Open access peer-reviewed chapter

Childhood Exposure to Violence: Looking through a Life-Course Perspective

Written By

Sílvia Fraga, Mariana Amorim and Sara Soares

Submitted: 13 December 2021 Reviewed: 10 January 2022 Published: 09 February 2022

DOI: 10.5772/intechopen.102581

From the Edited Volume

Child Abuse and Neglect

Edited by Michael Fitzgerald

Chapter metrics overview

273 Chapter Downloads

View Full Metrics

Abstract

Childhood is the most important period of development during life course, highly sensitive to external influences and with a profound impact on children’s well-being. During this period, the foundations for every individual’s physical and mental health capacities and attainment are laid, influencing children’s lives throughout adolescence, adulthood and aging. Violence is one of the most traumatic experiences that can impact the healthy development of the child, compromising its growth and future health. Although violence assessment in the scope of a cohort study comprises methodological and ethical challenges, a life-course perspective allows researchers to understand the effects of multiple forms of violence by distinguishing between repetitive violence over time and isolated incidents, the occurrence of violent experiences in different contexts and settings, as well as the interconnection between different experiences of trauma. This chapter aims to demonstrate the importance of a life-course perspective to understand the detrimental relationship between early exposure to violence and worse health in the first years of life.

Keywords

  • violence at home
  • peer violence
  • child’s violence

1. Introduction

Exposure to violence can have different manifestations throughout the life course, but all forms of violent behaviors share common characteristics: (1) the use of control strategies depriving others of safety, freedom, health and, in extreme cases, life; (2) the magnitude of the problem affecting particularly the most vulnerable groups; (3) the potential for intergenerational transmission; and (4) a smashing impact at different levels of influence, namely individuals, families, neighborhoods, communities, and society [1, 2, 3, 4].

Violence can take several forms throughout the life cycle, being an individual’s age is an important determinant of the type of violence that people may experience [5]. Therefore, violence has causes, intents, circumstances, and contributing factors that vary according to each individual life cycle stage. Linking together all these events, experiences and behaviors makes it possible to map out developmental pathways from childhood to later ages [5, 6]. With longitudinal studies, it is possible to study continuity and changes occurring in life as they are central to identifying links between distinct phenomena over the life course and thus to describe social processes that both produce and alter developmental trajectories. Such links are essential to understanding the continuity of the experience and behavior, as well as life-course changes that create new states and circumstances that might be unexpected.

Importantly, this concern with the development process includes a wide range of life outcomes [7]. One of the more prominent themes in life-course research is the identification of factors that put one at risk for adversity in later life.

Additionally, violence is a sensitive topic to research, with specific challenges that are different from those that arise when studying other social or health problems. Usually, monitoring systems restrict the occurrence of interpersonal violence to those who seek hospital care or report their experiences to authorities or social support systems [8]. Therefore, researchers who aim to identify and measure violence besides the tip of the iceberg will need to ask people about violent experiences that occur behind closed doors. Even more challenging is when the goal is to study violence in children, where more delicate ethical issues arise, such as obtaining consent for participation in the research that needed to be provided by their own parents; conducting interviews with or administering tests to the children; and providing information about test results to parents or others outside the research team. The research team faces questions as who is responsible for giving consent for children to participate in research, which is further complicated by the potential adversarial relationship between abusing parent and abused child [9].

Among the methodological challenges that should be addressed are the need to explore the effects of multiple forms of maltreatment and the timing, chronicity, and discontinuity of violence episodes. Some children may live in a continually abusive environment, while others may experience only one incident of brief duration [10]. Thus, longitudinal research allows distinguishing between repetitive violence episodes over time and isolated incidents of violence. Moreover, false reporting may not be discarded when we are assessing violence or abuse experiences. However, evidence shows that false allegations of abuse are much less common than the problem of victims who fail to report abuse, and the widespread false denials and minimization of violence by perpetrators, and in general, abuse is vastly under-reported [11]. Also, many of these experiences, especially those suffered by children and women, may remain hidden since perpetrators have an interest in hampering reports and detection.

This chapter aims to demonstrate the importance of a life-course perspective to understand the detrimental relationship between early exposure to violence and worse health in the first years of life.

Advertisement

2. The early exposure to violence

Violence against children includes all forms of violence whether perpetrated by parents or other caregivers, peers, partners, or strangers. This problem is identified as a public health issue and a human rights concern with high social impact, and potentially devastating and costly consequences [12]. Levels of violence against children are extremely high, and it is estimated that, worldwide, up to 1 billion children aged 2–17 years experienced neglect or were victims of physical, sexual, or emotional violence in their lifetime [13].

Childhood is the most important period of development during life course, highly sensitive to external influences and with a profound impact on children’s lives [14, 15]. During this period, the foundations for every individual’s physical and mental health capacities and attainment are laid, influencing growth, development, and well-being in adolescence and adulthood.

Early life experiences and environments may negatively influence later experiences, opportunities, and health risk factors [16]. Thus, being exposed to violence during childhood and adolescence could be particularly disruptive to normal psychological development when it occurs during these periods [17] and may damage health over time.

The exposure to violent experiences should be approached since the perinatal period.

2.1 Intergenerational effects of maternal exposure to violence

Violence against women may have direct and indirect effects on the children. Intimate partner violence can have significant adverse effects on victims at any time in their life but has special significance during pregnancy because of the added potential harms to the unborn child. The detrimental effects of adverse and negative gestational experiences, including exposure to violence during pregnancy, on many aspects of a child’s development, are well described in the literature [18]. Intimate partner violence during pregnancy is associated with poor health outcomes for the fetus, newborn, and infant up to 1 year postpartum [19, 20]. A study conducted in a public maternity of a general Portuguese university hospital showed that one in 10 women reported physical abuse during pregnancy, and almost half of them reported they had suffered severe acts, such as punching, kicking, bruises, cuts, and/or continuing pain, beaten up, severe contusions, broken bones, head, internal, and/or permanent injury [21]. Also, this study showed that reports of physical abuse during pregnancy were significantly associated with preterm delivery (odds ratio (OR) =3.72; 95% confidence interval (95%CI) between 2.59 and 5.33) [21]. Exposure to violence during pregnancy also increases the risk for antepartum hemorrhage, a condition that can be fatal for the unborn [22, 23], increased fetal morbidity [24], intrauterine growth restriction [23], and low birth weight [19].

