Open access peer-reviewed chapter

Taxon-Specific Pair Bonding in Gibbons (Hylobatidae)

Written By

Thomas Geissmann, Simone Rosenkranz-Weck, Judith J.G.M. Van Der Loo and Mathias Orgeldinger

Submitted: 26 November 2020 Reviewed: 27 November 2020 Published: 30 December 2020

DOI: 10.5772/intechopen.95270

From the Edited Volume

Updates on Veterinary Anatomy and Physiology

Edited by Catrin Sian Rutland and Samir A.A. El-Gendy

Chapter metrics overview

638 Chapter Downloads

View Full Metrics

Abstract

This study provides the first statistically significant evidence that the mechanisms of how pair bonds are created or maintained differ between gibbon taxa. We examine the pair bond in captive pairs of three genera of gibbons (Hylobatidae): siamangs (Symphalangus, N = 17 pairs), crested gibbons (Nomascus, N = 7 pairs), and pileated gibbons (Hylobates pileatus, N = 9 pairs). In the first part of this study, we determine three generally-accepted indicators of pair-bond strength (mutual grooming, behavioral synchronization and partner distance). A pairwise comparison of our samples reveals a difference in relative partner distances between siamangs and pileated gibbons, suggesting that siamangs may have a stronger pair bond than pileated gibbons. No difference among the three taxa was found in other variables believed to indicate pair bond strength. In the second part we examine the amount of partner-directed grooming in each sex. In siamangs, males invest significantly more into pair bonds than females, whereas the opposite is true in crested and pileated gibbons. Our results for siamangs correspond to predictions derived from the ‘mate-defense hypothesis’ for the evolution of pair bonds, whereas our results for crested gibbons and pileated gibbons correspond to predictions derived from the ‘male-services hypothesis’.

Keywords

  • social structure
  • pair bond
  • Symphalangus
  • Nomascus
  • Hylobates
  • sex-specific investment
  • Hylobatidae

1. Introduction

Whereas the genera of great apes are known to differ strongly among each other in their social structure, the small apes or gibbons clearly are a more uniform group [1, 2, 3]. Distributed in Asian rain forests, its members typically live in socially monogamous, unimale unifemale, territorial groups [4, 5], although some flexibility in group composition and sexual behavior occurs [6, 7, 8]. Non-monogamous (extra-pair) matings and groups with multiple adult males and multiple adult females occasionally occur in gibbons [7, 9, 10, 11, 12, 13, 14, 15].

It has repeatedly been suggested, however, that gibbon taxa may differ in subtle details of their social organization [16]:

Wild family groups of Malayan siamangs (Symphalangus syndactylus) appear to be more tightly knit than those of white-handed gibbons (Hylobates lar), with siamang intra-group distances being shorter and intra-group communicatory signals being fewer or less conspicuous to observers, and paternal infant-carrying only occurring in siamangs [17, 18, 19, 20, 21]. A greater heterosexual cohesion in pair bonds of siamangs, as compared to white-handed gibbons, was also found in a field study in Sumatra [22], but differences in paternal investment appear to be less clear-cut. Paternal infant-carrying appears to be absent in some wild and many captive siamang groups and varies dramatically among males of the same population [10, 23, 24, 25], while it may occasionally occur in other gibbon taxa, at least in captive groups [23, 26].

Several reports suggest that the black-cheeked species of the crested gibbons (genus Nomascus) differ from other hylobatids in their social organization by more often forming bi-female groups [27, 28, 29, 30, 31, 32, 33]. This may not appear to apply to light-cheeked crested gibbon species [34, 35], but see [36].

Recent studies on gibbon calls documented that the various taxa strongly differ in how they present their long and loud morning song bouts [37, 38, 39, 40]. In some taxa, mated pairs produce duet song bouts but usually no solo songs (genera Hoolock, Nomascus and Symphalangus), others produce sex-specific solo songs in addition to duets (Hylobates agilis, H. lar, H. muelleri, H. pileatus), and others yet produce sex-specific solo song bouts only (H. klossii, H. moloch). In all members of the genus Nomascus and most species of the genus Hylobates, for instance, most of the singing is produced by males, whereas in H. moloch, males sing rarely and most songs are produced by females. This pronounced diversity of sex-specific investment in resource defense provides indirect evidence for taxon-specific differences in social organization, and, possibly, in previously unrecognized factors of ecological adaptation or inter-specific competition.

Moreover, duets strongly differ in their complexity among taxa, with the most complex ones being uttered by siamangs (S. syndactylus) [41, 42]. These differences in song organization also strongly suggest differences in social organization. Because duet song bouts are believed to serve, among other things, to strengthen or advertise pair bonds, duetting and non-duetting gibbon species should differ either in their pair bond strength or in how the pair bond strength is achieved [43, 44].

Although the reports cited above suggest that some gibbon taxa may differ in social organization, very little quantitative evidence for such species-specific differences in the social structure are currently available. Previous comparisons have been limited to sample sizes of 2–3 pairs per genus [17, 19, 22, 23], thus precluding statistical testing. Palombit [3] correctly identified a great need for detailed data on more hylobatid pair bonds, so that we may identify consistent social patterns in light of intra-specific variation.

Early reviews on monogamy found shared behavioral traits in monogamous primates and suggested that the males generally initiate grooming and groom females more often than the reverse situation occurs [45]. In socially monogamous pairs, pair partners usually maintain close spatial association and often perform spectacular, well-coordinated, pair-specific display behavior. This does not necessarily imply, however, that the sexes share mutual socioreproductive interests [46]. Shared interests may be not be required for the evolution of social monogamy, and pair formation does not require an absence of sexual conflict, or symmetric costs and benefits for males and females.

Several of the hypotheses explaining the evolution and maintenance of social monogamy in mammals make predictions regarding female and male contributions to the pair bond [47].

  1. According to the ‘resource-defense hypothesis’, both a male and a female benefit from pair bonding to defend resources together [48]. In this case, a male and a female should be equally interested in maintaining proximity and affiliation with a pair mate and defending their territory.

  2. According to the ‘mate-defense hypothesis’, a male should bond with a female when either the spatial distribution of females or the temporal distribution of fertile periods makes it difficult for males to defend access to more than one female at a time [49]. In this case, a male should be more interested in maintaining proximity and affiliation with the partner.

  3. According to the ‘male-services hypothesis’, a female benefits from bonding with a male when the male provides important services such as territorial or antipredator defense, infant care, or protection from infanticide by competing males [48, 50, 51, 52]. In this case, a female should to be more interested in maintaining proximity and affiliation with the partner while the male should provide some significant services.

The goal of our study was to answer the following two questions: (1) Do gibbon taxa differ in the strength of their pair bonds? (2) Do gibbon taxa differ in the way pair bonds are created and maintained?

We present the first comparative and quantitative study on differences of the pair bond among multiple gibbon taxa. We have collected observational data on several captive groups of siamangs (genus Symphalangus), three species of the crested gibbons (genus Nomascus), and the pileated gibbon (Hylobates pileatus) as a representative of the dwarf gibbons (genus Hylobates). Our study will focus, therefore, on a comparison of these three genera. Photographs of three of the species we studied are shown in Figure 1. In addition, a compilation of previously-published data also permits us to make a limited comparison with other gibbons of the genus Hylobates.

Figure 1.

Three of the gibbon species that were observed during this study. (a) Siamang (Symphalangus syndactylus), family group, showing from left to right: juvenile, adult male, and adult female carrying an infant. Siamangs are of mostly blackish fur coloration. Notice the half-inflated throat sacs, which play a role in siamang territorial vocalizations and can be inflated to about the size of the animals’ head. (b) Northern White-cheeked Crested Gibbon (Nomascus leucogenys) pair, showing from left to right: adult female, and adult male. Adult male and female differ markedly in their fur coloration. Females are mostly yellowish, and males are blackish. (c) Pileated Gibbon (Hylobates pileatus) family group, showing from left to right: adult female carrying a neonate infant, adult male, subadult male, and juvenile male. Adult male and female of this species also differ markedly in their fur coloration. Females are pale grey or fawn-buff with black on crown, cheeks and chest, while males are blackish with white facial border, corona, digits, and genital tuft. Photographs by Thomas Geissmann.

