Open access peer-reviewed chapter

Wavelet Correlation Analysis for Quantifying Similarities and Real-Time Estimates of Information Encoded or Decoded in Single-Trial Oscillatory Brain Waves

Written By

Takaaki Sato, Riichi Kajiwara, Ichiro Takashima and Toshio Iijima

Submitted: 05 October 2017 Reviewed: 02 February 2018 Published: 01 March 2018

DOI: 10.5772/intechopen.74810

From the Edited Volume

Wavelet Theory and Its Applications

Edited by Sudhakar Radhakrishnan

Chapter metrics overview

1,248 Chapter Downloads

View Full Metrics

Abstract

Even in cases when we recognize identical objects or when we behave similarly, the spatiotemporal activities in the brain are likely to fluctuate to various degrees. Temporally fluctuating responses easily decrease by averaging replicate measures. We previously developed a wavelet correlation analysis that tolerates the across-trial oscillatory phase variability observed in odor-induced cortical responses. The wavelet correlation analysis revealed a change in the neuronal information redundancy of transient and oscillatory brain waves from the dependencies on stimulus experience (high redundancy) to stimulus quality (low redundancy) between the input and output layers of the anterior piriform cortex in guinea pigs. We report on its application to estimate information in the fine temporal structures of single-trial brain waves. By using a set of standard brain waves for each information in a given category, the highest wavelet correlation coefficients provided the first candidate of estimated information with 75% accuracy. Moreover, the probability of including the correct information for the two upper candidates, regardless of information redundancy of the signal sources, was >92%. The wavelet correlation analysis is useful for similarity analyses and real-time estimates of in-brain information and for its application to brain-machine interfaces or medical/research tools.

Keywords

  • cross-correlation analysis
  • electroencephalography
  • information redundancy
  • odor representation
  • oscillatory local field potentials
  • real-time estimation
  • wavelet transformation

1. Introduction

In the sensory system, a stimulant likely activates stimulant-specific subsets of neurons with a stimulant-specific response profile through the sensory pathway from the sensory organ to the primary sensory cortex, resulting in identical sensory perception of the stimulant. At different stages of this neuronal information processing, the redundancy in sensory information changes by summing or subtracting overlapping signals from cognate and noncognate receptors for common and unique elements. The sensory systems generate oscillatory activities between related cortical regions and the thalamus, except in the olfactory system. The olfactory system generates oscillatory activities in the first and second olfactory centers, the olfactory bulb, and the anterior piriform cortex (aPC). It is significantly more difficult to quantify the degree of similarity or difference in these transient oscillatory responses compared to stationary oscillatory activities. We previously developed a wavelet correlation analysis that is phase-tolerant for transient oscillatory responses and demonstrated a stimulus dependency of the odor-evoked oscillatory brain waves (oscillatory local field potentials, osci-LFPs) in the aPC output layer and an experience dependency in the input layer [1]. These results suggest that the redundancy in the neural representation of olfactory information may change in the aPC.

Sensory systems are incorporated in higher brain functions that synergistically control animal behaviors through multiple neural systems including sensory, memory, decision, motor, or other systems. Generally, all neural systems would maintain the reliability of signal processing in identical activities of identical subsets of neurons in identical time courses through neural pathways with acceptable across-trial variability. This suggests that brain waves in identical behaviors could be, to some extent, reproduced in each brain. Small fluctuations, however, sometimes change oscillatory phases across trials, as has been observed in odor-induced oscillatory brain waves [1]. The fine temporal structures of phase-fluctuated oscillatory activities responsible for informational differences are easily lost by averaging several brain waves, even for identical information in each brain. Associations of single-trial brain waves with in-brain information have been rarely studied. Regarding mental states, the most important individual-independent frequencies of electroencephalography (EEG) are 7–12 Hz at the P1 electrode and <5 Hz at Fz for attention, 10–20 Hz at F4 for fatigue, and 4–7 Hz at Fz and 10–20 Hz at Cz for frustration, with even greater variations in frequencies observed across individuals [2]. Alpha-band oscillations (8–13 Hz) exert top-down influences on the early visual processing for attention orienting [3] and are sensitive markers in the auditory memory loading process [4]. As a test case, we applied a wavelet correlation analysis to estimate odor information in the fine temporal structures of single-trial brain waves.

Advertisement

2. Wavelet correlation analysis

2.1. Characteristics of odor-evoked oscillatory brain waves in the aPC

Odor-evoked oscillatory brain waves in the aPC are not stationary over the time window of interest, even in an ex vivo isolated whole brain with attached nose preparation under the condition of no inputs from the nonolfactory sensory systems (Figure 1) [1, 5]. Oscillatory brain waves initiate during the 1-s odor presentation before the peak of the receptor potential, the electro-olfactogram (EOG) (the lowest trace in Figure 1) [1]. A pair of quite different odors, lavender essential oil (Lav), and a mixture of three fatty acids—mc4 + mc6 + mc8 (mc468)—were selected as plant- and animal-related odors, respectively. Linalool (Lina) and n-butanoic acid (mc4) were selected as the single-compound odors of Lav and mc468, respectively, with partial overlaps of the activated olfactory receptors and their respective signal pathways with their original mixtures as well as 0.1 Lav (10-fold diluted Lav). As expected, oscillatory brain waves of a pair of quite different Lav and mc468 odors look dissimilar in the initial phase but are partially similar in the late phase.

Figure 1.

Odor-evoked oscillatory brain waves in layer I of the anterior piriform cortex (aPC) [1]. Time courses of low-pass-filtered (045 Hz) oscillatory brain waves and the receptor potential (electro-olfactogram, EOG) at the centromedial or caudocentral** site of the aPC in the isolated whole brain are shown for three odors (Lav, lavender essential oil as an odor from a plant; 0.1 Lav (10-fold diluted Lav); and mc468, a mixture of three fatty acids as an imitated odor from animals). Ringer solution (RN) was used as a control. The odor or RN was presented to the nose of the isolated brain for 1 or 4 s* (only for the sixth Lav), as indicated by the horizontal bar in the in-presentation order for each odor (entire presentation order). The responses in the 2.5-s time window* of interest were analyzed.