Being that the womb is a shared environment between mother and infant, maternal experiences can also affect the developing fetus. On one hand, experiences of abuse, occurring either before and during pregnancy, increase the likelihood of abused women being involved in behaviors that may be detrimental to the fetus, including smoking [25], drug use [26, 27], being overweight [28], stress [29], when compared to unexposed women. On the other hand, increased risk of the health outcomes on the child may occur through different pathways: (1) dysregulation of the locus coeruleus-noradrenergic system through the effects of maternal cortisol on epigenetic modification of genes controlling the development of this system [3031]); (2) disruption of brain development by impairing placental circulation [32]); (3) dysregulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis in the fetus [18, 33]; and (4) triggering of developmental immunotoxicity, through autoimmunity or inflammation of myelomonocytic cells in the brain [34]. Depending on the system affected, several outcomes may emerge on the child’s health. Prenatal stress may have a lasting impact on the child’s behavior, increasing the risk of autism spectrum disorder [35], Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) [36], and worse general intellectual and language functioning [37]. For instance, increased maternal cortisol along with a downregulation of the enzyme 11β-HSD2, which converts cortisol into its inactive form, can lead to changes in behavioral development and make the infant more susceptible to stress later in life [38].

On the other hand, children living in a family where the mother is exposed to violence are frequently abused themselves, and mothers exposed to violence or threats are often insufficient caregivers which could affect the children regardless of whether they have seen the violent act or not [39].

2.2 Violence experiences at home

In 1962, child maltreatment received widespread attention by the medical profession and the general public after Kempe’s publication [40]. Kempe described the battered child syndrome, characterized by the clinical manifestations of serious physical abuse in young children, generally inflicted by a parent or a foster parent. In this chapter, Kempe stated that “physicians, because of their feelings and their difficulty in playing a role that they find hard to assume, may have great reluctance in believing that parents were guilty of abuse” [40].

Violence against children perpetrated by adults within the family is one of the least visible forms of child maltreatment, as much of it takes place in the privacy of domestic life. However, this problem is widely prevalent in all societies [41]. Much physical violence against children is inflicted as a punishment, and it is accepted by parents once it is considered by the prevailing social norms as the correct means of discipline. Corporal punishment of children in the form of hitting, punching, kicking, or beating, is socially and legally accepted in some countries [12], being, therefore, a common form of parental discipline toward their children.

Several factors and conditions have been associated with parental violence, including parent characteristics (i.e., parents’ own experience of child maltreatment, age and educational level, cognitive ability, and personality), child characteristics (i.e., age and sex), and sociodemographic conditions (i.e., household income, number of children in the household) [42, 43, 44]. Low-income and economic hardship strain parents’ mental health, increase the likelihood of family conflict, and reduce interaction among family members in a responsive and nurturing manner, which predict poor child developmental outcomes [45, 46, 47].

Also, parental beliefs and cultural acceptance that corporal punishment is a way to raise and educate their children to contribute to these forms of discipline have not been yet abandoned. Even in wealthy and considered highly developed societies, such as Switzerland, it was estimated that 54.4% of children aged 1–14 suffered forms of corporal punishment at home [48]. In the United States, corporal punishment remains a legal and well-accepted form of disciplining children, with prevalence studies reporting 64–95% of parents use spanking between the ages of 2 and 3 [49]. Worldwide, one in four adults reports having been physically abused as a child by their parents or other caregivers [12], three-quarters of world children aged 2 to 4 are regularly victims of violent discipline by their parents or other caregivers [50], and around six in 10 children aged 2–14 are frequently punished physically [51]. However, a growing number of countries are passing laws prohibiting its use at home. A study conducted in the scope of a Portuguese population-based cohort (the Generation XXI) showed a high prevalence of physical violent discipline [52]. In this study, the parent-child Conflict Tactics Scale (CTSPC) was administered to 4175 children by trained interviewers to report parents’ disciplinary practices. This instrument includes 23 items that allow us to measure three different forms of lifetime parental disciplinary acts: (1) non-violent discipline, characterized by positive practices widely used as alternatives to corporal punishment; (2) psychological aggression, which includes verbal and symbolic acts to cause psychological pain or fear to the child; and (3) physical assault, which comprises the use of corporal punishment that may include acts of physical abuse. An interviewer shows the child a picture card and reads a description, such as “This girl’s father hits her with a belt when she does something wrong. When you do something wrong, does your father hit you with a belt?” If the response is yes, the interviewer shows a second card with the response categories in the form of stacked circles that the child could point to. Answers to the child-report form items were rated using a 5-point Likert-type scale, ranging from “Never” to “Always,” with higher scores indicating a higher occurrence of the parental disciplinary act. Child-reported discipline practices used by parents were recoded as “never” if the child did not report any act of parental violent discipline, as “sometimes” if the child reported that the tactic occurred “once” or “sometimes” and as “frequently” if the child reported its occurrence as “frequently” and “always.”

Table 1 shows the frequency of physical violence reported by the children. These results show us the high frequency of corporal punishment as a tactic of parental discipline. In Portugal, although a Law introduced in 2007 has amended the Portuguese Penal Code to prohibit all forms of corporal punishment of children, including by parents [53], physical discipline is still observed.

n (%)
Corporal punishmentNever671 (16.1)
Sometimes867 (20.8)
Frequently2637 (63.1)
Severe physical assaultNever3605 (86.3)
Sometimes401 (9.6)
Frequently169 (4.1)
Very severe physical assaultNever4056 (97.1)
Sometimes95 (2.3)
Frequently24 (0.6)

Table 1.

Prevalence of parental physical violence (assessed with the Parent-Child Conflict Tactics Scale) in a sample of 7-year-old children from Generation XXI, a birth cohort from Porto, Portugal (N = 4175).

Also, the social environment in which a child is born and raised affects the nature and quality of social relations and interactions, which, in turn, impacts growth, development, and future achievements. In literature, it has been described that a warm and sensitive parenting style contributes to a child’s positive social behavior and supportive peer relationships [54]. In contrast, unstable, neglectful, or abusive families are associated with episodes of aggressiveness, and impulsivity in the children, impairing their development of tactics to solve a conflict with peers [55], and increasing the risk of violent, aggressive, and bullying behaviors in settings, such as school, that is, outside home environments [56].

Results from the children followed by the cohort Generation XXI showed an increased likelihood of involvement as a bully in children from families with a history of household criminality, that witness parental violence and victims of physical violence [57]. These findings suggest that exposure to household dysfunction might impact children’s emotional and behavioral development. Thus, being exposed to or witnessing other forms of victimization at home might increase their susceptibility to being involved in bullying [58], as children might see it as an acceptable way to manage interpersonal conflicts.

2.3 Peer violence

Children are entitled and must be provided with a safe, nurturing, and inclusive environment where they can grow, learn, thrive, and succeed, achieving their full potential as students and citizens [59]. Communities devote their confidence and expect that schools are the providers of such non-violent environments. However, students all over the world see their ability to fully benefit from educational opportunities endangered by the presence or threat of violence at school, exerted mainly by their peers.

Violence among school-age children and adolescents is a worldwide problem, with negative impact and consequences for the physical and psychological health of those involved, and also, increased risk of behavioral and social problems [12]. Violence at school has also other consequences, such as lower rates of attendance, contributes to lower academic results, and leads to higher drop-out rates [50].

The most traditional forms of peer violence occurring in the educational context comprise bullying, cyberbullying, and physical fighting.