In order to estimate pair bond strength, we quantified the following three generally-accepted indicators of pair bond strength (behavioral synchronization, relative distance between mates, and amount of partner directed grooming) following [44, 53].

In order to gain insight in the pair bonding mechanism, we examined which sex invests more in the pair bond by measuring the amount of grooming directed at the respective partner.

Although allogrooming per se may serve hygienic, social, communicatory, stress relief or thermal functions [54, 55, 56, 57, 58, 59], these functions may be influenced by a species’s social organization. For social systems with stable pair structures, allogrooming has been proposed to serve a pair-bonding function [45, 60, 61] and to reflect the investment into a pair bond [62, 63] and, therefore, the ultimate costs and benefits which partners can expect from a relationship [64]. Thus, sex-specific differences in partner-directed allogrooming indicate – on a proximate level – ultimate sex-specific strategies.

Advertisement

2. Methods

Our data collection methods have previously been described [44, 53]. Siamang (Symphalangus syndactylus) data were collected in a consistent form by one of us (M.O.) between April 1985 and March 1993. A total of 17 siamang groups were observed at the following zoos: Antwerp (An), Belgium, Branféré (Br1, Br2, Br3), France, Budapest (Bu), Hungary, Berlin Zoo (Be), Dortmund (Do), Dresden (Dr1, Dr2), Duisburg (Du), Frankfurt (Fr), Krefeld (Kr1, Kr2), Munich (Mn), Germany, Studen (St), Zurich (Zh), Switzerland and Washington (Wa), U.S.A., with group size ranging from two to six animals.

Crested gibbon data were collected in the same way by S.R.-W. between August and October 2001. A total of seven crested gibbon groups (Nomascus) were observed at the following zoos: Duisburg (Du), Eberswalde (Eb), Osnabrück (Os1, Os2), Germany, and Mulhouse (Mu1, Mu2, Mu3), France, with group sizes ranging from two to five animals. Three crested gibbon species are represented in our sample, including the Northern White-cheeked Crested Gibbon (N. leucogenys): Du, Mu1 and Os2; the Southern White-cheeked Crested Gibbon (N. siki): Mu2; and the Southern Yellow-cheeked Crested Gibbon (N. gabriellae): Eb, Mu3 and Os1. The gibbon classification used here follows [65].

Data for Pileated Gibbons (Hylobates pileatus) were collected in the same way by J.v.d.L. and K.N between February and May 2007. A total of nine groups were observed at the following zoos: Phnom Tamao, Cambodia (PT1–7), and Zurich, Switzerland (Zu1, Zu2), with group size ranging from two to five animals.

In order to assure comparability of data collected by the observers M.O., S.R., J.v.d.L. and K.N., dual observations were carried out on 31 July 2001 and on 13 February 2007, respectively, until consistent values of inter-observer concordance were obtained [66].

At each zoo, observation time for each sampling method was distributed evenly across the animals’ activity period between 0700 and 1800 h (until 1700 h during the winter months, and between 0800 and 1800 h for crested gibbon groups Du, Eb, Mu, Os2).

We used focal animal sampling with the continuous recording rule [66, 67, 68, 69] to collect information on the frequency and duration of grooming behavior between mates. Focal animals were changed every 20 min. Each of 11 siamang pairs was observed for 80 h, except for pairs Mu (50 h) and Du (90 h), and each crested and pileated gibbon pair for 35 h. Grooming occurred in discrete sessions that could be counted. We allowed an interval of up to 10 seconds between bouts of grooming before we counted them as two sessions, rather than one.

We used scan sampling to record behavioral synchronization of activities between mates. We defined 11 behavioral categories: socio-positive behavior (including allogrooming, embracing) and infant care, play, agonistic, territorial, sexual, comfort-related, feeding and food-related behavior, observe, rest and sleep, excretion, and locomotion. Scans were made every 1 min (or every 2 min in siamang groups Dr1, Kr1, Kr2, St). Siamang pairs were scanned for synchronization of behavioral categories during blocks of 5 or 10 min, separated by intervals of 20 min. Crested and pileated gibbon pairs were scanned for synchronization in parallel to the focal animal observations of grooming behavior. Each of 13 siamangs pairs was observed for 20 h, except pairs Zu (15 h), Be and Fr (30 h), and Du (40 h). Each crested and pileated gibbon pair was observed for 35 h. The occurrence of synchronized behavior between pair-mates is expressed in % of the total number of scans for a given pair.

We also used scan sampling to record the distance between mates. Distances were recorded to an accuracy of 0.5 m. If the individuals were closer to each other than 0.5 m, we recorded distance according to the following definitions: 0.3 m: shortest distance without body contact, 0.2 m: body contact through extremities, 0 m: body contact through trunk. Siamang pairs were scanned during blocks of 10 min, separated by intervals of at least 10 min. During each scan sampling block, distance was recorded every 10 s. Crested and pileated gibbon pairs were scanned for the distance between mates every 1 min, and scans were carried out in parallel to the focal animal observations of grooming behavior. Each of 17 siamangs pairs was observed for 10 h, except pairs BrA, Bu, DrA (20 h), Be, Du, KrA (30 h), and Fr (210 h). Each crested and pileated gibbon pair was observed for 35 h.

The size of the enclosure varied between zoos (some gibbon groups were held in cages, others on islands). In small cages, the cage walls set outer limits to the inter-individual distances. Because small cages may have forced our pairs into closer proximity than bigger enclosures, we did not directly use absolute inter-individual distances in our comparisons. Instead, we calculated the relative distance (%) between mates, i.e. the inter-individual distance relative to the maximal possible distance in the pair’s given environment (cage or island). This method was described by [44]. In order to test whether cage size had an influence on pair bonding behavior, we used the maximal possible distance in the pair’s given environment as an indicator of cage size. In siamangs, our largest sample, this value ranged from 5.7 m in the smallest cage to 43.5 m on the largest island. We arbitrarily defined cages with values of less than 10 m as “small enclosures”, the others as “large enclosures”.

In addition to determining relative distance, we used scan sampling to estimate the time pair partners spent in each of the following distance classes: 1: body contact or distance of less than 0.3 m, 2: 0.3–1 m, 3: >1 m–3 m, 4: >3 m.

For comparison of our data on partner-directed behavior with literature data, we used male and female proportions of these behavioral variables, where male and female proportions complement each other to 100%. Proportions should be independent of the observation method and permit comparison of data from different observers.

One-sample sign test tests were used to compare classes of sex-specific grooming proportions within genera. For comparison of data among three genera, we used Kruskal-Wallis tests with Dunn’s post hoc tests [70]. In order to compare data between H. pileatus and H. lar (i.e. after inclusion of data compiled from the literature), we used the Mann–Whitney U tests [71]. All tests were two-tailed, and the null hypothesis was rejected at P = 0.05. Statistical were calculated using the software StatView 5.0.1 and SPSS 17.0 on a Macintosh G4 computer.

Advertisement

3. Results

3.1 Strength of pair bonds

3.1.1 Synchronization of behavioral variables

The average degree of behavioral synchronization across 11 behavioral variables is shown in Figure 2. Values range from 15.5% to 63.9% in siamangs, from 8.0% to 38.7% in crested gibbons, and from 19.2% and 42.7%. As shown in Table 1, however, the overall degree of behavioral synchronization does not differ significantly between the genera (Kruskal-Wallis test, P = 0.186).

Figure 2.

Comparison of the average degree of behavioral synchronization between siamangs (Symphalangus, N = 13 pairs), crested gibbons (Nomascus, N = 7 pairs), and pileated gibbons (Hylobates pileatus, N = 9 pairs). Box plots show mean values, standard deviations and minimum and maximum values. The difference between the genera is not statistically significant (Kruskal-Wallis test, P > 0.05, see text).

TaxonKruskal-
Wallis
test (p)
SymphalangusNomascusHylobates
pileatus
Taxon mean35.02 ± 15.39
(N = 14)
23.73 ± 11.30
(N = 7)
32.69 ± 7.95
(N = 9)
0.186

Table 1.

Average degree of synchronization [% ± standerd deviation] across 11 behavioral variables for siamangs (Symphalangus syndactylus), crested gibbons (Nomascus spp.), and pileated gibbons (Hylobates pileatus). Abbreviation: p = error probability.