The correlations of the temporal profiles of oscillatory brain waves in the aPC for a 2.5-s time window, which comprised the 1-s odor presentation and the following 1.5 s, were not homogeneously high between identical odors (Figure 2A) [1]. Only a few identical odor pairs for Lav or 0.1 Lav demonstrated relatively high correlations (0.7–0.74), whereas the remaining pairs demonstrated intermediate (0.47–0.69) or low (0.29) correlations. These low correlations are attributable to the independent fluctuations in the oscillatory phase angles and powers including a few synchronous cycles (indicated by the daggers), in the fast Fourier transform (FFT) components even between identical odors, indicating that oscillatory responses are not strictly phase-locked to the stimulus onset (Figure 3) [1]. The spurious high correlations of the 0–45 Hz components are attributable to the similarities in the temporal profiles of the 0–2 Hz components [1]. The 0–2 Hz component resulted in high correlations (>0.77) for all the Lav and 0.1-Lav pairs (Figure 2B), whereas the 2–45 Hz components resulted in low correlations (<0.4) for all pairs (Figure 2C). To address these weaknesses of the conventional analyses, we tested a novel correlation analysis of wavelet profiles.

Figure 2.

Correlation matrices among odor-evoked oscillatory brain waves in layer I of the aPC [1]. (A) Matrix of cross-/autocorrelations of the 0–45 Hz components of the odor-evoked oscillatory brain waves in the 2.5-s time window* of interest (shown in Figure 1). Some of the identical odor pairs produced high correlations >0.7. Identical odors are grouped in the order of stimulus presentation. (B) Cross-/autocorrelation matrix of the 0–2 Hz components of the odor-evoked oscillatory brain waves. (C) Cross/autocorrelation matrix of the 2–45 Hz components of the odor-evoked oscillatory brain waves. By omitting the 0–2 Hz component, all correlations were reduced to <0.4. (D) the matrix in B rearranged in the entire presentation order did not demonstrate an approach of the high correlations of the 0–2 Hz components to the diagonal line (between the dashed lines). The color represents the respective amplitude range of the cross-correlations: black, <0.60; green, 0.60–0.69; pink, 0.70–0.79; red, 0.80–0.89; orange, 0.90–0.99; and white, 1.00.

Figure 3.

The oscillatory phases of the odor-evoked oscillatory brain waves differed between identical stimuli [1]. The 0–45 Hz and six frequency band components of the odor-evoked oscillatory brain waves were obtained by using an FFT bandpass filter. The two responses in the left and middle columns were superimposed on the respective frequency bands in the right column, indicating the trial-by-trial oscillatory phase differences and their fluctuations. The phase-matching points are indicated by the daggers.

2.2. Wavelet correlation analysis procedure for oscillatory brain waves in the time window of interest

Figure 4 shows the procedure for the wavelet transformation and its conversion to a data array for the wavelet correlation analysis [1]. The wavelet time-frequency power profiles enable us to quantify the similarity of the odor-evoked oscillatory brain waves. The wavelet transform is like a running, windowed Fourier transform; it uses a certain window size and slides it along in time, computing the FFT at each time using only the data within the window. The original wavelet software libraries were provided by Torrence and Compo [6] and modified with respect to the following points. Because of the spurious high correlations in the low-frequency band, all 0–2 Hz components were removed prior to the phase-tolerant analysis of the 2–45 Hz components of the oscillatory brain waves. The 2–45 Hz bandpass-filtered brain waves (Figure 4A) were subjected to a Morlet wavelet analysis by using the following equations:

Figure 4.

Wavelet transformation and wavelet cross-correlation profile of an oscillatory response [1]. (A) The 2–45 Hz component of a single-trial 1-s odor-evoked oscillatory brain wave (oscillatory local field potentials, osci-LFPs) in the anterior piriform cortex in an isolated guinea-pig whole brain (second presentation of lavender odor, indicated by the bold bar). (B) A Morlet wavelet time-frequency power spectrum of the second Lav-evoked oscillatory brain wave. Subsequently, seven sets of 2048-point wavelet transformations of the oscillatory brain waves were computed. (C) A columnar array of wavelet cross-/autocorrelations of the second Lav-evoked response. One of the responses for the 2.5-s time window at nine representative frequencies and sets of logarithmic ratios of the cross-correlation to the autocorrelation between wavelet pairs of the second Lav-evoked response (target) were serially concatenated into a data array, in which the wavelet correlations were calculated as correlation coefficients.

Wns=n=0N1xn'Ψn'nδtsE1
Ψ0η=π1/4eiω0ηeη2/2E2
ωj=ω0+2+ω024πsjE3
sj=so2jδjj=01JE4
J=δj1lnNδts0E5

where (*) indicates the complex conjugate, ω0 = 6, N = 2048, δt = 0.001, s0 = 2δt, and δj = 0.1. The wavelet power spectrum, Wns2, was plotted in the 1.89–42.78 Hz frequency (ωj) range (Figure 4B) [1]. To avoid the frequency-dependent errors that increase at the edges of epochs, the 8192 data points (213 sequential points at the 1000 Hz sampling rate) were divided into seven epochs of 2048 (211) data points (2048 ms, centered every 1024 data points to the 7336th data point) with a 50% overlap and subjected to wavelet transformations (Figure 4B) [1]. Around the edge of each epoch, the time series was padded with the actual data (s ≥ 0) or zeros (s < 0). To reconstruct a continuous wavelet transform from 0 to 8191 ms, the middle two quarters of each epoch of seven wavelets were combined (Figure 4B) [1]. Compared to the average wavelet power of the pre-stimulus period (10–2057 ms, marked with double asterisks in Figure 4A), the wavelet power in the regions within the black lines was highly significant (P < 0.0001, chi-squared test, Figures 4B and 5) across all recordings from the same preparation at each frequency [1].

Figure 5.

The wavelet profiles of odor-evoked oscillatory brain waves differed between the input and output layers of the aPC [1]. Of the 21 pairs of 1-s odor-evoked oscillatory brain waves (upper traces) that were simultaneously recorded in layers I (input) or III (output) of the aPC, 10 pairs are represented. In the wavelet time-frequency power profiles (lower traces) for the 2.2-s time window (marked by the asterisk), the ~10 Hz components remained prominent in layer III, whereas the <8 Hz components became less prominent compared to those in layer I. The in-stimulant presentation order is indicated. Statistically significant oscillatory powers were located within the black lines compared to those before presentation of odors (P < 0.0001, chi-squared test).