Bullying is an intentional aggressive and negative behavior, repeated over time, that involves a power imbalance favoring the aggressor, with victims having no means to defend themselves [60]. The most common forms of bullying behaviors among adolescents are name-calling, teasing, making threats, spreading rumors, taking of personal belongings, and rejection by excluding someone from a group on purpose [61]. Most of the bullying situations tend to start in school, and sometimes they are not taken as seriously but, instead, as a normal interaction between peers [62]. Children or adolescents involved in bullying can dress the role of victims when the child suffers from bullying but is not an aggressor; as bullies, when the involvement is sole as the aggressor, but not as a victim; and as bully-victim when the child is involved as both victim and aggressor simultaneously.

Estimates of bullying prevalence vary greatly across surveys. A survey conducted by the Health Behavior in School-Aged Children (HBSC) study, in 42 countries and regions across the World Health Organization (WHO) European Region and North America, showed that between 3 and 35% of young people reported involvement in bullying during the past 2 months [63]. According to the WHO European Health Information Gateway from 2017, 11% of girls and 17% of boys aged 11 years old, and 14% of girls and 16% of boys aged 13 have reported being victims of bullying at least twice in the previous 2 months [64].

In a study conducted in the scope of a Portuguese population-based cohort (the Generation XXI) the bullying behavior was assessed through the Bully Scale Survey developed by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) [65]. This scale collects information on the experience of bullying as a victim (11 items) and as a bully (11 items). At the age of 10 years, for each item, the child was asked to indicate the frequency of bullying involvement, choosing between five options—“never,” “rarely,” “sometimes,” “often,” and “always.” As bullying is a repeated behavior, the child was categorized as a victim, when reporting the occurrence of at least one of the items as “often” or “always” in the victimization scale, but answered “never,” “rarely,” or “sometimes” in the aggression scale; the child was classified as a “bully” when answered “often” or “always” in the aggression scale, but answered “never,” “rarely,” or “sometimes” in the victimization scale; finally, the child was categorized as “bully-victim” when reported to be involved both as a victim and as an aggressor simultaneously.

Figure 1 shows results from 5338 participants of Generation XXI. Overall, near 20% of children aged 10 years reported to have been involved in bullying; involvement as only the victim was reported by 14.4% of participants, involvement as only-bully by 1.4%, and involvement as both bully and victim by 3.9%. Boys were more frequently involved in bullying than girls (16.6% versus 12.0% as victims; 2.0% versus 0.7% as bullies; and 5.5% versus 2.3% as bully-victims) [57].

Figure 1.

Prevalence of bullying by sex according to the type of involvement assessed (victimization, aggression, and victimization and aggression simultaneously) among 10-year-old children.

In our society, gender constitutes a structure of social practice that establishes relations of power, attitudes, and hierarchies among people, groups, and institutions [66], and this is reflected in the interaction between children, and consequently in bullying behaviors. Research suggests that boys are more prone to be victims and aggressors of bullying, especially in its physical expression [63, 67], while girls are more likely to engage in situations of indirect bullying, such as teasing or gossiping [62, 67, 68].

With the democratization of the use of new technologies and social media, a new form of peer violence has emerged, cyberbullying, that uses that platform as the scenario for the perpetration of aggressive behaviors [69]. Cyberbullying is the act of sending, posting, or sharing negative, harmful, false, or mean content about someone else through SMS, MMS, and apps, or online in social media, forums, or gaming where people can view, participate in, or share content. It aims to share personal or private information about someone else to cause embarrassment or humiliation. Some cyberbullying crosses the line into unlawful or criminal behavior [70].

However, as for bullying, the prevalence of cyberbullying estimates varies greatly across surveys. A previous scoping review described the prevalence of lifetime cybervictimization as ranging between 4.9 and 65.0%, prevalence of aggression ranging between 1.2 and 44.1%, and prevalence of being involved as victim and aggressor simultaneously ranging from 5.0 to 64.3% [71]. It is known that the attacks cause greater insecurity in the victim, as there are no places or moments to hide since aggressors can reach them almost everywhere [72]. In addition, due to how the violence is carried out, it can be observed by numerous bystanders for an indefinite number of times, which makes the potential damage even greater than that of traditional harassment [72]. Due to the potential of widespread accessibility of victims and an infinite audience by using communication technologies [73], cyberbullying is another important source of stress. Thus, we should also be monitoring the use of technologies as a common form of violence at these ages.

Physical fighting has been measured as a form of violence strongly related to violence in a community. This form of violence is of easy assessment and considered one of the main causes of child morbidity and mortality, accounting for one of the leading causes of death among adolescents aged 15 or over in Europe and America [74]. Physical fighting is defined as the use of an intentional force against others, with potentially serious consequences and injuries or death [75].

Particularly, there is a significant association between physical fighting involvement and other violent behaviors, such as carrying weapons and greater involvement in risk behaviors, being often associated with substance misuse, such as alcohol and drug use [75, 76, 77, 78], as well as media violence exposure [79, 80]. Studies examining the association between both the quantity and quality of sleep and aggression behavior among male adolescents showed that hostility was associated with both reduced quantity [81] and quality of sleep [82].

Advertisement

3. Consequences of violence for health in childhood and adolescence

Violence can have different impacts and effects on the health of children and adolescents. Literature shows that exposure to stressful and traumatic experiences during sensitive periods of neurological and cognitive development in childhood may have lasting implications for physical, emotional, and mental health [12, 83] being a significant early determinant of disease onset and all-cause premature mortality [84, 85, 86].

The physical impacts of violence episodes are the most easily observable and recognizable and may include mild or serious wounds, bruises, fractures. However, we cannot discard that some violence experiences are not so visible but have a significant impact on children’s future health and well-being.

Regarding parental violence, parental and cultural beliefs that corporal punishment is an acceptable way to discipline and educate children, contribute to these forms of discipline not being yet abandoned. However, corporal punishment is responsible for thousands of deaths during childhood each year and regarding its survivors, it has been associated with health problems in childhood and later in life [87]. Given the stigmatizing nature of violence, and its occurrence in a place where the child is supposed to be safe, mostly perpetrated by the ones that should be the main protectors of the child by providing them with a healthy and safe environment, over-reporting is not common, and it is expected that prevalence estimates tend to underestimate the true magnitude of interpersonal violence.

Literature has shown that any experience of violence causes psychological distress and long-term mental ill-health [88]. Exposure to household conflicts poses a significant threat to children’s ability to process and regulate emotions, and it may result in uncontrolled or overcontrolled emotional reactions, contributing to both internalizing and externalizing behaviors [89]. Also, victims of corporal punishment both at home and at school are more likely to become passive and excessively cautious and to a fear-free expression of their ideas and feelings. Children who suffer physical punishment are less likely than other children to internalize moral values, to be altruistic, empathic, or to exercise moral judgment of any kind. Also, being a victim of both physical and psychological abuse increases the risk of depression, posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and anxiety [90, 91].