3.1.2 Relative partner-distance

Average relative partner distances and time proportions spent in four distance classes for each study group are listed in Table 2. Considerable differences were found among pairs. Time spent in distance class 1, for instance, varies from 0.3% to 49.7% in siamangs, from 5.6% to 32.3% in crested gibbons, and from 0.0% to 20.5% in pileated gibbons. Similarly, time spent in distance class 4 varies from 1.3% to 61.2% in siamangs, from 14.1% to 47.4% in crested gibbons, and from 31.6–84% in pileated gibbons. The time gibbon pairs spent in each of the four partner distance classes are shown in Figure 3. The three taxa do not differ significantly among each other in the time groups spent in any of the four partner distance classes (Kruskal-Wallis tests, P > 0.05), except for time spent in distance class 4 (P = 0.014). Dunn post-hoc tests revealed that pileated gibbon pairs spent more time in distance class 4 than siamangs (P < 0.02). Moreover, the difference in distance class 2 is close to significance (P = 0.051).

GroupRelative distanceDistance classes [%]
[%]1234
(a) Siamangs
Antwerp32.8011.9014.0050.0024.10
Berlin Zoo29.8013.7010.0040.8035.50
Branféré 114.505.4019.5031.2043.90
Branféré 212.1010.3029.7023.9036.10
Branféré 318.301.8017.0020.0061.20
Budapest29.3010.7021.1034.1034.10
Dortmund10.1029.5034.8016.6019.10
Dresden 129.0012.5018.1067.501.90
Dresden 224.0012.9031.1054.701.30
Duisburg29.1012.3012.2048.7026.80
Frankfurt40.403.3012.6062.3021.80
Krefeld 130.903.5015.1037.6043.80
Krefeld 235.900.307.4038.6053.70
Munich31.2024.9013.8016.2045.10
Studen11.5049.7022.4019.308.60
Washington26.5020.5014.9021.4043.20
Zurich36.101.3017.8062.3018.60
Mean25.9713.2118.3237.9530.52
(b) Crested gibbons
Duisburg33.685.7039.1941.0414.07
Eberswalde12.1232.3017.0115.6835.01
Mulhouse 130.407.6519.5225.4047.42
Mulhouse 223.3123.6814.3315.8546.14
Mulhouse 321.1230.7314.7917.0337.45
Osnabrück 121.9917.8016.2224.0941.89
Osnabrück 221.635.6430.0527.0137.30
Mean23.4717.6421.5923.7337.04
(c) Pileated gibbons
Phnom Tamao 128.777.3014.2028.2050.30
Phnom Tamao 27.2810.7017.7017.7054.40
Phnom Tamao 341.990.003.9011.3084.80
Phnom Tamao 423.1820.506.0028.9044.60
Phnom Tamao 524.5811.3016.7023.6048.40
Phnom Tamao 612.8213.8011.2026.4048.50
Phnom Tamao 723.135.509.7053.2031.60
Zurich 118.2914.9020.1024.7040.40
Zurich 234.830.907.4017.2047.50
Mean23.479.4311.8825.6950.06

Table 2.

Average relative partner distances and time proportions spent in four distance classes: (a) siamangs (Symphalangus syndactylus, N = 17 groups), (b) crested gibbons (Nomascus spp., N = 7 groups), (c) pileated gibbons (Hylobates pileatus, N = 9 groups).

Figure 3.

Time proportion spent in 4 distance classes (left) and of the mean relative partner distances (right) in siamangs (Symphalangus, N = 17 pairs), crested gibbons (Nomascus, N = 7 pairs), and pileated gibbons (Hylobates pileatus, N = 9 pairs). Box plots show mean values, standard deviations and minimum and maximum values. In a comparison between the genera (Kruskal-Wallis tests), only one of the five variables (distance class 4) are statistically significant (P < 0.05, see text).

The relative distance between pair partners is also shown in Figure 3. The three taxa do not differ in this variable (Kruskal-Wallis test, P > 0.05).

3.1.3 Allogrooming

The number of grooming sessions/hour (average of male and female) varies from 0.0 to 3.9 in siamangs (Symphalangus, N = 12 pairs), from 0.5 to 2.0 in crested gibbons (Nomascus, N = 7 pairs), and from 0.0 to 2.1 in pileated gibbons. The difference is not statistically significant (Kruskal-Wallis test, P > 0.05). The average duration of grooming sessions varies from 0 s to 76.0 s in siamangs, from 50.5 s to 132.1 s in crested gibbons, and from 0 s to 101.0 s in pileated gibbons. This difference is not statistically significant (Kruskal-Wallis test, P > 0.05). The proportion of time spent grooming varies from 0% to 66.9% in siamang pairs, from 9.3% to 28.7% in crested gibbon pairs, and from 0% to 57.7% in pileated gibbons. The difference is not statistically significant (Kruskal-Wallis test, P > 0.05). As a result, siamang pairs, crested gibbon pairs, and pileated gibbon pairs spend similar amounts of time grooming (Figure 4).

Figure 4.

Average intra-pair grooming frequency per hour, mean duration of grooming sessions, and proportion of time spent grooming in siamangs (Symphalangus, N = 11 pairs), crested gibbons (Nomascus, N = 7 pairs), and pileated gibbons (Hylobates pileatus, N = 9 pairs). Box plots show mean values, standard deviations and minimum and maximum values. In a comparison between the genera (Kruskal-Wallis tests), none of the three variables are statistically significant (P > 0.05, see text).

3.2 Mechanism of pair bonds

In order to study which sex invested more in maintaining the pair bond, we determined the %-proportion of partner-directed grooming for each adult. Because male and female proportions in a pair complement each other to 100%, the grooming proportion of one sex will suffice to provide the full information. The results are summarized in Figure 5.

Figure 5.

Average male-female proportions of intra-pair grooming frequency per hour, mean duration of grooming sessions, and time spent grooming in siamangs (Symphalangus, N = 10 pairs), crested gibbons (Nomascus, N = 7 pairs), and pileated gibbons (Hylobates pileatus, N = 8 pairs). Box plots show mean values, standard deviations and minimum and maximum values. In a comparison between the genera (Kruskal-Wallis tests), all three variables are statistically significant (P < 0.05, see text). Abbreviations: M = males, F = females.

In these analyses, one pair of siamangs (Kr2) and one pair of pileated gibbons (PT3) had to be excluded because pair partners were not observed to groom each other at all and male–female proportions of grooming variables could, therefore, not be calculated. Neither Kr2 nor PT3 were newly formed pairs, and the reason why no grooming occurred among pair partners is unknown.

Male proportions in the number of grooming sessions per hour varied from 8.5% to 78.3% in siamangs, from 2.9% to 62.5% in crested gibbons, and from 0.0% to 85.4% in pileated gibbons. The difference between the genera is statistically significant (Kruskal-Wallis test, P = 0.032). The Dunn post-hoc test revealed no significant pair-wise differences, but as a trend, male proportions were higher in siamangs than in pileated gibbons (P < 0.1). Male proportions in grooming session duration varied from 26.7% to 74.6% in siamangs, from 16.6% to 68.2% in crested gibbons, and, and from 0.0% to 48.0% in pileated gibbons. The difference between the genera is statistically significant (Kruskal-Wallis test, P = 0.043), and the Dunn post-hoc test revealed that male proportions were higher in siamangs than in pileated gibbons (P < 0.05). Male proportions in the time spent grooming varied from 3.3% to 90.4% in siamangs, from 0.9% to 69.1% in crested gibbons, and from 0.0% to 84.3% in pileated gibbons. The difference between the genera is statistically significant (Kruskal-Wallis test, P = 0.035), and the Dunn post-hoc test revealed that male proportions were higher in siamangs than in pileated gibbons (P < 0.05). As a result, siamang males groom partners in longer sessions and spend more time grooming them than pileated gibbon males. Only as a trend, siamang males also tend to groom their partners during more grooming sessions than pileated gibbons.

In addition to the grooming data collected by focal animal sampling, we also collected data on male–female grooming proportions for three additional siamang groups (An, Be, Zu) during the scan sampling observations. Male grooming proportions in these groups amounted to 95.4%, 85.7% and 100%, respectively.

