol upon mood and/or psychological state, felt for the first time in life:

  1. offer and effect occurred during the same year;

  2. effect occurred one or more years after the offer;

  3. effect was experienced before the offer from an adult person.

The first two groups appeared to have acted in line with unwritten social rules of handling situations where alcohol is offered, that is, group 1 (no difference in the age, i.e., offer and effect in the same year), group 2 (effect delayed), and finally, the third group, where offer of alcohol from an adult was not related to the effect felt (which thus is unlikely to be associated with an adult offer) (Figure 4) [14].

Figure 4.

Average ages of the first experience with alcohol effect (“tipsiness”): After offer of alcohol; and before offer of alcohol by adult person.

These groups were then compared with respect to some of the variables from TAD2 surveys’ results to find the possible differences in lifetime prevalence of cannabis. There were no significant differences in numbers of boys and girls in any of these three groups.

As we might see, the third group differed from first and second groups in higher proportion of cannabis lifetime use (Table 2), in this case characterized by an almost reverse percentages of cannabis lifetime prevalence. And roughly similar third groups were identified also in the series of previous TAD2 surveys, as well as across the teenage period (Table 3).

Effect_Before_After variableTotal
1st group2nd group3rd group
Offer & effect in the same ageTipsiness after Adult OfferTipsiness before Adult Offer
Did you ever smoked marihuana or used hashish?YESn = 260n = 370n = 204n = 834
41,3%37,8%61,6%43,0%
NOn = 370n = 608n = 127n = 1105
58,7%62,2%38,4%57,0%
Totaln = 630n = 978n = 331n = 1939
100,0%100,0%100,0%100,0%

Table 2.

Cannabis use differences between three groups of respondents in TAD2 2018 survey (Chi2 significant at 0,000).

Effect_Before_After variable
1st group2nd group3rd group
Offer & effect in the same ageTipsiness after Adult OfferTipsiness before Adult Offer
Students from 16 to 19 years:161718191617181916171819
YES
Did you ever smoked cannabis or used hashish?
NO
37,047,446,943,833,937,137,937,147,271,658,667,3
63,052,651,356,266,162,962,162,952,828,441,432,7

Table 3.

Cannabis lifetime prevalence in three groups differing in the age of alcohol offer and tipsiness effects sorted by age from 16 to 19.

The relationship between cannabis use and the tipsiness experienced before an adult offer of alcohol did not appear to be incidental as it was observable not only in the year of the last implemented TAD2 survey, but also across all surveys from 1998 till 2018. This was an interesting finding, and we wondered whether these differences of the third group from two others might be related to other variables, such as parental control, family status, school attendance, or others.

Therefore, we decided to use the two questions about the offer and the tipsiness in a forthcoming broader survey implemented across Europe, namely the ESPAD project during the Spring 2019 [9]. (See Table 4).

Effect_Before_After variableTotal
1st group2nd group3rd group
Offer & effect in the same ageTipsiness after Adult OfferTipsiness before Adult Offer
Did you ever smoked marihuana or used hashish?YESn = 536n = 754n = 594n = 1884
41,6%38,2%63,9%44,9%
NOn = 752n = 1221n = 336n = 2309
58,4%61,8%36,1%55,1%
Totaln = 1228n = 1975n = 930n = 4193
100,0%100,0%100,0%100,0%

Table 4.

Cannabis use in three groups of respondents in ESPAD 2019 survey differing in the age of alcohol offer and tipsiness (Chi2 significant at 0,000).

The results outlined in Table 4 appear to confirm the trend revealed by TAD2 survey implemented in 2018: the third group as documented in ESPAD survey carried out 1 year later (n = 9338) was different from the two other groups in lifetime prevalence of cannabis use, even though these results were not fully identical.

As there are likely to be additional variables relevant to this problem, we tried to map some of them within our existing datasets. Even though our surveys, both TAD and ESPAD, were not specifically designed to examine this problem (early experience with alcohol and its subjectively felt pharmacological effect), we decided to map at least some of the variables such as the important persons from the family environment, peers, older friends, classmates, or other people, as well as relevant events from close social environments of adolescents.