We calculated correlation coefficients between logarithmic ratio arrays of the cross-correlations to the autocorrelations of the wavelet power profile for the time window of interest at the following nine representative frequencies (selected from the calculated wavelet frequencies) to quantify the similarities of the wavelet time-frequency power profiles between identical and different odors:

  1.  Delta (2–4 Hz): 3.78 Hz.

  2.  Theta (4–8 Hz): 7.56 Hz.

  3.  Alpha (8–13 Hz): 10.7 Hz for the dominant oscillation and 12.29 Hz.

  4.  Low beta (13–20 Hz): 15.13 Hz.

  5.  High beta (20–30 Hz): 21.39 and 26.33 Hz.

  6.  Gamma (30–45 Hz): 30.25 and 34.75 Hz.

The cross-correlation was calculated as the sum of the products of the wavelet power for a pair comprising the target response (Wtsfi) and one of the other responses (Wnsfi) at the representative frequencies (fi) for T1 [ms] ≤ s ≤ T2 [ms]. In a similar manner, the nine sums of the squared wavelet power for the target response were used to calculate the autocorrelation. Moreover, the logarithms of the ratios [Rn(fi)] of the cross-correlations to the autocorrelations at the representative frequencies (fi) were used to equalize the contributions of the increases and decreases in the response amplitude to the correlation analysis:

Rnfi=s=T1T2WnsfiWtsfis=T1T2WtsfiWtsfiE6

A serially concatenated columnar array of all sets of the nine logarithmic ratios of the cross-correlations to the autocorrelations of the target response in the identical order of responses is a form of a wavelet cross-correlation profile (Figure 4C) [1]. The wavelet correlations were calculated as the correlation coefficients between these columnar arrays and employed to quantify the similarities of the odor-evoked oscillatory brain waves in the aPC.

Other mother wavelets such as Meyer and Mexican hat were considered to be inadequate for application to the odor-evoked oscillatory brain waves because their shapes appeared more dissimilar to any FFT components of the oscillatory brain waves than that of the Morlet (Figure 3). To date, except for one case [1], there are no published results of quantifying the similarities between oscillatory brain waves. Regarding the time-frequency power profiles, three reports were found. In one study, a discrete wavelet transform was used to identify and compare the timings of spike trains in an insect antennal lobe (corresponding to the mammal olfactory bulb) [7]. In another study, the Morlet wavelet transform was used to identify dominant oscillatory frequency bands and the synchrony between the oscillatory brain waves in different olfactory regions [8]. In the third study, the Hilbert transform was used to identify the dominant oscillations of the odor-evoked responses in the theta band in the posterior piriform cortex with phase-locked activities in the hippocampus in humans [9]. The Hilbert transform produced similar oscillation powers in a wide frequency range of 60–140 Hz, which is inconsistent with the decreased powers of the Morlet wavelet. Considering these results, we did not intend to analyze the odor-evoked oscillatory brain waves with the Meyer or Mexican hat mother wavelets or the Hilbert transform.

2.3. Wavelet correlation analysis of the time-frequency power profiles for revealing the stimulus dependency of odor-evoked oscillatory brain waves

The wavelet correlation analysis revealed that the olfactory information redundancy of a neural representation changes from experience (high redundancy) to a stimulus dependency (low redundancy) in the aPC [1]. The origins of the activities in layer I of the aPC are mainly the afferent fibers (input), association fibers, and postsynaptic inhibitory feedback input, whereas the activities in layer III primarily originate from the responses (output) of pyramidal cells, which are the principal neurons in the aPC and receive signals from multiple ORs. The wavelet profiles of identical odors resembled each other more than they resembled those of different odors in layers I (input signals) and III (output signals) of the aPC (Figure 5) [1]. In addition, the wavelet transformation visualized moderately clustered spot-like transient reductions in oscillatory power at frequencies just above 10 Hz in the odor-evoked oscillatory brain waves in layer I of the aPC (Figure 5). The most characteristic odor-dependent differences appeared in the initial phase of the wavelets for odor-evoked oscillatory brain waves in layer I of aPC. The mc468-evoked oscillatory brain wave was markedly greater especially at low frequencies in the initial phase than that of the Lav-evoked response [1].

The array data of the logarithmic ratios of the wavelet cross-/autocorrelations between 21 odor-evoked oscillatory brain waves differed slightly between layers I and III of the aPC (Figure 6) [1]. The lengths of the bars reflect the differences between a pair of oscillatory brain waves in such a way that the values of +1, 0, and −1 represent cross-correlations that are 10-fold, equal to, and one-tenth of the autocorrelation at the respective frequencies.

Figure 6.

The wavelet cross-correlation profiles of odor-evoked oscillatory brain waves slightly differed between the input and output layers of the aPC [1]. The five pairs of logarithmic ratio arrays of the wavelet cross-/autocorrelations are exemplified. These ratio arrays suggest that the mc468-evoked responses markedly differed from those of Lav or Lina in each layer of the aPC and that they slightly differed between the input and output layers.

In layer III, the Lav odor pairs (broken yellow square in Figure 7C) showed homogeneously high correlations, except for the ninth Lav, whereas the identical Lav pairs in layer I resulted in more heterogeneous correlations (Figure 7A) [1]. In addition, the correlations between different single-component odors (Lina and mc4, in the broken blue squares in Figure 7C) decreased to <0.6 in layer III, whereas the corresponding correlations in layer I were mostly greater than 0.6 (Figure 7A) [1]. Notably, the heterogeneous correlations changed into an experience-dependent response similarity, which was observed for some of the odors in layer I of the aPC (a cluster of high correlations between the dashed lines in Figure 7B vs. 7A) but was not clearly observed in layer III (Figure 7D vs. 7A) as well as the 0–2 Hz components in layer I (Figure 2D) [1]. In layer III, the <8 Hz components decreased relative to those in layer I, with the prominent ~10 Hz oscillation remaining [1]. These results indicate a change in the neuronal information redundancy of transient and oscillatory brain waves from the dependencies on stimulus experience (high redundancy) to stimulus quality (low redundancy) between the input and output layers of the aPC. Recently, in the olfactory bulb that is upstream of the aPC in the olfactory pathway, stimulus history-dependent odor processing was observed [10]. This means that the wavelet correlation analysis had revealed a consistent experience dependency in input signals in the aPC from the olfactory bulb.

Figure 7.

The wavelet correlation matrices of oscillatory brain waves differed between the input and output signals in the aPC [1]. (A) The wavelet correlation matrix of oscillatory brain waves in layer I (input) of the aPC. (B) The matrix in A rearranged in the entire presentation order. High correlations approached the diagonal line. (C) The wavelet correlation matrix of osci-LFPs in layer III (output) of the aPC. (D) The matrix in C rearranged in the entire presentation order. The colors representing power magnitudes are the same as in Figure 2.

2.4. Effects of changes in oscillatory components on the wavelet correlation analysis

We evaluated the ability of the wavelet correlation analysis to detect changes in oscillatory powers at specific frequencies by 0.2-fold step modified wavelet powers at 1–8 frequency bands (Figure 8) [1]. Greater decreases in correlations (0.4–0.7) were observed as a result of the 0.2-fold power modification at only 1–2 frequencies than those of eight frequencies (number/9 given in parentheses on the Y-axis). For 0.2-fold power amplification, the largest and smallest decreases were observed at 8–13 and 4–8 Hz, respectively. This analysis revealed that in the aPC, the 8–13 Hz component of the oscillatory brain waves contributes to the correlation coefficients more than the 4–8 Hz component. The wavelet correlation analysis enables the estimation of the relative contributions of oscillatory components to the similarities and differences between oscillatory brain waves.