Finally, we compiled data from the pertinent literature on other gibbon groups. If several reports were available on the same group, we used the study with the larger data base. These data are summarized in Table 3 and also includes members of the dwarf gibbons (Hylobates) and hoolock gibbons (Hoolock) other that the species observed by us. The sample size for the hoolocks (Table 3d), however, comprises only three groups and is too small for statistical analysis. Pairs that did not exhibit partner-directed grooming are also excluded from the analysis. Our resulting sample comprises 76 pairs. For summary statistics, we split male grooming contribution evenly into three classes: (1) 0–33%, (2) >33–66%, (3) >66%. Pairs should be evenly distributed across these classes if male and female contributions were balanced. As shown in Table 3, this is not the case in siamangs (N = 28). Most pairs fall into class 3, suggesting that siamang males, as a rule, provide most of the intra-pair grooming. In crested gibbons (N = 22) and dwarf gibbons (N = 26), the situation is exactly reversed. Most pairs fall into class 1, indicating that females provide most of the intra-pair grooming in Nomascus and Hylobates. The difference from the expected value of 50% is statistically significant for the genera Nomascus and Symphalangus (One-sample sign test, P = 0.002, and P = 0.013), but not for Hylobates (One-sample sign test, P > 0.05). As indicated by the species labels in Figure 6c, the distribution appears to differ among species of the genus Hylobates. Whereas partner-directed grooming is mainly provided by females in H. pileatus (N = 11), the distribution appears to be more randomly distributed in H. lar (N = 11). Although the difference between the two species is statistically significant (Mann–Whitney U test, P = 0.032), the samples are relatively small and the result should be regarded with caution. If only H. pileatus is considered, the difference from the expected value of 50% is still not significant (One-sample sign test, P > 0.05), but the sample is very small in this case (N = 11).

GroupCap- tive/wildData typeClasses of male grooming proportion [%]Source
123
(a) Siamangs (Symphalangus)
Ancf95.4ts
Becf85.7ts
Br1cf8.5ts
Br2cf78.3ts
Br3cf74.1ts
Bucf29.5ts
Docf60.1ts
Dr1cf69.7ts
Dr2cf51.5ts
Ducf49.2ts
Frcfts
Kr1cf76.7ts
Kr2cf72.0ts
Mucf8.5ts
Stcf78.3ts
Wacf74.1ts
Zhcf100.0ts
TS1wt60.5[19]
RS2wt73.7[19]
Milwaukeecf26.0[72]
Tulsacf?88.8[73]
Berlincf[74]
Cheyenne, MH 21ct86.3[75]
Cheyenne, MH 23ct7.0[75]
Melbournec?60.8[76]
Ketambe, CH-CJwfca 48.0[10]
Ketambe, PP-PNwfca 84.0[10]
Ketambe, Pm-Pnwfca 60.0[10]
Lourosa, pair 1ct88.9[77]
ICGSct56.9[26]
Siamangs, total number of pairs5815
(b) Crested gibbons (Nomascus)
Du (Nle)cf4.4ts
Eb (Nga)cf24.7ts
Mu1 (Nle)cf62.5ts
Mu2 (Nsi)cf20.6ts
Mu3 (Nga)cf22.1ts
Os1 (Nga)cf38.3ts
Os2 (Nle)cf2.9ts
Twycross (Nco + Nle mixed pair)ct17.7[78]
Twycross (Nle)ct18.0[78]
Perth, old pair (Nle)ct36.4[79]
Perth, new pair (Nle)ct50.0[79]
Perth, family gr. (Nle)ct37.9[79]
Melbourne (Nle)cf19.5[76]
Besançon (Nga)ct6.0[80]
Mulhouse, group 1 (Nga)ct0[81]
Mulhouse, group 2 (Nle)ct[81]
Mulhouse, group 3 (Nsi)ct0[81]
Amsterdam (Nle)ct16.0[82]
Beekse Bergen (Nle)ct22.2[82]
Hannover (Nle)ct57.5[82]
ICGS (Nle)ct100[26]
Lincoln Park (Nle)cf0[83]
San Antonio (Nle)c?1.6[84, 85]
Nomascus gabriellae, total number of pairs410
Nomascus leucogenys, total number of pairs851
Crested gibbons, total number of pairs1561
(c) Dwarf gibbons (Hylobates)
Phnom Tamao 1 (Hpi)`cf55.7ts
Phnom Tamao 2 (Hpi)cf0ts
Phnom Tamao 3 (Hpi)cfts
Phnom Tamao 4 (Hpi)cf8.1ts
Phnom Tamao 5 (Hpi)cf0ts
Phnom Tamao 6 (Hpi)cf85.4ts
Phnom Tamao 7 (Hpi)cf12.5ts
Zurich 1 (Hpi)cf38.2ts
Zurich 2 (Hpi)cf0ts
ICGS (Hpi)ct69.6[26]
Perth, group 6 (Hpi)cf?0[86]
Blackpool (Hpi)cf0[87]
Hylobates pileatus, total number of pairs722
Bronx, New York (Hla)ct71.3[88]
Portland, Oregon (Hla)cf45.0[89]
Berlin (Hla)cf25.0[74]
Besançon (Hla)ct53.6[80]
Melbourne (Hla)cf10.8[76]
Ketambe, AS-AY (Hla)wfca 81.0[10]
Ketambe, GD-GM (Hla)wf93.0[10]
Khao Yai, Pair A (Hla)wt64.3[13]
Khao Yai, Pair B (Hla)wt37.5[13]
Khao Yai, Pair C (Hla)wt71.4[13]
Khao Yai, Pair T (Hla)wt20.0[90]
Hylobates lar, total number of pairs344
Berlin (Hmo)cf[74]
ICGS (Hmo)ct0[26]
Munich (Hmo)cf50.7Average of [91, 92, 93]
Perth (Hmo)ct50.0[79]
Hylobates moloch, total number of pairs120
ICGS (Hag)ct0[26]
Dwarf gibbons, total number of pairs1286
(d) Hoolock gibbons (Hoolock)
Gibbon Wildlife Sanctuary, Assam, 1 (Hho)wf25.0[94]
Gibbon Wildlife Sanctuary, Assam, 2 (Hho)wf50.0[94]
Gibbon Wildlife Sanctuary, Assam, 3 (Hho)wf41.5[94]
Hoolock gibbons, total number of pairs120

Table 3.

Male contributions (%) to intra-pair grooming in gibbons. Classes of male grooming proportion are defined as (1) 0–33%, (2) >33–66% and (3) >66%. Abbreviations: Hoolock: Hho = H. hoolock; Hylobates: Hag = H. agilis, Hla = H. lar, Hmo = H. moloch, Hpi = H. pileatus. Nomascus: Nco = N. concolor, Nga = N. gabriellae, Nle = N. leucogenys, Nsi = N. siki. Captive/wild: c = captive, w = wild. Data type: f = frequency, t = time. Grooming: – = no partner-directed grooming observed. Source: ts = this study.

Figure 6.

Male contributions to intra-pair grooming in gibbons. (a) Siamangs (Symphalangus, N = 28 pairs); (b) crested gibbons (Nomascus, N = 18 pairs); (c) dwarf gibbons (Hylobates, N = 23 pairs); (d) hoolock gibbons (Hoolock, N = 3 pairs). Abbreviations in (c) identify the following species: a – H. agilis, l – H. lar, m – H. moloch, and p – H. pileatus.

Especially in siamangs and crested gibbons, the unilateral distribution of male grooming proportion is surprisingly consistent. We wondered whether there was something about the pairs which do not exhibit consistent results. Of the gibbons we observed, only the siamang sample was large enough to test several potential influences statistically. In the 16 siamang pairs that showed grooming, “Having infants” had no influence on the proportion of male grooming (Mann–Whitney U test, 16 pairs, U = 15.5, P > 0.05). However, “Having a family group” did: Pairs without a family showed a smaller proportion of male grooming than pairs with offspring in the family (Mann–Whitney U test, 16 pairs, U = 11.0, P = 0.03). We also wondered whether there were any differences between pairs kept in smaller cages and pairs kept in bigger enclosures. In order to study the effect of cage size on the male proportion of pair-grooming in siamang pairs, we used the maximal possible distance in the pair’s given environment as an indicator of cage size. We compared male grooming proportion between siamangs kept in small enclosures (N = 9 groups) to siamangs kept in large enclosures (N = 7 groups). The difference was not statistically significant (Mann–Whitney U test, P > 0.05). The correlation between cage size and male grooming proportion was also not significant (Spearman rank correlation, Rho = −0.165, P > 0.05).