The involvement in physical fights and bullying victimization is related to somatic symptoms and disturbances, reflecting the emotional effects of aggression. Adolescents involved in physical fights are more likely to present negative health outcomes, such as sleep problems, appetite suppression, and headaches [92, 93]. Similarly, in victims of bullying the most common stress-related symptoms include sleep disorders [94], gastrointestinal complaints, headaches, chronic pain [62, 95], and also bedwetting and tummy ache [96].

Additionally, a girl victim of bullying is at higher risk of suicidal ideation, feeling more nervous or stressed and angry than a boy [97]. One possible explanation is the fact that girls seem to externalize their emotions better and disclose their depressive feelings more easily than boys [62, 98, 99]. Results from a study conducted with adolescents showed that those involved in bullying were more likely to present negative well-being-related feelings, including feeling “nervous or stressed,” “angry,” “sad and desperate,” and also “suicidal ideation.” Suicidal ideation was strongly associated with being involved in bullying as bully-victim, in girls (OR = 8.34; 95% CI: 5.03; 3.82) and in boys (OR = 8.05; 95% CI: 4.24; 15.28) [67].

The link between violence and health may be explained by the biology of social adversity. Therefore, the exposure to violence during childhood may result in early life stress that has the potential to alter physiological systems, thus accounting for a more immediate effect of these exposures, including all the effects that occurred during childhood and adolescence, before adulthood, but that may not necessarily lead to disease. Although the mechanisms explaining the involvement in the biological embodiment of violence are poorly understood in early ages, accumulating evidence suggests that adversity may become programmed molecularly, leaving behind biological memories that can persistently translate into an increased susceptibility to disease later in life [100, 101, 102].

Inflammation, for instance, may be one of the potential mechanisms explaining the link between trauma and health outcomes. Longitudinal studies showed that elevated markers of inflammation, namely C-reactive protein (CRP) levels were observed in adults who experienced childhood adversity, such as parental separation [103], child maltreatment [104], and low socioeconomic status [105]. CRP is an acute-phase protein of hepatic origin whose circulating concentrations rise in response to inflammation. In hospital settings, it can be used to determine the risk of developing coronary artery disease. Although the health effects of violence are well documented in adults, more and more literature has been showing that these alterations start at very early ages. A systematic review aimed to summarize evidence reporting epigenetic and/or neuro-immuno-endocrine embedding of adverse childhood experiences, including violence and episodes of bullying, in children, with a particular focus on the short-term biological effect of those events [106]. The authors observed that the associations reported across studies followed the hypothesis that exposure to adversity is associated with increased biological alterations already at early ages, which may increase the risk of later health outcomes [106].

Empirical data from a birth cohort from Portugal, Generation XXI, showed that at the age of 7 years, children who reported more severe violence perpetrated by their parents presented significantly higher levels of hs-CRP [52]. Higher hs-CRP levels were observed among children reporting extreme violence, including “grab the children by the neck and choke them” or “burn the children or scald them on purpose” [52].

This is supporting evidence that adversity appears to get “under the skin” and induce physiological changes. Although little is known if these alterations in biological markers after experiencing abuse at early ages may be reversed, these results seem to support evidence for biological imprinting and short-term physiological effect of violence that might be strongly associated with later development of disease.

Due to some specificities regarding the type of involvement, bullying might have a different biological impact or health consequences depending on the involvement as a victim, aggressor, or both simultaneously. While evidence has shown that being bullied predicted higher increases in CRP levels, bullying others predicted lower increases in CRP compared with those uninvolved in bullying [107]. A systematic review [106] described other health consequences that were observed, such as higher DNA methylation, shorter telomere length [108], and lower cortisol levels among victims of bullying [109]. However, further investigation is needed to explore the impact of children’s type of involvement in bullying on different biological markers.

In conclusion, literature shows that violence and toxic stress induce physiological changes already in childhood and put children at increased risk for developing several diseases in adulthood, negatively impacting their quality of life and setting them in a less advantageous position [110, 111] from the early life onwards. Exposure to psychosocial stressors leads to continuous dysregulation of physiological responses resulting in the wear and tear on the body, an allostatic load with detrimental long-term health consequences [112].

Advertisement

4. Overcoming and thriving adversity: the resilience framework

Despite growing up and living in contexts of violence, not all children will develop the illness. Some of them even present indicators of healthy development, demonstrating to be resilient to such a disadvantaged environment [113]. The impact of social disadvantage in childhood and allostatic load in later life can be modified by individuals’ psychosocial resilience [112].

Resilience is the individuals’ capacity for overcoming the negative effects of risk exposure, coping successfully with adverse experiences as well as avoiding the negative trajectories associated with risk. This process is influenced by biological, psychological, social, and contextual factors [114]. The most consistent protective factors associated with resilience in children exposed to violence recognized by the literature are supportive parent-child relationships at a family level and self-regulation at an individual level [113].

Examining what differentiates children who demonstrate resilience from those who develop illness and assessing their ability to cope with unfavorable events is essential for informing interventions aiming to improve coping skills and competences promoting healthy trajectories [110, 115, 116]. The identification of the multidimensional processes underlying successful adaptation under adverse conditions allows the design and implementation of successful interventions for the most vulnerable children.

Resiliency Theory focuses on strengths rather than deficits, giving attention to assets (i.e., individual protective factors, such as social skills, coping skills, healthy beliefs, and self-efficacy) and resources (i.e., social and environmental context strengthening individuals facing the risk) which help children to be healthy adults and to have a good quality of life [114, 117, 118]. Studies on Resiliency Theory use three models of resilience—the compensatory, protective, and challenge models—to explain the processes by which promotive factors positively influence the adversity trajectories [114]. The compensatory model defends the idea of a promotive factor acting in an opposite direction of a risk factor on an outcome. The protective factor model highlights the moderating effect of assets and resources on the relationships between a risk factor and a negative outcome. The challenge model suggests that the exposure to moderate levels of a risk factor is associated with less negative, or even positive, outcomes, while low levels and high levels of a risk factor are associated with negative outcomes [114].

Recent studies on the impact of advantageous childhood experiences on adult health have been using the compensatory model of Resiliency Theory, defending that positive childhood experiences will have a direct influence on an outcome [117], counteracting the negative effects of adverse events [118]. The cumulative number of childhood positive experiences leading to resilience and better lifelong health are considered as counter-ACES, including positive parenting, school involvement, meaningful beliefs, and positive and close relationships with family, friends, and other adults [117, 119].

Positive and advantageous childhood experiences and supportive relationships may improve future social experiences and healthy relationships, protecting children against poor health and promoting well-being throughout life [119, 120]. To reduce health problems and improve the quality of life of vulnerable populations, it may be more important to increase counter-ACEs than decrease ACEs. Public health programs focusing on counter-ACEs are able to help families and communities to surround vulnerable children with counter-ACEs, such as parent-child attachment or household routines, helping to neutralize the negative effect of ACEs on children’s health and well-being [120].