Figure 8.

Sensitivity of the wavelet correlation analysis to changes in the oscillatory components [1]. A 0.2-fold power amplification resulted in the largest and smallest decreases in the wavelet correlations for 8–13 and 4–8 Hz, respectively. As the number of power-modified frequencies increased to more than four, changes in the wavelet correlations were reduced.

Advertisement

3. Method for estimating in-brain information

3.1. Ranking of the correlation coefficients of several brain waves for identical information

Here, the odor-evoked brain waves were the same as those used in the previous section. To estimate the in-brain information, two standard brain waves, covering a wide range of variations for identical information, were selected. The criteria for selecting the two standard brain waves were as follows: (i) a brain wave with the highest pairwise correlation coefficient and a high average of pairwise correlation coefficients in the given information for each individual and (ii) a brain wave with the second highest pairwise correlation coefficient and a differently ranked average of pairwise correlation coefficients in the given information for the same individual.

To select standard brain waves for the four odors, the correlation coefficients in the 2.2-s time window of interest were ranked between single-trial brain waves for all possible pairs of identical odors. Among the 28 pairs of brain waves for Lav, the highest correlation was obtained for the second Lav and fourth Lav pair that provided the fourth (median) and second highest averages of pairwise correlation coefficients, respectively (Table 1). The second highest correlation coefficient was obtained for the third and fifth Lav brain wave pair that provided the seventh and third highest averages of pairwise correlation coefficients, respectively. On the basis of the criteria, the fourth and third Lav brain waves were selected as the two standard brain waves for Lav information.

Ranking of wavelet correlationsStandard set
LavFirst LavSecond LavThird LavFourth LavSixth LavSeventh LavEighth LavNinth LavCorr. coeff. RankAve. corr. Coeff.Ave. rankMemo.11-m11-m1p11-mp22-m2ps1s1 m1s2
First
Lav
1.000.639950.260.590.440.590.600.3160.556
Second Lav0.641.000.600.730.600.520.470.1110.594median
Third
Lav
0.260.601.000.470.6830.280.540.3820.537
Fourth Lav0.590.730.471.000.680.640.500.2110.602
Sixth
Lav
0.440.600.6830.6821.000.550.510.3020.603
Seventh Lav0.590.520.280.639980.551.000.590.4150.575
Eighth
Lav
0.600.470.540.500.510.591.000.6640.611
Ninth
Lav
0.310.110.380.210.300.410.661.0040.428

Table 1.

Pairwise wavelet correlations of single-trial brain waves for Lav in layer III of the aPC, their ranking, and various sets of standard brain waves.

With regard to the pairwise correlation coefficients, their values for Lav pairs tended to be greater than those for mc4 pairs, and the values for Lina pairs tended to be greater than those for mc468 pairs. The lower correlation coefficients between identical odors suggest a greater across-trial variability in the time-frequency power profiles of single-trial brain waves, despite the tolerance of oscillatory phase differences. Similarly, the first and third Lina brain waves (Table 2), the fourth and first mc4 brain waves (Table 3), and the third and first mc468 brain waves (Table 4) were selected as standard brain waves for the respective information. These eight standard brain waves, as well as a control brain wave evoked by an odorless Ringer solution (second RN), were used as Set 1 of standard brain waves.

Ranking of wavelet correlationsStandard set
LinaFirst LinaSecond LinaThird LinaFourth LinaCorr. coeff. RankAve. corr. Coeff.Ave. rankMemo.11-m11-m1p11-mp22-m2ps1s1 m1s2
First Lina1.000.490.220.0410.441
Second Lina0.491.00−0.130.1810.383Median
Third Lina0.22−0.131.000.3420.364
fourth Lina0.040.180.341.0020.392

Table 2.

Pairwise wavelet correlations of single-trial brain waves for Lina in layer III of the aPC, their ranking, and various sets of standard brain waves.

Ranking of wavelet correlationsStandard set
mc4First mc4Second mc4Third mc4Fourth mc4Fifth mc4Corr. coeff. RankAve. corr. Coeff.Ave. rankMemo.11-m11-m1p11-mp22-m2ps1s1 m1s2
First mc41.000.040.4670.350.1520.405
Second mc40.041.000.250.3660.36840.404
Third mc40.4670.251.000.460.1820.472
Fourth mc40.350.370.461.000.47210.531
Fifth mc40.150.370.180.4721.0010.433Median

Table 3.

Pairwise wavelet correlations of single-trial brain waves for mc4 in layer III of the aPC, their ranking, and various sets of standard brain waves.

Ranking of wavelet correlationsStandard set
mc468First mc468Third mc468Fourth mc468Corr. coeff. RankAve. corr. Coeff.Ave. rankMemo.11-m11-m1p11-mp22-m2ps1s1 m1s2
First mc4681.000.140.0520.393
Third mc4680.141.000.2310.461
Fourth mc4680.050.231.0010.432Median

Table 4.

Pairwise wavelet correlations of single-trial brain waves for mc468 in layer III of the aPC and various sets of standard brain waves.

3.2. Estimates of the most probable information for single-trial brain waves using a pair of standard brain waves for each item of information

Using the wavelet correlation analysis, all possible pairwise correlation coefficients between a given single-trial brain wave and each standard brain wave (Set 1) were calculated. The first candidate was selected as the standard brain wave with the highest correlation coefficient to a target single-trial brain wave. The wavelet correlation analysis provided the first candidates for 12 single-trial brain waves with an accuracy of 75% (Table 5). An accuracy of 100% was achieved for Lina (2/2) and mc468 (1/1), whereas an accuracy of 67% was achieved for Lav (4/6) and mc4 (2/3). Notably, the single-trial brain waves tested were not any of the Set 1 standard brain waves. The accuracy of the first candidates was more than threefold higher than chance in five cases (20%). The probability of including the correct information for the two upper candidates was 92% (Table 5). However, the third candidates did not improve the probability of including the correct information for the three upper candidates (92%). In the estimates of information, candidates with correlation coefficients <0.6 were disregarded as nonspecific ones.