Results for the dwarf gibbons are less consistent than those for siamangs or crested gibbons (Table 3). Could the differences within the first two genera be influenced by wild vs. captive gibbons? In siamangs, captive pairs did not differ from wild ones (Mann–Whitney U-test, 23 captive pairs vs. 5 wild pairs, U = 51.0, P > 0.05). In dwarf gibbons, on the other hand, captive pairs differ significantly from wild ones (Mann–Whitney U test, 20 captive pairs vs. 6 wild pairs, U = 24.0, P = 0.027). It should be noted, however, that all available data for wild dwarf gibbons stem from only one species (H. lar), whereas several other species are represented in the captive sample of the same genus. If the comparison is restricted to Hylobates lar, the difference is not statistically significant (Mann–Whitney U test, 5 captive pairs vs. 6 wild pairs, U = 8.0, P > 0.05). Therefore, the variability of male grooming proportion among dwarf gibbons may be influenced by, and differ among, the species.

The frequency distribution of male grooming proportion is shown in Figure 6. These data differ significantly among the genera (Kruskal-Wallis test, df = 2, P < 0.0001). As revealed by the Dunn post-hoc tests, the male proportion in partner grooming is significantly higher in Symphalangus than in both Nomascus (P < 0.001) and Hylobates (P < 0.005), whereas no differences were found between Hylobates and Nomascus (P > 0.05).

Advertisement

4. Discussion

Monogamy is common among birds [95], but established in only about 3–9% of all mammals and about 15–29% of all primate species [45, 48, 96]. Among hominoid apes, only gibbons typically live in social monogamy (in the sense of [46]).

Various hypotheses explaining the proximate and ultimate mechanisms, which led to the evolution of social monogamy among gibbons are under debate [48, 97, 98, 99]. In these discussions, monogamy among gibbons is usually treated as, and implicitly assumed to be, a comparable, uniform entity. Cowlishaw [100], for instance, assumes that the pair bond is created by the different resource interests of the partners. The female is interested in the territory and the food resources in it, whereas the male is interested in the female partner.

Although several reports suggested that gibbon taxa might exhibit subtle distinctions in their group coherence or group composition (see Introduction), quantitative data for representative numbers of pairs have been lacking. It is generally assumed that pair bonds in all gibbon taxa are built up and maintained in the same way, and that males are mainly responsible for maintaining the pair bonds [3, 52].

As will be discussed below, this study provides evidence to the contrary. We compared indicators of pair bond strength and sex-specific pair bond investment between 7 pairs of crested gibbons, 9 pairs of pileated gibbons, and 11–17 pairs of siamangs (depending on the variable in question).

4.1 Pair bond strength

We determined three variables to compare pair bond strength between siamangs and crested gibbons (synchronization of behavioral variables, relative partner-distance, and allogrooming).

  1. Synchronization of behavioral variables: The overall degree of behavioral synchronization does not differ significantly among the genera, suggesting that they do not differ in the strength of the pair bond as expressed by behavioral synchronization.

  2. Relative partner-distance: The three gibbon taxa did not differ in the time spent in any of the four partner distance classes, except that siamang pairs spent less time in the largest distance class 4 (>3 m) than pileated gibbon (H. pileatus) pairs, suggesting that pair bond strength in siamangs may be more pronounced than in pileated gibbons. Similarly, Palombit [22] found that siamang pairs spent significantly more time in close proximity to one another than white-handed gibbons (H. lar). However, we found no significant differences in the other distance classes or in the mean relative distance between pair partners.

  3. Allogrooming: The three gibbon taxa did not differ in the number of grooming sessions/hour (average male and female), the proportion of time spent grooming, and the average duration of grooming sessions. As a result, siamang pairs, crested gibbon pairs and pileated gibbon pairs are involved in similar numbers of grooming sessions and spend similar amounts of time grooming.

In summary, pileated gibbons appear to spend more time apart by the largest distance class than siamangs. Based on this variable alone, their pair bond may be weaker than that of siamangs. No consistent differences in pair bond strength were found between siamangs and crested gibbons or between crested gibbons and pileated gibbons.

4.2 Pair bond maintenance

We examined which sex invests more in the pair bond by measuring the amount of grooming directed at the respective partner. For simplicity, we indicate the male proportion only; the female partner’s proportion is its complement to 100%.

Our results show that in pileated and crested gibbon pairs partner-directed grooming is mostly provided by females, whereas males are the main groomers in siamang pairs. This result is further supported by additional data we collected from the literature. In most siamang pairs, males are the main groomers. Furthermore, male proportion in grooming session duration and time spent grooming are higher in siamangs than in pileated gibbons, whereas the male proportions in the numbers of grooming sessions per hour do not differ between siamangs and pileated gibbons. Siamang males groom their partners more often than crested gibbon males do, but time spent grooming and male proportion in duration of grooming do not differ between siamangs and crested gibbons. Our pairwise comparison revealed statistically significant differences for Symphalangus/Nomascus, but not for Symphalangus/Hylobates or Hylobates/Nomascus.

These results suggest that each genus differs in the mechanism of how pair bonds are created or maintained. Especially siamangs differ compared to pileated and crested gibbons: male-driven in the former, female-driven in the latter two. Obviously, the pair bond in gibbons does not appear to be a uniform entity. Date compiled in Table 3 also suggest that field and zoo observations are consistent (except that only one wild pair exhibits a “Class 1” male grooming proportion of 0–33%).

Our results support vocal and molecular studies suggesting that gibbons are a much less homogenous group than generally assumed [39, 101, 102]. It is becoming more and more obvious that including one gibbon taxon into comparative studies in order to represent “the gibbon” is not useful practice anymore.

In our overall sample of dwarf gibbon pairs (Hylobates, N = 26) as well as in the subset of H. pileatus-pairs (N = 11), females provided more partner-grooming than males in most pairs. In H. lar-pairs (N = 11), on the other hand, the amount of grooming provided by males and females was very variable (Table 3) and the reason for this variability in this sample is not clear.

Kleiman [45] proposed that males should be the more active groomers in monogamous primates because their dominance situation is reversed as compared to primates with polygynous social organizations. Simple dominance relationships, however, do not seem the only variables influencing partner-directed allogrooming in gibbons.

If partner-directed allogrooming reflects the investment into a pair bond [6263], then our results document that the readiness to invest differs among pairs. In most (but not necessarily all) pairs, both partners appeared to be interested in maintaining the pair pond, and both partners provided at least some allogrooming. In addition to individual differences, the interest in a pair partner may vary with time. Probably, the benefit of a pair bond is related to the reproductive potential of a partner. Observations on wild H. lar and H. moloch suggest that the reproductive status of females may play an important role [13, 103, 104]. Males may have a higher interest to invest into the pair bond with females when they are receptive, in order to guard them more efficiently, copulate more frequently and improve the probability of their paternity. If partner-directed grooming is part of a mate-guarding strategy with fluctuating relevance to the groomer, it becomes clear why data of relatively large numbers of pairs need to be compared in order to discover species-specific differences.

How do our findings compare to the predictions of the three hypotheses for the evolution of pair bonds presented in the Introduction?

  1. The ‘male-services hypothesis’ predicts that a female will invest substantially in a social relationship with a male willing to assume the costs of territorial or antipredator defense, infant care or protection from infanticidal males. This should result in females investing more than males in maintaining the pair bond. This prediction is met by our samples of crested gibbons (N = 22 pairs, Table 3), pileated gibbons (N = 11 pairs) and the combined sample of all dwarf gibbons (N = 26 pairs). In all three samples, females were the main groomers in most pairs.