Studying childhood maltreatment with a resiliency framework can be particularly important due to the harmful and long-term effects of violence in childhood. This framework allows us to analyze the positive and negative trajectories of children who experienced violence, to understand how maltreated and neglected children overcome the adverse experiences, and to explore the processes, moderators, and mechanisms that facilitate a positive adaptation to violence [121].

Previous studies highlight the important role of families, schools, and peers as well as of individuals’ self-regulation in promoting a positive developmental trajectory in children exposed to violence [113]. The existence of a supportive and stable carer is one of the most important protective factors associated with positive outcomes in this population [121]. Therefore, health promotion strategies directed to children living in violent contexts should be focused on strengthening supportive relationships across ecological contexts, including families, schools, and communities, and on the development of school-based programs aiming at developing children’s self-regulatory capacities [113].

In conclusion, there is a need to deepen the knowledge on childhood resilience to inform the design and development of public health intervention strategies and policies to relieve the impact of violence suffered by individuals during their developmental years, allowing them to achieve good health and quality of life. These strategies will give children living in adverse environments hope and tools to change their negative path, tackling costly social and health inequities. Building resilience in early childhood offers an opportunity to improve the quality of life of the next generation, enhance productivity, and reduce healthcare costs [122].

Advertisement

5. Conclusions

Growing up in a context of violence mostly perpetrated by the ones that should be the main protectors of the child by providing them with a healthy and safe environment may trigger a cascade of psychosocial vulnerabilities. The child may be vulnerable to being exposed to violence at home and then at school, and these experiences can be manifested in different ways. Regardless of the type of exposure to violence, it has a serious impact on child health and development. Although assessing violence experiences in cohort studies may be challenging, it is very relevant to include these questions in the cohort assessments. First, it is a human rights question; second, it impacts the child’s development and well-being; and third, it will impact long-term health. A longitudinal perspective will contribute to understanding the intersection of different violent experiences and their contribution to the production of health inequalities. In addition, we can explore the resilience factors in a life-course perspective, which will help to inform the design and development of interventions enhancing existing skills, encouraging healthy adjustment trajectories, and nurturing resilient adaptation.

Advertisement

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to acknowledge the vision of Professor Henrique Barros for always considering violence as a relevant topic for public health and including measures of violence in the cohort assessments.

Support for the completion of this work was provided by the European Regional Development Fund (ERDF) through the Operational Programme Competitiveness and Internationalization and national funding from the Foundation for Science and Technology (FCT), Portuguese Ministry of Science, Technology and Higher Education through the financing of the project “HIneC: When do health inequalities start? Understanding the impact of childhood social adversity on health trajectories from birth to early adolescence” (POCI-01-0145-FEDER-029567; PTDC/SAU-PUB/29567/2017), the Epidemiology Research Unit—Instituto de Saúde Pública da Universidade do Porto (EPIUnit) (UIDB/04750/2020), the Associate Laboratory ITR (LA/P/0064/2020), and the individual researcher contract to SF (CEECIND/01516/2017).