Standard brain wavesThird LavFourth LavFirst LinaThird LinaFirst mc468Third mc468First mc4Fourth mc4Second RNHighest corr.
Third Lav1.000.670.850.660.630.500.440.360.45Lina
Fourth Lav0.671.000.580.610.700.540.280.540.41mc468
First Lina0.850.581.000.600.580.400.410.300.46Lav
Third Lina0.660.610.601.000.630.720.540.580.41mc468
First mc4680.630.700.580.631.000.7280.630.7230.40mc468
Third mc4680.500.540.400.720.731.000.500.630.52mc468
First mc40.440.280.410.540.630.501.000.730.35mc4
Fourth mc40.360.540.300.580.720.630.731.000.37mc4
Second RN0.450.410.460.410.400.520.350.371.00
Single-trial brain wavesEstimated informationSecond candidate (>0.6)Third candidate (>0.6)
First Lav0.560.770.470.480.530.470.390.590.49Lav
Second Lav0.690.820.620.560.510.430.260.410.58LavLav
Sixth Lav0.7740.780.7660.790.690.510.400.460.39LinaLavLav
Seventh Lav0.530.790.420.650.710.630.500.750.51Lavmc4mc468
Eighth Lav0.6410.6930.520.6890.680.560.630.630.33LavLinamc468
Ninth Lav0.510.430.460.610.730.510.740.720.20mc4mc468mc4
Second Lina0.710.440.790.530.560.290.480.290.28LinaLav
Fourth Lina0.6520.560.6540.790.710.630.570.470.24Linamc468Lina
Fourth mc4680.580.560.540.7750.7770.860.600.630.33mc468mc468Lina
Second mc40.360.440.230.600.680.840.580.800.51mc468mc4mc468
Third mc40.350.350.250.550.660.610.810.850.34mc4mc4mc468
Fifth mc40.360.450.250.570.540.550.680.810.41mc4mc4
Correct rate75%92%92%

Table 5.

Estimated information of single-trial brain waves in layer III of the aPC by ranking of wavelet correlations using two standard brain waves (set 1).

To compare the ideal set of standard brain waves (Set 1) with different sets of standard brain waves (standard Set 1-m) in terms of their accuracies for estimating information, wavelet correlation analyses were performed with partial replacements of standard brain waves. When one or three of the nine Set 1 standard brain waves were replaced with brain waves that did not meet the criteria, there were no changes in the 75% accuracy for the first candidates, and a 92% probability of including the correct information for the two upper candidates was observed. Nevertheless, there were some exchanges between correct and incorrect estimates for identical information (data not shown).

In contrast, by using the pair of brain waves with the highest pairwise correlation coefficients as the two standard brain waves for each odor (standard Set 2), the accuracies of estimation were reduced by 100% for Lina (2/2 → 0/2) and 34% for Lav (4/6 → 2/6), but no change occurred for mc468 (1/1) and mc4 (2/3) (Table 6). This standard Set 2 provided a total accuracy of 42% (33% reduction) and a 75% probability (17% reduction) of including the correct information for the two upper candidates (Figure 9). By replacing two of the nine Set 2 standard brain waves with one that did not meet the criteria, the accuracy for the first candidates increased by 25% and the 92% probability of including the correct information for the two upper candidates was recovered (Figure 9). Therefore, the proposed criteria of selecting standard brain waves with a wide variation are likely appropriate and achieve better estimation than the selection of those with a narrow range (the most similar brain wave pairs).

Standard brain wavesSecond LavFourth LavFirst LinaSecond LinaThird mc468Fourth mc468Fourth mc4Fifth mc4Second RNHighest corr.
Second Lav1.000.800.570.470.400.370.350.390.55Lav
Fourth Lav0.801.000.470.370.500.540.500.460.35Lav
First Lina0.570.471.000.800.380.560.280.240.42Lina
Second Lina0.470.370.801.000.280.490.290.290.27Lina
Third mc4680.400.500.380.281.000.850.540.550.48mc468
Fourth mc4680.370.540.560.490.851.000.580.530.29mc468
Fourth mc40.350.500.280.290.540.581.000.830.27mc4
Fifth mc40.390.460.240.290.550.530.831.000.38mc4
Second RN0.550.350.420.270.480.290.270.381.00
Single-trial brain wavesEstimated informationSecond candidate (>0.6)Third candidate (>0.6)
First Lav0.780.760.420.360.440.410.580.700.43LavLavmc4
Third Lav0.610.580.870.760.490.620.350.360.41LinaLinamc468
Sixth Lav0.650.730.700.740.560.710.410.520.32LinaLavmc468
Seventh Lav0.620.740.360.400.570.630.720.650.41Lavmc4mc4
Eighth Lav0.610.680.470.520.570.630.650.720.33mc4Lavmc4
Ninth Lav0.270.400.420.420.470.600.750.700.18mc4mc4mc468
Third Lina0.520.570.550.540.690.770.540.600.40mc468mc468mc4
Fourth Lina0.340.500.620.700.600.840.440.400.18mc468LinaLina
First mc4680.500.660.560.550.710.790.660.550.35mc468mc468mc4
First mc40.170.220.370.450.450.570.750.680.27mc4mc4
Second mc40.260.420.210.150.830.700.710.720.48mc468mc4mc4
Third mc40.190.320.240.280.600.630.850.770.28mc4mc4mc468
Correct rate42%75%75%

Table 6.

Estimated information of single-trial brain waves in layer III of the aPC by ranking of wavelet correlations using two standard brain waves with the highest pairwise correlation coefficients (set 2).

Figure 9.

Variation-dependent changes in the accuracy of estimated information of single-trial brain waves in layer III of the aPC.

3.3. Estimates of the most probable information for single-trial brain waves with a standard brain wave for each item of information

By using a set of single standard brain waves for four odors that met only the first criterion (standard Set s1), a similar accuracy of estimated information and probability of including the correct information for the two upper candidates was obtained for the 12 target brain waves (data not shown). The Set s1 standard brain waves were composed of the fourth Lav, first Lina, third mc468, fourth mc4, and second RN. Among the 16 target brain waves, the accuracy and probability slightly decreased by 6 and 4%, respectively, compared to those of the 12 target brain waves (data not shown). When one or two of the five Set-s1 standard brain waves were replaced with those that did not meet the criteria, the accuracy was reduced to 67 or 42%, respectively (data not shown). The probability of including the correct information for the two upper candidates was also reduced by 9 and 25%, respectively. For the 16 target brain waves, the accuracy and probability showed almost no changes when one of the five Set s1 standard brain waves was replaced, whereas the accuracy and probability for the estimated information were reduced by 13% when two of the Set s1 standard brain waves were replaced (data not shown).