  2. The ‘mate-defense hypothesis’ predicts that bonding with a female is beneficial for a male when either the spatial distribution of females or the temporal distribution of fertile periods makes it difficult for the males to defend access to more than one female at a time. This should result in males investing more than females in maintaining the pair bond. This prediction is met by our sample of siamangs (N = 28 pairs), where males were the main groomers in most pairs.

  3. The ‘resource-defense hypothesis’ predicts that both a male and a female benefit from pair bonding to defend resources together. This should result in a male and a female being equally interested in maintaining proximity and affiliation with a pair mate and defending their territory. None of the gibbon samples of this study appears to meet this prediction.

Only very limited information on the direction of partner-grooming is available for the fourth of the gibbon genera, the hoolocks (genus Hoolock). Ahsan [105], who studied three groups of the western hoolock gibbon (H. hoolock) at two sites in Bangladesh, reported that grooming was most frequent between adult pairs and that it was “mostly performed by the adult male”. Unfortunately, the author did not publish the quantitative data in support of his statement. Sankaran [94] observed three groups of the same species in the Gibbon Wildlife Sanctuary in Assam. However, none of his males provided more than 50% of the partner-grooming (Table 3). Apparently, the results of the two studies differ, but the overall sample size is too small to assess the directionality of partner grooming in hoolock gibbons with any reliability.

It has also been reported that allogrooming between pair mates is virtually nonexistent in wild Hylobates agilis [21] and H. klossii [106], in contrast to the situation in wild H. lar and siamangs [10, 13, 19, 103]. This suggests that the range of variation in gibbon pair bonds may be larger than what we covered in our study. Several species of dwarf gibbons (Hylobates) are hardly represented or not represented at all in our data, including H. agilis and H. klossii.

Within crested gibbons (Nomascus), most of our data are from one species, N. leucogenys (N = 14 pairs), whereas few pairs of other light-cheeked species and only one male of a black-cheeked species (N. concolor in a mixed pair) are available.

Advertisement

5. Conclusions

  1. A comparison of pair bond strength in three gibbon taxa – siamangs (Symphalangus), crested gibbons (Nomascus) and pileated gibbons (Hylobates pileatus) revealed a difference in relative partner distances between siamangs and pileated gibbons, suggesting that siamangs may have a stronger pair bond than pileated gibbons. No difference between the three taxa was found in other variables believed to indicate pair bond strength: degree of behavioral synchronization and amounts of grooming (both numbers of events and actual grooming time).

  2. This study provides the first statistically significant evidence that the mechanisms of how pair bonds are created or maintained, differ between gibbon taxa. As indicated by the amount of partner-directed grooming, siamang males invest significantly more into the pair bond than females, whereas the opposite is true in crested gibbons, pileated gibbons, and an enlarged sample of dwarf gibbons (genus Hylobates). Additional species-specific differences may, however, occur within the latter group, with partner-grooming investment being highly variable in H. lar.

  3. Our results for crested gibbons, pileated gibbons, and a combined sample of dwarf gibbons correspond to predictions derived from the ‘male-services hypothesis’ for the evolution of pair bonds. According to this hypothesis, a female will invest substantially in a social relationship with a male willing to assume the costs of territorial or antipredator defense, infant care or protection from infanticidal males.

  4. In contrast, our results for siamangs correspond to predictions derived from the ‘mate-defense hypothesis’. According to this hypothesis, bonding with a female is beneficial for a male when either the spatial distribution of females or the temporal distribution of fertile periods makes it difficult for the males to defend access to more than one female at a time.

  5. Species-specific analyses are recommended for additional species of the genera Hylobates, Nomascus (especially the black-cheeked taxa) and Hoolock.

Advertisement

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the staff members of the numerous zoos for permission to study the gibbons in their care. We also would like to thank Kim J.J.M. Nouwen for her contribution in the data collection for our sample of pileated gibbons (Hylobates pileatus). We are grateful to Linda Burman-Hall for supporting this publication and for helpful comments on an earlier version of this manuscript.

Advertisement

Conflict of interest

There is no financial/personal interest, or contractual employment involving matters in this article or belief that could affect the authors’ objectivity.