We tried to map the following variables:

  1. School attendance

  2. Legal and illegal drugs use

  3. Alcohol abuse and related problems

  4. Cannabis use related problems

  5. Bullying and aggression at school

The third group, which appeared to have a tendency to circumvent accepted social norms and conventions about the initiation and/or the entrance into the adult community, was different from the two other groups also in other variables, not only in cannabis use prevalence (and it seems at the same time, that those variables were relatively less socially desirable).

In our preliminary analysis, we examined the variables such as school attendance with three main reasons of missing at school (see Table 5).

Effect_Before_After variableChi2
1st group2nd group3rd group
Offer & effect in the same ageTipsiness after Adult OfferTipsiness before Adult Offer
Missing at school because of:Illness58,9%60,4%56,9%,056
Truancy34,2%33,1%42,1%0,001
Something else60,8%62,2%62,8%,229

Table 5.

School attendance in three groups differing in the age of the first offer of alcohol and in the age of the first tipsiness felt in the ESPAD2019 survey.

As for drugs use, the second and the third group were compared by nonparametric tests, and only results indicating significant differences are included in Table 6:

Question in TAD2(*), or questions in ESPAD (**)Effect_after_offerEffect_before_offerChi2
Smoking cigarette at least once during lifetime*65,286,90,000
Occasional smoking (1–2 cigarettes per month)*36,861,70,000
Regular smoking (1–3 and more cigarettes per day)*22,941,20,000
Hard drunkenness with the occurrence of palimpsests*49,168,10,000
Lifetime use of ecstasy*6,812,10,001
Lifetime use of LSD or hallucinogens*3,47,90,000
C A G E**0,830,920,40
A D S**7,708,650,010

Table 6.

Legal and illegal drugs and related problems.

As for alcohol and related problems, except of usual core questions on prevalence (lifetime, 12 months, and 30 days), we also used a separate module with 30 items, containing two screening scales—CAGE and ADS [12, 13, 14].

Finally, cannabis-related problems were mapped by the items of CAST, used regularly in the ESPAD survey [15, 16]—see Figure 5:

Figure 5.

The results of the CAST screening – Problems with cannabis use.

Cutoff point, that is, point indicating case finding for CAST (which is 7 points) [17], we have found significantly higher number of the third group members, reporting more cannabis-use-related problems compared to group 1 and 2% (Chi2 significant at 0.000).

Only one from the items mapping aggression and bullying at school had shown significant difference in comparison of three groups:

He/she took part when group of his/her friends attacked other group.

Offer before Effect: (Chi2 significant at 0.05);

As for items mapping group differences in perceived risk of drug use:

Five or more drinks every weekend—great perceived risk.

Offer before Effect: 54.3%; Effect before Offer: 47.3% (Chi2 significant at 0,000).

Regular smoking of marihuana—great perceived risk.

Offer before Effect: 49.0%; Effect before Offer: 40.2% (Chi2 significant at 0,000).

As for items mapping risky or hazardous behavior, like gambling:

He/she played for money—once monthly to 2–3 times weekly.

Offer before Effect: 15.3%; Effect before Offer: 21.0% (Chi2 significant at 0,000).

He/she played for money on slot-machines—once monthly to 2–3 times weekly.

Offer before Effect: 5.1%; Effect before Offer: 9.9% (Chi2 significant at 0,000).

Finally, the last items estimated a level of parental control:

His/her parents are setting rules on what I can do outside—almost never.

Offer before Effect: 36.4%; Effect before Offer: 44.2% (Chi2 significant at 0,000).

His/her parents do know where he/she is at Saturday evenings—usually they do not.

Offer before Effect: 4.6%; Effect before Offer: 5.4% (Chi2 significant at 0.023).