3.4. Single-trial brain waves composed of redundant signals in the olfactory pathway exhibiting a similar accuracy and probability for estimated information

It is interesting to examine the accuracy of the wavelet correlation analysis for predicting the in-brain information of single-trial brain waves comprising redundant signals in layer I of the aPC. By using a set of standard brain waves that meet the proposed criteria for the redundant brain waves recorded in layer I (standard Set 1r), the wavelet correlation analysis provided a similar accuracy (75%) of estimated information and probability (100%) of including the correct information for the two upper candidates (Table 7) compared to the results observed for the brain waves recorded in layer III (Table 5). In contrast, by using the pairs of brain waves corresponding to the Set 1 of layer III (standard Set 1′ in layer I), the accuracy of estimation was reduced by 17%, and the probability of including the correct information for the two upper candidates was reduced by 25% (to 75%) (data not shown). By using single standard brain waves (standard Set s1r), the accuracy and probability were slightly reduced compared to those of the standard Set s1 (data not shown).

Standard brain wavesSecond LavEighth LavSecond LinaThird LinaFirst mc468Fourth mc468First mc4Fifth mc4Second RNHighest corr.
Second Lav1.000.560.640.730.630.560.590.420.47Lina
Eighth Lav0.561.000.470.500.570.630.660.760.09mc4
Second Lina0.640.471.000.670.850.680.760.490.48mc468
Third Lina0.730.500.671.000.690.750.740.480.25mc468
First mc4680.630.570.850.691.000.790.810.540.35Lav
Fourth mc4680.560.630.680.750.791.000.870.580.23mc4
First mc40.590.660.760.740.810.871.000.690.30mc468
Fifth mc40.420.760.490.480.540.580.691.000.20Lav
Second RN0.470.090.480.250.350.230.300.201.00
Single-trial brain wavesEstimated informationSecond candidate (>0.6)Third candidate (>0.6)
First Lav0.770.700.660.520.590.550.630.700.43LavLavmc4
Third Lav0.790.430.740.600.720.490.510.380.48LavLinamc468
Fourth Lav0.830.680.580.700.660.580.610.530.20LavLinaLav
Sixth Lav0.810.520.790.850.780.730.750.520.37LinaLavmc468
Seventh Lav0.560.910.450.470.530.590.640.760.06Lavmc4mc4
Ninth Lav0.560.830.620.480.680.670.720.760.28Lavmc4mc468
First Lina0.730.440.780.660.690.550.540.380.63LinaLavmc468
Fourth Lina0.590.400.77580.840.850.750.77560.420.27mc468LinaLina
Third mc4680.640.550.770.8370.830.890.8390.510.36mc468mc4Lina
Second mc40.520.620.690.770.840.8910.8870.640.27mc468mc4mc468
Third mc40.530.7790.710.640.7750.7780.840.800.18mc4mc4Lav
Fourth mc40.590.650.570.670.650.790.850.700.21mc4mc468mc4
Correct rate75%100%100%

Table 7.

Estimated information of single-trial brain waves in layer I of the aPC by ranking of wavelet correlations using two standard brain waves (set 1r).

Finally, it was examined whether the combination of data for two recording sites (layers I and III) affected the accuracy for the first candidates. Using this method, the accuracy (75%) of estimated information was maintained but not improved in standard Set 1 + 1′ and Set 1r + 1r’ (data not shown).

3.5. A new method of real-time estimation of in-brain information of single-trial brain waves

A new method is proposed for estimating the information of single-trial brain waves in fine temporal structures with a cross-trial variability by using a set of standard brain waves in a given category for each individual. In the oscillatory brain waves recorded in layer III or I of the aPC of the isolated whole brain of a guinea pig, the wavelet correlation analysis provided a 75% accuracy for the first candidate and a > 92% probability of including the correct information for the two upper candidates (Tables 5 and 7). The results support the validity of the proposed criteria for selecting standard brain waves with a wide variation for estimating different information in a given category.

The accuracy of this method was not affected by the information redundancy of signal sources, such as those resulting from olfactory receptors with overlapping tuning specificities and an experience dependency in layer I or from pyramidal cells with a stimulus dependency after the integration of signals from multiple cognate olfactory receptors in layer III (Table 8). Layer I brain waves comprising redundant signals exhibited a similar accuracy of estimated information and a slightly increased probability of including the correct information for the two upper candidates compared to layer III brain waves.

InformationRecoding sitesEstimated information
LavLinamc468mc4
LavLayer I (input)62.8%20.9% (e)9.3% (e)7.0% (e)
Layer III (output)57.9%18.4% (e)1.3% (e)22.4% (e)
LinaLayer I (input)0.0% (e)53.3%46.7% (e)0.0% (e)
Layer III (output)7.7% (e)73.1%19.2% (e)0.0% (e)
mc468Layer I (input)0.0% (e)25.0% (e)75.0%0.0% (e)
Layer III (output)0.0% (e)26.7% (e)73.3%0.0% (e)
mc4Layer I (input)4.5% (e)0.0% (e)13.6% (e)81.8%
Layer III (output)0.0% (e)0.0% (e)30.8% (e)69.2%

Table 8.

Correct and error rates (e) of estimated information in single-trial brain waves recorded in layers I and III of the aPC by the wavelet correlation analysis.

The redundancies of brain waves are attributable to two origins: information and signaling. In the olfactory system, the information redundancy changes through the signal pathway from the receptors to the higher cortical areas via signal integration in the third- or higher-order neurons and/or mutual inhibition [1, 1113] for category [14] or elemental odor representation [15]. Unlike the >80% overlap of about 70 receptors for carvone enantiomers having similar odors [16], the quite different odors of Lav and mc468 evoked different amplitude receptor potentials in the olfactory epithelium and dissimilar brain waves in the anterior piriform cortex [1]. Nevertheless, the wavelet correlation analysis sometimes produced the highest correlation coefficients of Lav for mc468. The error rate of Lav for mc468 was 9.3% in layer I brain waves but was reduced to 1.7% in layer III brain waves (Table 8 and Figure 10), which is consistent with the change in the information redundancy from high to low stages between layers I and III. On the other hand, the error rate of mc468 for Lav was 0% in both layers I and III. For the single-compound odors, Lina and mc4 exhibited odor similarity-dependent changes in the error rates of the estimated information between layers I and III. The error rates of the single compounds for their original mixture odors (partially similar odor) increased between layers I and III (0 → 7.7% in Lina and 13.6 → 30.8% in mc4) and those of single compounds for their nonrelative mixture odors (dissimilar odor) decreased between layers I and III (46.7 → 19.2% in Lina and 4.5 → 0% in mc4). Notably, the error rates between these single compounds were 0% in both layers I and III. These results suggest a partial overlap of the elemental odors that are represented in the pyramidal cells in the aPC and are recorded in layer III as brain waves. The total error rates of Lina decreased in layer III compared to those of layer I (and vice versa for the correct rate), whereas those of mc4 increased.

Figure 10.

Correct and error rates of estimated information in single-trial brain waves recorded in layers I and III of the aPC by the wavelet correlation analysis. These values are listed in Table 8.