References

  1. 1. Tuttle, RH. Apes of the World. Their Social Behavior, Communication, Mentality and Ecology. Park Ridge, NJ: Noyes Publications; 1986. xix+421 p.
  2. 2. Fleagle JG. Primate Adaptation and Evolution. 3rd ed. San Diego and London: Academic Press; 2013. 441 p.
  3. 3. Palombit RA. Infanticide and the evolution of pair bonds in nonhuman primates. Evolutionary Anthropology. 1999;7:117-129. DOI: 10.1002/(SICI)1520-6505(1999)7:4<117::AID-EVAN2>3.0.CO;2-O
  4. 4. Chivers DJ. The Lesser Apes. In: Prince Rainier III of Monaco, Bourne GH, editors. Primate Conservation. New York: Academic Press; 1977. p. 539-598.
  5. 5. Leighton DR. Gibbons: Territoriality and monogamy. In: Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW, Struhsaker TT, editors. Primate Societies. Chicago and London: University of Chicago Press; 1987. p. 135-145.
  6. 6. Chivers DJ, Anandam MV, Groves CP, Molur S, Rawson BM, Richardson MC, Roos C, Whittaker DJ. Family Hylobatidae (Gibbons). In: Mittermeier RA, Rylands AB, Wilson DE, editors. Handbook of the Mammals of the World, Vol 3: Primates. Barcelona: Lynx Edicions; 2013. p. 754-791.
  7. 7. Lappan S. Social relationships among males in multimale siamang groups. International Journal of Primatology. 2007;28:369-387. DOI: 10.1007/s10764-007-9122-z
  8. 8. Whittaker DJ, Lappan S. The diversity of small apes and the importance of population-level studies. In: Lappan S, Whittacker DJ, editors. The Gibbons: New Perspectives on Small Ape Socioecology and Population Biology. New York: Springer; 2009. p. 3-10.
  9. 9. Lappan S. Biparental care and male reproductive strategies in siamangs (Symphalangus syndactylus) in southern Sumatra [Ph.D. thesis]. New York: Department of Anthropology, New York University; 2005.
  10. 10. Palombit RA. Pair bonds and monogamy in wild siamang (Hylobates syndactylus) and white-handed gibbon (Hylobates lar) in northern Sumatra [Ph.D. thesis]. Davis: University of California; 1992.
  11. 11. Palombit RA. Dynamic pair bonds in hylobatids: Implications regarding monogamous social systems. Behaviour. 1994;128:65-101. DOI: 10.1163/156853994X00055
  12. 12. Palombit RA. Extra-pair copulations in a monogamous ape. Animal Behaviour. 1994;47:721-723. DOI: 10.1006/anbe.1994.1097
  13. 13. Reichard U. Sozial- und Fortpflanzungsverhalten von Weisshandgibbons (Hylobates lar): Eine Freilandstudie im thailändischen Khao Yai Regenwald. [Ph.D. thesis]. Göttingen: Mathematisch-Naturwissenschaftliche Fachbereiche, Georg-August-Universität; 1995.
  14. 14. Reichard UH. The social organization and mating system of Khao Yai white-handed gibbons: 1992-2006. In: Lappan S, Whittacker DJ, editors. The Gibbons: New Perspectives on Small Ape Socioecology and Population Biology. New York: Springer; 2009. p. 347-384.
  15. 15. Savini T, Boesch C, Reichard R. Varying ecological quality influences the probability of polyandry in white-handed gibbons (Hylobates lar) in Thailand. Biotropica. 2009;41:503-513. DOI: 10.1111/j.1744-7429.2009.00507.x
  16. 16. Bartlett TQ. Intragroup and intergroup social interactions in white-handed gibbons. International Journal of Primatology. 2003;24:239-259. DOI: 10.1023/A:1023088814263
  17. 17. Chivers DJ. Communication within and between family groups of siamang (Symphalangus syndactylus). Behaviour. 1976;57:116-135. DOI: 10.1163/156853976X00136
  18. 18. Chivers DJ. The siamang and the gibbon in the Malay peninsula. In: Rumbaugh DM, editor. Gibbon and Siamang, Vol. 1. Basel and New York: Karger; 1972. p. 103-135.
  19. 19. Chivers DJ. The Siamang in Malaya: A Field Study of a Primate in Tropical Rain Forest (Contributions to Primatology vol. 4). Basel and New York, Karger; 1974. xiii+335 p.
  20. 20. Chivers DJ. Gibbons. In: MacDonald, D., editor. The Encyclopedia of Mammals, Vol. 1. London: Allen & Unwin; 1984. p. 415-419.
  21. 21. Gittins SP, Raemaekers JJ. Siamang, lar and agile gibbons. In: Chivers DJ, editor. Malayan Forest Primates: Ten Years’ Study in Tropical Rain Forest. New York: Plenum Press; 1980. p. 63-105.
  22. 22. Palombit RA. Pair bonds in monogamous apes: A comparison of the siamang Hylobates syndactylus and the white-handed gibbons Hylobates lar. Behaviour. 1996;133:321-356. DOI: 10.1163/156853996X00486
  23. 23. Fischer JO, Geissmann T. Group harmony in gibbons: Comparison between white-handed gibbon (Hylobates lar) and siamang (H. syndactylus). Primates. 1990;31:481-494. DOI: 10.1007/BF02382532
  24. 24. Jones AR. Infant transfer in a captive family of siamangs (Hylobates syndactylus) [Master’s thesis]. Monroe: Faculty of the Graduate School, Northeast Louisiana University; 1995.
  25. 25. Lappan S. Patterns of infant care in wild siamangs (Symphalangus syndactylus) in southern Sumatra. In: Lappan S, Whittacker DJ, editors. The Gibbons: New Perspectives on Small Ape Socioecology and Population Biology. New York: Springer; 2009. p. 327-345.
  26. 26. Eardley J. Attachment and social relationships between members of gibbon and siamang families with a discussion on human-infant attachment figures and implications of these relationships [Master’s thesis]. Fullerton: California State University; 2000.
  27. 27. Fan P-F, Jiang X-L, Liu C-M, Luo W-S. Polygynous mating system and behavioural reason of black crested gibbon (Nomascus concolor jingdongensis) at Dazhaizi, Mt. Wuliang, Yunnan, China. Zoological Research. 2006;27:216-220 (Mandarin text, English abstract).
  28. 28. Fan P-F, Bartlett TQ, Fei H-L, Ma C-Y, Zhang W. Understanding stable bi-female grouping in gibbons: Feeding competition and reproductive success. Frontiers in Zoology. 2015;12:5. DOI 10.1186/s12983-015-0098-9
  29. 29. Haimoff EH, Yang X-J, He S-J, Chen N. Census and survey of wild black-crested gibbons (Hylobates concolor concolor) in Yunnan Province, People’s Republic of China. Folia Primatologica. 1986;46:205-214. DOI: 10.1159/000156254
  30. 30. Haimoff EH, Yang X-J, He S-J, Chen N. Preliminary observations of wild black-crested gibbons (Hylobates concolor concolor) in Yunnan Province, People’s Republic of China. Primates. 1987;28:319-335. DOI: 10.1007/BF02381015
  31. 31. Jiang XL, Wang YX. Population and conservation of black-crested gibbon (Hylobates concolor jingdongensis) in Wuliang Nature Reserve, Yunnan. Zoological Research. 1999;20:421-425.
  32. 32. Liu Z, Jiang H, Zhang Y, Liu Y, Chou T, Manry D, Southwick C. Field report on the Hainan gibbon. Primate Conservation. 1987; 8: 49-50.
  33. 33. Liu Z, Zhang Y, Jiang H, Southwick C. Population structure of Hylobates concolor in Bawanglin Nature Reserve, Hainan, China. American Journal of Primatology. 1989;19:247-254. DOI: 10.1002/ajp.1350190406
  34. 34. Geissmann T, Nguyen Xuan Dang, Lormée, N, Momberg, F. Vietnam Primate Conservation Status Review 2000. Part 1: Gibbons (English Edition). Hanoi: Fauna & Flora International, Indochina Programme; 2000. 135 p.
  35. 35. Kenyon MA. The ecology of the golden-cheeked gibbon (Nomascus gabriellae) in Cat Tien National Park, Vietnam [Ph.D. thesis]. Cambridge: Department of Anatomy, University of Cambridge; 2007.
  36. 36. Barca B, Vincent C, Khang Soeung, Nuttall M, Hobson K. Multi-female group in the southernmost species of Nomascus: Field observations in eastern Cambodia reveal multiple breeding females in a single group of southern yellow-cheeked crested gibbon Nomascus gabriellae. Asian Primates Journal. 2016;6:15-19.
  37. 37. Dallmann R, Geissmann T. Different levels of variability in the female song of wild silvery gibbons (Hylobates moloch). Behaviour. 2001;138,629-648. DOI: 10.1163/156853901316924511
  38. 38. Dallmann R, Geissmann T. Individuality in the female songs of wild silvery gibbons (Hylobates moloch) on Java, Indonesia. Contributions to Zoology. 2001; 70: 41-50. DOI: 10.1163/18759866-07001003
  39. 39. Geissmann T. Duet-splitting and the evolution of gibbon songs. Biological Reviews. 2002;77:57-76. DOI: 10.1017/s1464793101005826
  40. 40. Geissmann T, Bohlen-Eyring S, and Heuck A. The male song of the Javan silvery gibbon (Hylobates moloch). Contributions to Zoology. 2005;74:1-25. DOI: 10.1163/18759866-0740102001
  41. 41. Geissmann T. Duet songs of the siamang, Hylobates syndactylus: I. Structure and organisation. Primate Report. 2000;56:33-60.
  42. 42. Geissmann T. Gibbon songs and human music from an evolutionary perspective. In: Wallin NL, Merker B, Brown S, editors. The Origins of Music. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press; 2000. p. 103-123. DOI: 10.7551/mitpress/5190.003.0011
  43. 43. Geissmann T. Duet songs of the siamang, Hylobates syndactylus: II. Testing the pair-bonding hypothesis during a partner exchange. Behaviour. 1999;136:1005-1039. DOI: 10.1163/156853999501694
  44. 44. Geissmann T, Orgeldinger M. The relationship between duet songs and pair bonds in siamangs, Hylobates syndactylus. Animal Behaviour. 2000;60:805-809. DOI: 10.1006/anbe.2000.1540
  45. 45. Kleiman DG. Monogamy in mammals. Quarterly Review of Biology. 1977;52:39-69. DOI: 10.1086/409721