Advertisement

3. Conclusions

In previous surveys, the mean age at two points was calculated for the whole sample, and the first offer of alcohol came earlier on average, while the effect of alcohol (i.e. being “tipsy” or “half drunk”) felt subjectively for the first time was observed around 1 year later. However, closer inspection of the data from 2018 survey revealed that in circa ¾ of the cases the effect occurred either later (mean age = 15.45) or during the same year (mean age = 14.94), but in the rest of the cases, this effect was felt before alcohol was offered by an adult person (mean age = 14.21).

Thus, around one-fifth to one-quarter of teens circumvented common ritual of alcohol drinking initiation by adults, and they have tried to do it their own way, not adhering to any symbolic adult permissions. Subsequent analysis had shown that this group not only has used cannabis more often than the rest of our sample, but there were also differences in other variables, such as school attendance, parental control, group aggression, and legal and illegal drug use.

These findings are consistent with several studies, where regular or even daily use of tobacco and alcohol was connected to the presence of risk behaviors correlated with early start of cannabis use by 15-year-olds or less, and this was even more intensive when such an early initiation had happened even earlier—in the age of 13 or less [18].

Perhaps some conclusions and recommendations might be eventually formulated. But at the moment, it would be not possible to define clear and rigorous facts with relevant explanatory power—our study was only mapping one part of a broad problem—how teens do start to form their drinking, smoking, and later on sometimes also drug taking habits. Together with other sorts of behaviors, which are, so to say, not very socially desirable—such as truancy, aggression, breaking accepted rules. All that taking place in social environment is now more than ever changing very rapidly with many unexpected and global changes and challenges.

If there would be some interest to investigate this problem in the future more deeply, planning of data gathering and introducing of relevant variables should take into account quite concrete matters—like how to ask appropriate questions about important others from family narrow environment, but maybe also broader environment such as districts or communities, social occasions where such an offer of alcohol from adult persons might came, and then types of such persons, such as peers of perhaps older siblings, etc.

Small preliminary attempts were already made also at our school via several bachelor theses [19], with some results indicating that the offer of alcohol by adult person occurred typically during family events, where mostly father, less frequently grandfather, or uncle has played active role in these processes, while females were involved also, but very rarely, in comparison with males. On the other hand, there were also indications of more frequent offers during less formal social events than social events within the family used to be—with offer of alcohol from older friends, peers, or sometimes from siblings.

So the main findings of this study on the distribution of the sample according to the age on an offer and effect of alcohol might be summarized as follows:

  1. First two groups differed clearly from the third group in the lower occurrence of cannabis use.

  2. This third group has had also higher use of tobacco.

  3. The same is true for alcohol—also in simultaneously used CAGE screening and ADS scale.

  4. This third group was also higher in illegal drug use—also in CAST screening targeted at problems connected with cannabis use.

  5. This third group was different also in socially less desirable behaviors, such as truancy or group aggression.

  6. This third group was trying to stay more or less out of parental control.

  7. Perceived risk of drug taking was much lower in this third group, which has displayed a tendency to engage in gaming and gambling.

  8. And finally, in spite of delineation of some personality traits in this group, we cannot say anything conclusive—also because of the fact that in this case, quantitative study with its rules and requirements, like those of anonymity, is reaching its limits and needs to be complemented perhaps by complementary qualitative studies.