The signaling redundancy originates from an identical temporal profile of different subsets of neurons tuned to distinct or shared information or from identical temporal profiles that are composed of multiple different profiles of various different subsets of neurons tuned to multiple distinct or shared information. The constant error rates of mc468 for Lina between layers I and III (both ~25%, Table 8 and Figure 10) are likely attributable to the signaling redundancy rather than the information similarity or information redundancy. Moreover, in the increased case, there was a threefold higher error rate of Lav for mc4 in layer III than layer I, whereas the error rates of Lav for Lina were almost constant between layers I and III.

3.6. Applicable examples of estimated in-brain information in humans using the wavelet correlation analysis

Each brain system (e.g., a sensory, memory, decision, or motor system) is organized in a hierarchical manner from simple to complicated matters. The sensory system generates oscillatory activities between the related cortical regions and the thalamus, and the latter acts (except in the olfactory system) to gate the sensory input to the cortex and provides feedback from the cortical pyramidal neurons. In olfaction, transient oscillatory brain waves are observed in the aPC [5, 1721]. Strong feed-forward inhibition [5, 22, 23] via the sensitive pathway from the olfactory bulb [24] and the other sensory thalamocortical circuit [25, 26] or higher olfactory centers [27] could induce oscillatory brain waves that would contribute to parts of the EEGs recorded at the respective positions on the human scalp, in analogy to these experimental animals. Such information-dependent temporal profiles of the EEGs may enable us to estimate in-brain information by comparison with a set of standard time-frequency power profiles of EEGs in each individual. To this aim, a wavelet correlation analysis of the brain waves in a guinea pig was conducted using standard brain waves with the proposed criteria and achieved an accuracy of 75% for the first candidates. This accuracy is attributable to the comparisons with standard single-trial responses in the wavelet time-frequency power profiles.

Conventional methods have focused only on some parts of the brain wave characteristics. For example, the FFT power spectra of sensorimotor EEGs [28, 29] or auditory EEGs [30] in specific frequency bands at a specific recording position were analyzed for the development of brain-computer interfaces. The Morlet wavelet convolutions for four-frequency band powers of the single-trial EEGs were analyzed to understand the cognitive control system via a priori estimation of information across three tasks [31]. By using the wavelet correlation analysis in the time-frequency power profiles at nine frequencies, these analyses could be improved in their subprocesses. Odor sensation [32, 33] and color-opponent responses [34] were also recorded in humans at Fz and an intermediate position between Oz and the inion, respectively, and they demonstrated informational differences in response amplitudes or profiles. Like EEGs in object recognition and those responsible for mental states, these EEGs are also subjects for the application of the wavelet correlation analysis for estimating in-brain fine information. Pain-related alpha-band desynchronization at contralateral-central electrodes (C2, C4, CP2, and CP4) and gamma-band synchronization at the ipsilateral-posterior electrodes (P3, P5, and so on) [35] are also good candidates for application. In animal models, the neural pathways of innate and learned fear responses have been revealed [36], and different pathways of stress relaxation using rose and hinokitiol odors were found [37, 38]. Therefore, determining their differing time-frequency power profiles would enable us to estimate the strengths of stress or relaxation in EEGs in humans. Future studies will focus on programming the wavelet correlation analysis for real-time estimates of in-brain information in humans.

Advertisement

4. Conclusions

We developed a new method for a similarity analysis and real-time estimates of in-brain information in single-trial brain waves by ranking the correlation coefficients in the wavelet correlation analysis. The wavelet correlation analysis with a set of standard brain waves provided the first candidate of estimated information with an accuracy of 75% with a > 92% probability of including the correct information for the two upper candidates, regardless of the information redundancy of signal sources. This method may be also useful for its applications to brain-machine interfaces or medical/research tools.

Advertisement

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Dr. Mutsumi Matsukawa for his contributions to the development of the isolated whole-brain experimental system that enabled the recordings of odor-induced and nonolfactory origin-free brain waves. We are also grateful to Kiyo Murano for writing the computer software for wavelet transformation. This work was supported by grants (T.S.) from METI, Japan, and Grant-in-Aids for Scientific Research (B) #15H02730 (T.S.) from the MEXT, Japan.

Advertisement

Abbreviations

aPCanterior piriform cortex
aPCvrventro-rostral region of the aPC
EEGelectroencephalography
EOGelectro-olfactogram
FFTfast Fourier transform
LFPlocal field potential
LOTlateral olfactory tract
ORolfactory receptor
osci-LFPoscillatory local field potential