Advertisement

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. 1. Brady SS. Lifetime community violence exposure and health risk behavior among young adults in college. The Journal of Adolescent Health. 2006;39(4):610-613
  2. 2. Cairney J, Krause N. Negative life events and age-related decline in mastery: Are older adults more vulnerable to the control-eroding effect of stress? The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2008;63(3):S162-S170
  3. 3. Campbell J, Jones AS, Dienemann J, Kub J, Schollenberger J, O’Campo P, et al. Intimate partner violence and physical health consequences. Archives of Internal Medicine. 2002;162(10):1157-1163
  4. 4. Heise L, Ellsberg M, Gottmoeller M. A global overview of gender-based violence. International Journal of Gynecology and Obstetrics. 2002;78(S1):S5-S14
  5. 5. Elder GH. The life course as developmental theory. Child Development. 1998;69(1):1-12
  6. 6. Macmillan R. Violence and the life course: The consequences of victimization for personal and social development. Annual Review of Sociology. 2001;27(1):1-22
  7. 7. Robins LNRM. Straight and Devious Pathways from Childhood to Adulthood. Cambridge, New York: Cambridge University Press; 1990
  8. 8. Fraga S. Methodological and ethical challenges in violence research. Porto Biomedical Journal. 2016;1(2):77-80
  9. 9. Kinard EM. Ethical issues in research with abused children. Child Abuse and Neglect. 1985;9(3):301-311
  10. 10. Kinard EM. Methodological issues and practical problems in conducting research on maltreated children. Child Abuse and Neglect. 1994;18(8):645-656
  11. 11. Johnston JR, Lee S, Olesen NW, Walters MG. Allegations and substantiations of abuse in custody-disputing families. Family Court Review. 2005;43(2):283-294
  12. 12. World Health Organization. Global Status Report on Violence Prevention. Geneve: World Health Organization; 2014
  13. 13. Hillis S, Mercy J, Amobi A, Kress H. Global prevalence of past-year violence against children: A systematic review and minimum estimates. Pediatrics. 2016;137(3):e20154079
  14. 14. Bowers A, Strelitz J, Allen J, Donkin A. An Equal Start: Improving outcomes in Children’s Centres, the Evidence Review. London: University College London; 2012
  15. 15. Pillas D, Marmot M, Naicker K, Goldblatt P, Morrison J, Pikhart H. Social inequalities in early childhood health and development: A European-wide systematic review. Pediatric Research. 2014;76(5):418-424
  16. 16. Commission on Social Determinants of Health. Closing the Gap in a Generation. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2008
  17. 17. Burman M, Öhman A, editors. Challenging gender and violence: Positions and discourses in Swedish and international contexts. Women’s Studies International Forum. 2014;46:81-82
  18. 18. Talge NM, Neal C, Glover V. Antenatal maternal stress and long-term effects on child neurodevelopment: How and why? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2007;48(3-4):245-261
  19. 19. Rosen D, Seng JS, Tolman RM, Mallinger G. Intimate partner violence, depression, and posttraumatic stress disorder as additional predictors of low birth weight infants among low-income mothers. Journal of Interpersonal Violence. 2007;22(10):1305-1314
  20. 20. Sarkar NN. The impact of intimate partner violence on women’s reproductive health and pregnancy outcome. Journal of Obstetrics and Gynaecology. 2008;28(3):266-271
  21. 21. Rodrigues T, Rocha L, Barros H. Physical abuse during pregnancy and preterm delivery. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology. 2008;198(2):171.e1-171.e6
  22. 22. Han A, Stewart DE. Maternal and fetal outcomes of intimate partner violence associated with pregnancy in the Latin American and Caribbean region. International Journal of Gynaecology and Obstetrics. 2014;124(1):6-11
  23. 23. Janssen PA, Holt VL, Sugg NK, Emanuel I, Critchlow CM, Henderson AD. Intimate partner violence and adverse pregnancy outcomes: A population-based study. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology. 2003;188(5):1341-1347
  24. 24. Donovan BM, Spracklen CN, Schweizer ML, Ryckman KK, Saftlas AF. Intimate partner violence during pregnancy and the risk for adverse infant outcomes: A systematic review and meta-analysis. BJOG: An International Journal of Obstetrics and Gynaecology. 2016;123(8):1289-1299
  25. 25. Cnattingius S. The epidemiology of smoking during pregnancy: Smoking prevalence, maternal characteristics, and pregnancy outcomes. Nicotine and Tobacco Research. 2004;6(Suppl. 2):S125-S140
  26. 26. Dube SR, Miller JW, Brown DW, Giles WH, Felitti VJ, Dong M, et al. Adverse childhood experiences and the association with ever using alcohol and initiating alcohol use during adolescence. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2006;38(4):444.e1-444.10
  27. 27. Lester BM, Tronick EZ, LaGasse L, Seifer R, Bauer CR, Shankaran S, et al. The maternal lifestyle study: Effects of substance exposure during pregnancy on neurodevelopmental outcome in 1-month-old infants. Pediatrics. 2002;110(6):1182-1192
  28. 28. Williamson DF, Thompson TJ, Anda RF, Dietz WH, Felitti V. Body weight and obesity in adults and self-reported abuse in childhood. International Journal of Obesity and Related Metabolic Disorders. 2002;26(8):1075-1082
  29. 29. Charil A, Laplante DP, Vaillancourt C, King S. Prenatal stress and brain development. Brain Research Reviews. 2010;65(1):56-79
  30. 30. Mehler MF, Purpura DP. Autism, fever, epigenetics and the locus coeruleus. Brain Research Reviews. 2009;59(2):388-392
  31. 31. Conradt E, Lester BM, Appleton AA, Armstrong DA, Marsit CJ. The roles of DNA methylation of NR3C1 and 11β-HSD2 and exposure to maternal mood disorder in utero on newborn neurobehavior. Epigenetics. 2013;8(12):1321-1329
  32. 32. Kinney DK, Miller AM, Crowley DJ, Huang E, Gerber E. Autism prevalence following prenatal exposure to hurricanes and tropical storms in Louisiana. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2008;38(3):481-488
  33. 33. Radtke KM, Ruf M, Gunter HM, Dohrmann K, Schauer M, Meyer A, et al. Transgenerational impact of intimate partner violence on methylation in the promoter of the glucocorticoid receptor. Translational Psychiatry. 2011;1(7):e21-e21
  34. 34. Dietert RR, Dietert JM. Potential for early-life immune insult including developmental immunotoxicity in autism and autism spectrum disorders: Focus on critical windows of immune vulnerability. Journal of Toxicology and Environmental Health. Part B, Critical Reviews. 2008;11(8):660-680
  35. 35. Roberts AL, Lyall K, Rich-Edwards JW, Ascherio A, Weisskopf MG. Maternal exposure to intimate partner abuse before birth is associated with autism spectrum disorder in offspring. Autism. 2016;20(1):26-36
  36. 36. Grizenko N, Fortier M-E, Zadorozny C, Thakur G, Schmitz N, Duval R. Maternal stress during pregnancy, ADHD symptomatology in children and genotype: Gene-environment interaction. Journal of the Canadian Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2012;21:9-15
  37. 37. Laplante DP, Barr RG, Brunet A, Galbaud du Fort G, Meaney ML, Saucier JF, et al. Stress during pregnancy affects general intellectual and language functioning in human toddlers. Pediatric Research. 2004;56(3):400-410
  38. 38. Davis EP, Sandman CA. The timing of prenatal exposure to maternal cortisol and psychosocial stress is associated with human infant cognitive development. Child Development. 2010;81(1):131-148
  39. 39. Bogat GA, DeJonghe E, Levendosky AA, Davidson WS, von Eye A. Trauma symptoms among infants exposed to intimate partner violence. Child Abuse and Neglect. 2006;30(2):109-125
  40. 40. Kempe CH, Silverman FN, Steele BF, Droegemueller W, Silver HK. The battered-child syndrome. Journal of the American Medical Association. 1962;181(1):17-24
  41. 41. World Health Organization. Preventing child maltreatment: A Guide to Taking Action and Generating Evidence/World Health Organization and International Society for Prevention of Child Abuse and Neglect. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2006