Advertisement

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. 1. Mustonen A, Alakokkare A-E, Salom C, et al. Age of first alcohol intoxication and psychiatric disorders in young adulthood – A prospective birth cohort study. Addictive Behaviors. 2021;118:106910
  2. 2. Härkönen J, Mäkelä P. Elaborating the association of age at first drink with risky drinking: Results from a cross-sectional survey. Drugs: Education, Prevention and Policy. 24 Jul 2021:1-8. DOI: 10.1080/09687637.2021.1952932
  3. 3. Newton-Howes G, Cook S, Martin G, Foulds JA, Boden JM. Comparison of age of first drink and age of first intoxication as predictors of substance use and mental health problems in adulthood. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2019;194:238-243
  4. 4. Kraus L et al. “Are the times A-Changin”? Trends in adolescent substance use In Europe. Addiction. 2018;113:1317-1332
  5. 5. Nociar A, Sierosƚawski J, Csémy L. Substance use among European students: East–west comparison between 1995 and 2011. Central European Journal of Public Health. 2016;24:281-288
  6. 6. Radliff KM, Wheaton JE, Robinson K, Morris J. Illuminating the relationship between bullying and substance use among middle and high school youth. Addictive Behaviors. 2012;37:569-572
  7. 7. Durand V, Hennessey J, Wells DS, Crothers LM, Kolbert JB, et al. Bullying and substance use in children and adolescents. Journal of Addiction Research and Therapy. 2013;4(4):58. DOI: 10.4172/2155-6105.1000 158
  8. 8. Nociar A. Age, gender and substance use in relation to violence at primary and secondary schools in Slovakia. In: 4th World Conference: Violence in School and Public Policies. Abstracts. Lisbon: Calouste Gulbenkian Foundation; June 23-25, 2008. p. 100
  9. 9. Zeigarnikova BV, Bratus BS. Očerki po Psichologii anomaľnogo Razvitija ličnosti. [Outlines of Psychology of Abnormal Development of Personality]. Moskva: Izd-vo. Moskovskogo Universiteta; 1980
  10. 10. Novotný I, Nociar A. School without alcohol, tobacco and drugs: Prevention project and target population survey in Slovakia. In: Proceedings of the 38th and 21st International Institutes on the Prevention and Treatment of Alcoholism and Drug Dependence. Abstracts. Prague: ICAA, CIPAT; June 5-10, 1994. p. 180
  11. 11. Olweus D. Bullying at School: What we Know and What we Can Do. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing; 2005. p. 140
  12. 12. Skinner HA, Allen BA. Alcohol dependence syndrome: Measurement and validation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1982;91:199-209
  13. 13. Nociar A, Juráš L, Škultétyová M. Škála alkoholovej závislosti – ADS [Alcohol Dependence Scale – ADS]. Bratislava: Psychodiagnostické a didaktické testy, š.p; 1990. p. 51
  14. 14. Nociar A. Záverečná správa z Prieskumu TAD u žiakov ZŠ, študentov SŠ a Ich učiteľov v Roku 2018 [Final Report from TAD Surveys among Elementary School Pupils, Secondary School Students and their Teachers in 2018]. Bratislava: VUDPaP; 2018. p. 87
  15. 15. Nociar A. Európsky školský prieskum o alkohole a iných drogách (ESPAD) v SR za rok 2019. In: Záverečná správa. [European School Survey on Alcohol and Other Drugs (ESPAD) in Slovakia in 2019. Final Report]. Bratislava: Research Institute for Child Psychology and Pathopsychology; 2019. p. 88
  16. 16. Molinaro S, Vincente J, Benedetti E, Cerrai S, et al. ESPAD Report 2019: Results from the European School Survey Project on Alcohol and Other Drugs. Luxembourg: Publication Office of the European Union; 2020. p. 130
  17. 17. Legleye S, Guignard R, Richard JB, Kraus L, Pabst A, Beck F. Properties of the cannabis abuse screening test (CAST) in the general population. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research. 2015;24:170-183
  18. 18. Kokkevi A, Gabhainn SN, Spyropoulou M. Early initiation of cannabis use: A cross-national European perspective. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2006;39:712-719
  19. 19. Šátor B. Prvá Ponuka Alkoholu Od dospelého Vo vzťahu k začiatku fajčenia Marihuany [the First Offer of Alcohol by an Adult Related to the Beginning of Marihuana Smoking] [Thesis]. Bratislava: Saint Elisabeth University of Health and Social Work, Department of Psychology; 2021

Written By

Alojz Nociar and Stanislava Šaffová

Submitted: 10 October 2022 Reviewed: 28 October 2022 Published: 22 November 2022