References

  1. 1. Sato T, Kajiwara R, Takashima I, Iijima T. A novel method for quantifying similarities between oscillatory neural responses in wavelet time-frequency power profiles. Brain Research. 2016;1636:107-117. DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2016.01.054
  2. 2. Myrden A, Chau T. A passive EEG-BCI for single-trial detection of changes in mental state. IEEE Transactions on Neural Systems and Rehabilitation Engineering. 2017;25:345-356
  3. 3. Doesburg SM, Bedo N, Ward LM. Top-down alpha oscillatory network interactions during visuospatial attention orienting. NeuroImage. 2016;132:512-519. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.02.076
  4. 4. Wilsch A, Obleser J. What works in auditory working memory? A neural oscillations perspective. Brain Research. 2016;1640:193-207. DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2015.10.054
  5. 5. Ishikawa T, Sato T, Shimizu A, de Curtis M, Kakei T, Iijima T. Odour-driven activity in the olfactory cortex of an in vitro isolated guinea-pig whole brain with olfactory epithelium. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2007;97:670‐679
  6. 6. Torrence C, Compo GP. A practical guide to wavelet analysis. Bulletin of the American Meteorological Society. 1998;79(61‐78)
  7. 7. Capurro A, Baroni F, Kuebler LS, Kárpáti Z, Dekker T, Lei H, Hansson BS, Pearce TC, Olsson SB. Temporal features of spike trains in the moth antennal lobe revealed by a comparative time-frequency analysis. PLoS One. 2014;9:e84037. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0084037
  8. 8. Pardo-Bellver C, Martínez-Bellver S, Martínez-García F, Lanuza E, Teruel-Martí V. Synchronized activity in the main and accessory olfactory bulbs and vomeronasal amygdala elicited by chemical signals in freely behaving mice. Scientific Reports. 2017;7:9924. DOI: 10.1038/s41598-017-10089-4
  9. 9. Jiang H, Schuele S, Rosenow J, Zelano C, Parvizi J, Tao JX, Wu S, Gottfried JA. Theta oscillations rapidly convey odor-specific content in human piriform cortex. Neuron. 2017;94:207-219
  10. 10. Vinograd A, Livneh Y, Mizrahi A. History-dependent odor processing in the mouse olfactory bulb. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2017;37:12018-12030
  11. 11. Desmaisons D, Vincent JD, Lledo P-M. Control of action potential timing by intrinsic subthreshold oscillations in olfactory bulb output neurons. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1999;19:10727-10737
  12. 12. Kashiwadani H, Sasaki YF, Uchida N, Mori K. Synchronized oscillatory discharges of mitral/tufted cells with different molecular receptive ranges in the rabbit olfactory bulb. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1999;82:1786-1792
  13. 13. Stettler DD, Axel R. Representations of odor in the piriform cortex. Neuron. 2009;63:854-864. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.09.005
  14. 14. Yoshida I, Mori K. Odorant category profile selectivity of olfactory cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:9105-9114
  15. 15. Sato T, Kawasaki T, Mine S, Matsumura H. Functional role of the C-terminal amphipathic helix 8 of olfactory receptors and other G protein-coupled receptors. International Journal of Molecular Sciences. 2016;17(pii):E1930. DOI: 10.3390/ijms17111930
  16. 16. Hamana H, Hirono J, Kizumi M, Sato T. Sensitivity-dependent hierarchical receptor codes for odours. Chemical Senses. 2003;28:87-104
  17. 17. Bressler SL, Freeman WG. Frequency analysis of olfactory system EEG cat, rabbit, and rat. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 1980;50:19-24
  18. 18. Ketchum KL, Haberly LB. Synaptic events that generate fast oscillations in piriform cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1993;13:3980-3985
  19. 19. de Curtis, M, Biella, G, Forti, M, Panzica, F. Multifocal spontaneous epileptic activity induced by restricted bicuculline ejection in the piriform cortex of the isolated guinea pig brain. Journal of Neurophysiology 1994;71:2463–2476
  20. 20. Chapman CA, Xu Y, Haykin S, Racine RJ. Beta-frequency (15-35 Hz) electroencephalogram activities elicited by toluene and electrical stimulation in the behaving rat. Neuroscience. 1998;86:1307-1319
  21. 21. Chabaud P, Ravel N, Wilson DA, Mouly AM, Vigouroux M, Farget V, Gervais R. Exposure to behaviourally relevant odour reveals differential characteristics in rat central olfactory pathways as studied through oscillatory activities. Chemical Senses. 2000;25:561-573
  22. 22. Sato T, Hirono J, Hamana H, Ishikawa T, Shimizu A, Takashima I, Kajiwara R, Iijima T. Architecture of odour information processing in the olfactory system. Anatomical Science International. 2008;83:195-206. DOI: 10.1111/j.1447-073X.2007.00215.x
  23. 23. Sato T, Kobayakawa R, Kobayakawa K, Emura M, Itohara S, Kizumi M, Hamana H, Tsuboi A, Hirono J. Supersensitive detection and discrimination of enantiomers by dorsal olfactory receptors: Evidence for hierarchical odour coding. Scientific Reports. 2015;5:14073. DOI: 10.1038/srep14073
  24. 24. Igarashi KM, Ieki N, An M, Yamaguchi Y, Nagayama S, Kobayakawa K, Kobayakawa R, Tanifuji M, Sakano H, Chen WR, Mori K. Parallel mitral and tufted cell pathways route distinct odor information to different targets in the olfactory cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:7970-7985. DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0154-12.2012
  25. 25. Bruno RM. Synchrony in sensation. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2011;21:701-708. DOI: 10.1016/j.conb.2011.06.003
  26. 26. Kajiwara R, Tominaga T, Takashima I. Olfactory information converges in the amygdaloid cortex via the piriform and entorhinal cortices: Observations in the guinea pig isolated whole-brain preparation. The European Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;25:3648-3658. DOI: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05610.x
  27. 27. Schoenbaum G, Eichenbaum H. Information coding in the rodent prefrontal cortex. I. Single-neuron activity in orbitofrontal cortex compared with that in pyriform cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1995;74:733-750
  28. 28. Cincotti F, Mattia D, Aloise F, Bufalari S, Astolfi L, De Vico Fallani F, Tocci A, Bianchi L, Marciani MG, Gao S, Millan J, Babiloni F. High-resolution EEG techniques for brain-computer interface applications. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 2008;167:31-42
  29. 29. Cincotti F, Mattia D, Aloise F, Bufalari S, Schalk G, Oriolo G, Cherubini A, Marciani MG, Babiloni F. Non-invasive brain-computer interface system: Towards its application as assistive technology. Brain Research Bulletin. 2008;75:796-803. DOI: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2008.01.007
  30. 30. Nijboer F, Furdea A, Gunst I, Mellinger J, McFarland DJ, Birbaumer N, Kübler A. An auditory brain-computer interface (BCI). Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 2008;167:43-50
  31. 31. Cooper PS, Darriba Á, Karayanidis F, Barceló F. Contextually sensitive power changes across multiple frequency bands underpin cognitive control. NeuroImage. 2016;132:499-511. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.03.010
  32. 32. Tonoike M. Emotional analysis of odors of equivalent sensory intensity. Bulletin Electro-Technical Laboratory. 1984;48:796-808
  33. 33. Tonoike M, Seta N, Maetani T, Koizuka I, Takebayashi M. Measurements of olfactory evoked potentials and event related potentials using odorant stimuli. In: Proceedings of the Twelfth Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society; Nov. 1–4, 1990. New York, NY: IEEE; 1990;2:912-913. DOI: 10.1109/IEMBS.1990.691487
  34. 34. Yamanaka T, Sobagaki H, Nayatani Y. Opponent-colors responses in the visually evoked potential in man. Vision Research. 1973;13:1319-1333
  35. 35. Peng W, Babiloni C, Mao Y, Hu Y. Subjective pain perception mediated by alpha rhythms. Biological Psychology. 2015;109:141-150. DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.05.004
  36. 36. Isosaka T, Matsuo T, Yamaguchi T, Funabiki K, Nakanishi S, Kobayakawa R, Kobayakawa K. Htr2a-expressing cells in the central amygdala control the hierarchy between innate and learned fear. Cell. 2015;163:1153-1164. DOI: 10.1016/j.cell.2015.10.047
  37. 37. Matsukawa M, Imada M, Murakami T, Aizawa S, Sato T. Rose odour can innately counteract predator odour. Brain Research. 2011;1381:117-123. DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.01.053
  38. 38. Murakami T, Matsukawa M, Katsuyama N, Imada M, Aizawa S, Sato T. Stress-related activities induced by predator odor may become indistinguishable by hinokitiol odor. Neuroreport. 2012;23:1071-1076. DOI: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e32835b373b

Written By

Takaaki Sato, Riichi Kajiwara, Ichiro Takashima and Toshio Iijima

Submitted: 05 October 2017 Reviewed: 02 February 2018 Published: 01 March 2018