  42. 42. Creavey KL, Gatzke-Kopp LM, Fosco GM. Differential effects of family stress exposure and harsh parental discipline on child social competence. Journal of Child and Family Studies. 2018;27(2):483-493
  43. 43. Friedson M. Authoritarian parenting attitudes and social origin: The multigenerational relationship of socioeconomic position to childrearing values. Child Abuse and Neglect. 2016;51:263-275
  44. 44. Yunus KRM, Dahlan NA. Child-rearing practices and socio-economic status: Possible implications for children’s educational outcomes. Procedia—Social and Behavioral Sciences. 2013;90:251-259
  45. 45. Chen E, Miller GE. Socioeconomic status and health: Mediating and moderating factors. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2013;9:723-749
  46. 46. Gülseven Z, Kumru A, Carlo G, Palermo F, Selçuk B, Sayıl M. The mediational roles of harsh and responsive parenting in the longitudinal relations between socioeconomic status and Turkish children’s emotional development. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2018;42(6):563-573
  47. 47. Roubinov DS, Boyce WT. Parenting and SES: Relative values or enduring principles? Current Opinion in Psychology. 2017;15:162-167
  48. 48. UNICEF. Know Violence in Childhood. Ending Violence in Childhood. 2017
  49. 49. Straus MA. Prevalence, societal causes, and trends in corporal punishment by parents in world perspective. Law and Contemporary Problems. 2010;73(2):1-30
  50. 50. United Nations Children’s Fund. A Familiar Face: Violence in the lives of children and adolescents. New York: UNICEF; 2017
  51. 51. UNICEF. Hidden in Plain Sight: A Statistical Analysis of Violence against Children. 2014. Available from: https://www.unicef.org/publications/index_74865.html
  52. 52. Fraga S, Soares S, Santos AC, Barros H. Parents’ use of extreme physical violence is associated with elevated high-sensitivity C-reactive protein in children. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2021;282:454-457
  53. 53. Decreto-Lei nº. 48/95 - Violência doméstica. 2007
  54. 54. Helsen M, Vollebergh W, Meeus W. Social support from parents and friends and emotional problems in adolescence. Journal of youth and adolescence. 2000;29(3):319-335
  55. 55. Van der Kolk BA. Developmental. Psychiatric Annals. 2005;35(5):401
  56. 56. Hong JS, Espelage DL. A review of research on bullying and peer victimization in school: An ecological system analysis. Aggression and Violent Behavior. 2012;17(4):311-322
  57. 57. Fraga S, Soares S, Peres FS, Barros H. Household dysfunction is associated with bullying behavior in 10-year-old children: Do socioeconomic circumstances matter? Journal of Interpersonal Violence. 2021;0(0):08862605211006352
  58. 58. Lereya ST, Copeland WE, Costello EJ, Wolke D. Adult mental health consequences of peer bullying and maltreatment in childhood: Two cohorts in two countries. The Lancet Psychiatry. 2015;2(6):524-531
  59. 59. United Nations. SDG-Education. 2030 Steering Committee. 2015. Available from: https://sustainabledevelopment.un.org/index.php?page=view&type=30022&nr=100&menu=3170
  60. 60. Olweus D. Bullying at school: What we know and what we can do. Psychology in the Schools. 1993;40(6):699-700
  61. 61. Smith PK, Cowie H, Olafsson RF, Liefooghe AP, Almeida A, Araki H, et al. Definitions of bullying: A comparison of terms used, and age and gender differences, in a fourteen-country international comparison. Child Development. 2002;73(4):1119-1133
  62. 62. National Academies of Sciences, Engineering and Medicine. Preventing Bullying Through Science, Policy, and Practice. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2016
  63. 63. Craig WM, Harel Y. Bullying, physical fighting and victimization. Young People’s Health in Context: International Report from the HBSC. 2001;2:133-144
  64. 64. World Health Organization. Child and Adolescent Health. 2017. Available from: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/adolescents-health-risks-and-solutions
  65. 65. Hamburger ME, Basile KC, Vivolo AM. Measuring Bullying Victimization, Perpetration, and Bystander Experiences; A Compendium of Assessment Tools. National Center for Injury Prevention and Control (US), Division of Violence Prevention. 2011
  66. 66. Steinfeldt JA, Vaughan EL, LaFollette JR, Steinfeldt MC. Bullying among adolescent football players: Role of masculinity and moral atmosphere. Psychology of Men and Masculinity. 2012;13(4):340-353
  67. 67. Silva-Rocha N, Soares S, Brochado S, Fraga S. Bullying involvement, family background, school life, and well-being feelings among adolescents. Journal of Public Health. 2020;28(5):481-489
  68. 68. Finkelhor D, Shattuck A, Turner H, Hamby S. A revised inventory of adverse childhood experiences. Child Abuse and Neglect. 2015;48:13-21
  69. 69. Aboujaoude E, Savage MW, Starcevic V, Salame WO. Cyberbullying: Review of an old problem gone viral. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2015;57(1):10-18
  70. 70. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. What Is Cyberbullying Washington: StopBullying.gov. 2017. Available from: https://www.stopbullying.gov/cyberbullying/what-is-it
  71. 71. Brochado S, Fraga S, Soares S, Ramos E, Barros H. Cyberbullying among adolescents: The influence of different modes of inquiry. Journal of Interpersonal Violence. 2017;36(3-4):1933-1950
  72. 72. Smith PK, Mahdavi J, Carvalho M, Fisher S, Russell S, Tippett N. Cyberbullying: Its nature and impact in secondary school pupils. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2008;49(4):376-385
  73. 73. Shariff S, Hoff DL. Cyber bullying: Clarifying Legal Boundaries for School Supervision in Cyberspace. 2016
  74. 74. Mokdad AH, Forouzanfar MH, Daoud F, Mokdad AA, El Bcheraoui C, Moradi-Lakeh M, et al. Global burden of diseases, injuries, and risk factors for young people’s health during 1990-2013: A systematic analysis for the global burden of disease study 2013. Lancet. 2016;387(10036):2383-2401
  75. 75. Pickett W, Molcho M, Elgar FJ, Brooks F, de Looze M, Rathmann K, et al. Trends and socioeconomic correlates of adolescent physical fighting in 30 countries. Pediatrics. 2013;131(1):e18-e26
  76. 76. Fraga S, Ramos E, Dias S, Barros H. Physical fighting among school-going Portuguese adolescents: Social and behavioural correlates. Preventive Medicine. 2011;52(5):401-404
  77. 77. Rudatsikira E, Muula AS, Siziya S. Prevalence and correlates of physical fighting among school-going adolescents in Santiago, Chile. Brazilian Journal of Psychiatry. 2008;30(3):197-202
  78. 78. Swahn MH, Bossarte RM, Palmier JB, Yao H, Van Dulmen MHM. Psychosocial characteristics associated with frequent physical fighting: Findings from the 2009 National Youth Risk Behavior Survey. Injury Prevention. 2013;19(2):143-146
  79. 79. Coker TR, Elliott MN, Schwebel DC, Windle M, Toomey SL, Tortolero SR, et al. Media violence exposure and physical aggression in fifth-grade children. Academic Pediatrics. 2015;15(1):82-88
  80. 80. Demissie Z, Lowry R, Eaton DK, Hertz MF, Lee SM. Associations of school violence with physical activity among U.S. high school students. Journal of Physical Activity and Health. 2014;11(4):705-711
  81. 81. Soares S, Araújo J, Ramos E, Fraga S. Sleep duration and physical fighting involvement in late adolescence. Journal of Public Health. 2019;27(3):341-348
  82. 82. Ireland JL, Culpin V. The relationship between sleeping problems and aggression, anger, and impulsivity in a population of juvenile and young offenders. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2006;38(6):649-655
  83. 83. Anda RF, Felitti VJ, Bremner JD, Walker JD, Whitfield C, Perry BD, et al. The enduring effects of abuse and related adverse experiences in childhood. A convergence of evidence from neurobiology and epidemiology. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience. 2006;256(3):174-186
  84. 84. Felitti VJ, Anda RF, Nordenberg D, Williamson DF, Spitz AM, Edwards V, et al. Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults. The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 1998;14(4):245-258
  85. 85. Kelly-Irving M, Mabile L, Grosclaude P, Lang T, Delpierre C. The embodiment of adverse childhood experiences and cancer development: Potential biological mechanisms and pathways across the life course. International journal of public health. 2012;58(1):3-11
  86. 86. Miller GE, Chen E, Parker KJ. Psychological stress in childhood and susceptibility to the chronic diseases of aging: Moving toward a model of behavioral and biological mechanisms. Psychological Bulletin. 2011;137(6):959-997
  87. 87. World Health Organization. Chapter 3: Child Abuse and Neglect by Parents and other Caregivers. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2002
  88. 88. Ferrara P, Franceschini G, Villani A, Corsello G. Physical, psychological and social impact of school violence on children. Italian Journal of Pediatrics. 2019;45(1):76
  89. 89. Zarling AL, Taber-Thomas S, Murray A, Knuston JF, Lawrence E, Valles N-L, et al. Internalizing and externalizing symptoms in young children exposed to intimate partner violence: Examining intervening processes. Journal of Family Psychology. 2013;27(6):945
  90. 90. Dillon G, Hussain R, Loxton D, Rahman S. Mental and physical health and intimate partner violence against women: A review of the literature. International Journal of Family Medicine. 2013;2013:313909
  91. 91. Lawrence E, Orengo-Aguayo R, Langer A, Brock R. The impact and consequences of partner abuse on partners. Partner Abuse. 2012;3:406-428
  92. 92. Fekkes M, Pijpers FIM, Fredriks AM, Vogels T, Verloove-Vanhorick SP. Do bullied children get Ill, or do Ill children get bullied? A prospective cohort study on the relationship between bullying and health-related symptoms. Pediatrics. 2006;117(5):1568-1574
  93. 93. Walsh SD, Molcho M, Craig W, Harel-Fisch Y, Huynh Q, Kukaswadia A, et al. Physical and emotional health problems experienced by youth engaged in physical fighting and weapon carrying. PLoS One. 2013;8(2):e56403
  94. 94. Hertz MF, Everett Jones S, Barrios L, David-Ferdon C, Holt M. Association between bullying victimization and health risk behaviors among high school students in the United States. The Journal of School Health. 2015;85(12):833-842
  95. 95. Beran T, Stanton L, Hetherington R, Mishna F, Shariff S. Development of the bullying and health experiences scale. Interactive Journal of Medical Research. 2012;1(2):e13
  96. 96. Kshirsagar VY, Agarwal R, Bavdekar SB. Bullying in schools: Prevalence and short-term impact. Indian Pediatrics. 2007;44(1):25-28
  97. 97. Barzilay S, Brunstein Klomek A, Apter A, Carli V, Wasserman C, Hadlaczky G, et al. Bullying victimization and suicide ideation and behavior among adolescents in Europe: A 10-country study. The Journal of Adolescent Health. 2017;61(2):179-186
  98. 98. Holt MK, Kaufman Kantor G, Finkelhor D. Parent/child concordance about bullying involvement and family characteristics related to bullying and peer victimization. Journal of School Violence. 2008;8(1):42-63
  99. 99. Renouf AG, Harter S. Low self-worth and anger as components of the depressive experience in young adolescents. Development and Psychopathology. 1990;2(3):293-310
  100. 100. Rutter M. Achievements and challenges in the biology of environmental effects. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2012;109(Suppl. 2):17149-17153
  101. 101. Meaney MJ. Maternal care, gene expression, and the transmission of individual differences in stress reactivity across generations. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2001;24:1161-1192
  102. 102. Liu D, Diorio J, Tannenbaum B, Caldji C, Francis D, Freedman A, et al. Maternal care, hippocampal glucocorticoid receptors, and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responses to stress. Science. 1997;277(5332):1659-1662
  103. 103. Lacey RE, Kumari M, McMunn A. Parental separation in childhood and adult inflammation: The importance of material and psychosocial pathways. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013;38(11):2476-2484
  104. 104. Coelho R, Viola T, Walss-Bass C, Brietzke E, Grassi-Oliveira R. Childhood maltreatment and inflammatory markers: A systematic review. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2014;129(3):180-192
  105. 105. Miller AH, Maletic V, Raison CL. Inflammation and its discontents: The role of cytokines in the pathophysiology of major depression. Biological Psychiatry. 2009;65(9):732-741
  106. 106. Soares S, Rocha V, Kelly-Irving M, Stringhini S, Fraga S. Adverse childhood events and health biomarkers: A systematic review. Frontiers Public Health. 2021;9:649-825
  107. 107. Copeland WE, Wolke D, Lereya ST, Shanahan L, Worthman C, Costello EJ. Childhood bullying involvement predicts low-grade systemic inflammation into adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2014;111(21):7570-7575
  108. 108. Shalev I, Moffitt TE, Sugden K, Williams B, Houts RM, Danese A, et al. Exposure to violence during childhood is associated with telomere erosion from 5 to 10 years of age: A longitudinal study. Molecular Psychiatry. 2013;18(5):576-581
  109. 109. Östberg V, Låftman SB, Modin B, Lindfors P. Bullying as a stressor in mid-adolescent girls and boys-associations with perceived stress, recurrent pain, and salivary cortisol. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2018;15(2):364
  110. 110. Hajat A, Nurius P, Song C. Differing trajectories of adversity over the life course: Implications for adult health and well-being. Child Abuse and Neglect. 2020;102:104392
  111. 111. Mackenbach JP. Persistence of social inequalities in modern welfare states: Explanation of a paradox. Scandinavian Journal of Public Health. 2017;45(2):113-120
  112. 112. Kelly-Irving M. Allostatic load: How stress in childhood affects life-course health outcomes. Health Foundation. 2019. Available from: https://www.health.org.uk/publications/allostatic-load
  113. 113. Yule K, Houston J, Grych J. Resilience in children exposed to violence: A meta-analysis of protective factors across ecological contexts. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review. 2019;22(3):406-431
  114. 114. Fergus S, Zimmerman MA. Adolescent resilience: A framework for understanding healthy development in the face of risk. Annual Review of Public Health. 2005;26:399-419
  115. 115. Fritz J, de Graaff AM, Caisley H, van Harmelen AL, Wilkinson PO. A systematic review of amenable resilience factors that moderate and/or mediate the relationship between childhood adversity and mental health in young people. Frontiers in Psychiatry. 2018;9:230
  116. 116. Pettoello-Mantovani M, Pop TL, Mestrovic J, Ferrara P, Giardino I, Carrasco-Sanz A, et al. Fostering resilience in children: The essential role of healthcare professionals and families. Journal of Pediatrics. 2019;205:298-299
  117. 117. Crandall A, Broadbent E, Stanfill M, Magnusson BM, Novilla MLB, Hanson CL, et al. The influence of adverse and advantageous childhood experiences during adolescence on young adult health. Child Abuse and Neglect. 2020;108:104644
  118. 118. Zimmerman MA. Resiliency theory: A strengths-based approach to research and practice for adolescent health. Health Education and Behavior. 2013;40(4):381-383
  119. 119. Narayan AJ, Rivera LM, Bernstein RE, Harris WW, Lieberman AF. Positive childhood experiences predict less psychopathology and stress in pregnant women with childhood adversity: A pilot study of the benevolent childhood experiences (BCEs) scale. Child Abuse and Neglect. 2018;78:19-30
  120. 120. Crandall A, Miller JR, Cheung A, Novilla LK, Glade R, Novilla MLB, et al. ACEs and counter-ACEs: How positive and negative childhood experiences influence adult health. Child Abuse and Neglect. 2019;96:104089
  121. 121. Houshyar S, Kaufman J. Resiliency in maltreated children. In: Goldstein SBRB, editor. Handbook of Resilience in Children. New York: Springer; 2005. pp. 181-200
  122. 122. Clark H, Coll-Seck AM, Banerjee A, Peterson S, Dalglish SL, Ameratunga S, et al. A future for the world’s children? A WHO-UNICEF-Lancet Commission. Lancet. 2020;395(10224):605-658

Written By

Sílvia Fraga, Mariana Amorim and Sara Soares

Submitted: 13 December 2021 Reviewed: 10 January 2022 Published: 09 February 2022