  46. 46. Reichard UH. Monogamy: Past and present. In: Reichard UH., Boesch C., editors. Monogamy: Mating Strategies and Partnerships in Birds, Humans and other Mammals. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2003. p. 3-25. DOI: 10.1017/CBO9781139087247.001
  47. 47. Dolotovskaya S, Walker S, Heymann EW. What makes a pair bond in a Neotropical primate: Female and male contributions. Royal Society Open Science. 2020;7:191489. DOI: 10.1098/rsos.191489
  48. 48. van Schaik CP, Dunbar RIM. The evolution of monogamy in large primates: A new hypothesis and some crucial tests. Behaviour. 1990;115:30-62. Doi:10.1163/156853990X00284
  49. 49. Emlen ST, Oring LW. Ecology, sexual selection, and the evolution of mating systems. Science. 1977;197:215-223. Doi:10.1126/science.327542
  50. 50. Birkhead TR, Møller AP. Sperm Competition in Birds: Evolutionary Causes and Consequences. London: Academic Press; 1992. 288 p.
  51. 51. Opie C, Atkinson QD, Dunbar RIM, Shultz S. Male infanticide leads to social monogamy in primates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA. 2013;110:13328-13332. Doi:10.1073/pnas.1307903110
  52. 52. Palombit RA. Infanticide and the evolution of male-female bonds in animals. In: van Schaik CP, Janson CH, editors. Infanticide by Males and its Implications. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2000. p. 239-268. DOI: 10.1017/CBO9780511542312.013
  53. 53. Orgeldinger M. Paarbeziehung beim Siamang-Gibbon (Hylobates syndactylus) im Zoo: Untersuchung über den Einfluss von Jungtieren auf die Paarbindung. Münster: Schüling Verlag; 1999.
  54. 54. Akinyi MY, Tung J, Jeneby, M, Patel NB, Altmann J, Alberts, SC. Role of grooming in reducing tick load in wild baboons (Papio cynocephalus). Animal Behaviour. 2013;85:559-568. DOI: 0.1016/j.anbehav.2012.12.012
  55. 55. Aureli F, Preston SD., de Waal FBM. Heart rate responses to social interactions in free-moving rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta): A pilot study. Journal of Comparative Psychology. 1999;113:59-65. DOI: 10.1037/0735-7036.113.1.59
  56. 56. Barton R. Grooming site preferences in primates and their functional implications. International Journal Primatology. 1985;6:519-532. DOI: 10.1007/BF02735574
  57. 57. Goosen C. Social grooming in primates. In: Mitchell G, Erwin J, editors. Comparative Primatology, Vol. 2B: Behavior, Cognition, and Motivation. New York: Alan R. Liss; 1987. p. 107-131.
  58. 58. Hutchins M, Barash DP. Grooming in primates: Implications for its utilitarian function. Primates. 1976;17:145-150. DOI: 10.1007/BF02382848
  59. 59. McFarland R., Henzi SP, Barrett L, Wanigaratne A, Coetzee E, Fuller A, Hetem RS, Mitchell D, and Maloney SK. Thermal consequences of increased pelt loft infer an additional utilitarian function for grooming. American Journal of Primatology. 2016;78:456-461. DOI: 10.1002/ajp.22519
  60. 60. Harrison CJO. Allopreening as agonistic behaviour. Behaviour. 1965;24:161-209. DOI: 10.1163/156853965X00011
  61. 61. Kunkel P. Mating systems of tropical birds: The effects of weakness or absence of external reproduction-timing factors with special reference to prolonged pair bonds. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie. 1974;34:265-307. DOI: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1974.tb01802.x
  62. 62. O’Brien TG. Allogrooming behaviour among adult female wedge-capped capuchin monkeys. Animal Behaviour. 1993;46:499-510. DOI: 10.1006/anbe.1993.1218
  63. 63. Seyfarth RM. A model of social grooming among adult female monkeys. Journal of Theoretical Biology. 1977;65:671-698. DOI: 10.1016/0022-5193(77)90015-7
  64. 64. Colvin J. Familiarity, rank and structure of rhesus male peer networks. In: Hinde RA, editor. Primate Social Relationships: An Integrated Approach. Sunderland: Sinauer Associates; 1983. p. 190-200.
  65. 65. Burgin CJ, Wilson DE, Mittermeier RA, Rylands AB, Lacher TE, Sechrest W. Illustrated Checklist of the Mammals of the World, Vol. 1: Monotremata to Rodentia. Barcelona: Lynx Editions; 2020. 631 p.
  66. 66. Martin P, Bateson P. Measuring Behaviour. An Introductory Guide, 3rd edition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2007. xi+176 p.
  67. 67. Altmann J. Observational study of behavior: Sampling methods. Behaviour. 1974;49:227-267. DOI: 10.1163/156853974X00534
  68. 68. Geissmann T. Verhaltensbiologische Forschungsmethoden: Eine Einführung. Münster: Schüling Verlag; 2002. 54 p.
  69. 69. Lehner PN. Handbook of Ethological Methods. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1998. 694 p.
  70. 70. Zar JH. Biostatistical Analysis. 4th ed. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall; 1999. 663 p.
  71. 71. Siegel S, Castellan NJ Jr. Nonparametric Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd Edition. New York: McGraw Hill; 1988. 330 p.
  72. 72. Fox GJ. Social dynamics in siamang [Ph.D. thesis]. Milwaukee: University of Wisconsin; 1977.
  73. 73. Kawata K. Notes on comparative behavior in three primate species in captivity. Der Zoologische Garten (N.F.). 1980;50:209-224.
  74. 74. Dielentheis TF, Zaiss E. Lokomotions- und Sozialverhalten dreier Hylobatiden-Arten [Praktikumsarbeit]. Berlin: AG Humanbiologie, Institut für Tierphysiologie, Freie Universität Berlin; 1987.
  75. 75. Philippart M. Captive siamang monogamous pairs at the Cheyenne Mountain Zoo [Undergraduate research report, primatology]. Colorado Springs: Anthropology Department, The Colorado College; 1991.
  76. 76. Bricknell S. A comparative study of the behaviour in captivity of three species of gibbon: Hylobates syndactylus, Hylobates leucogenys and Hylobates lar. BA [Honour’s thesis]. Canberra: Department of Archaeology and Anthropology, Australian National University; 1992.
  77. 77. Caeiro Pereira de Sousa A. Estudo e comparação dos laços de união (“pair bonds”) em dois casais de siamangs (Hylobates syndactylus) em cativeiro [M.Sc. thesis]. Lisbon, Portugal: Instituto Superior de Psicologia Aplicada; 1998.
  78. 78. Pollard GM. The behaviour of captive hylobatids and a comparison with the behaviour of those species in the wild [B.Sc. (Honours Biol. Sci.) thesis]. Wolverhampton, UK: Polytechnic Wolverhampton; 1983.
  79. 79. Dooley H. Social behaviour in gibbon groups of gibbons (Hylobates leucogenys and Hylobates moloch) at Perth Zoo [Honour’s thesis]. Perth: School of Anatomy and Human Biology, The University of Western Australia; 2006.
  80. 80. Hénon C. Étude des interactions comportementales dans le cadre d’un déplacement induisant la proximité spatiale de deux espèces proches de gibbon [Maîtrise de biologie des populations et des écosystèmes]. Besançon: Université de Franche-comté; 2002.
  81. 81. Schlegel K. Ethologische und endokrinologische Untersuchungen an Schopfgibbons Hylobates concolor in Gefangenschaft [Lizentiatsarbeit thesis]. Zurich: Psychologisches Institut, Biologisch-mathematische Abteilung, Philosophische Fakultät I, Zurich University; 1995.
  82. 82. Hold A. Das Verhaltensrepertoire des Weisswangen-Schopfgibbons (Hylobates leucogenys) [Diploma thesis]. Hannover: Institut für Zoologie, Tierärztliche Hochschule Hannover; 1998.
  83. 83. Lukas KE, Barkauskas RT, Maher SA, Jacobs BA, Bauman JE, Henderson AJ, Calcagno JM. Longitudinal study of delayed reproductive success in a pair of white-cheeked gibbon (Hylobates leucogenys). Zoo Biology. 2002;21:413-434.
  84. 84. Poyas AL. Pair bonding and parental care in socially monogamous primates: A comparative study of the white-cheeked gibbon (Nomascus leucogenys) and the white-faced saki (Pithecia pithecia) [Master’s thesis]. San Antonio: Department of Anthropology, University of Texas; 2008.
  85. 85. Poyas AL, Bartlett TQ. Pair bonding in socially monogamous primates: A comparative study of the white-cheeked gibbon (Nomascus leucogenys) and the white-faced saki (Pithecia pithecia). American Journal of Physical Anthropology Supplement. 2009;48:214 (Abstract only).
  86. 86. Embury AS. A study of the behaviour of captive gibbons [B.Sc. (Honour’s) thesis]. Parkville: Department of Zoology, University of Melbourne; 1983.
  87. 87. Skyner L. The effect of visitors on the self-mutilation behaviour of a male pileated gibbon (Hylobates pileatus) at Blackpool Zoological Gardens [BSc (Honour’s) Animal Behaviour and Welfare thesis]. Myerscough: Myerscough College, in association with The University of Central Lancashire, UK; 2002.
  88. 88. Riess BF. The behavior and social relations of the gibbon (Hylobates lar) observed under restricted free-range conditions. Zoologica. 1956;41:89-99.
  89. 89. Steen, JC. Behavior of captive gibbons (Hylobates sp.): Study of a group of six animals in the Portland (Oreg.) Zoo [MSc (Master of Arts) thesis]. Portland: Department of Anthropology, University of Oregon; 1969.
  90. 90. Ebert J. Paarungsverhalten weiblicher Weisshandgibbons (Hylobates lar) [Diploma thesis]. Berlin: Fachbereich Biologie, Chemie, Pharmazie, Freie Universität Berlin; 1999.
  91. 91. Eichmüller P. Verhaltensbiologische Beobachtung der Silbergibbons (Hylobates moloch) des Tierparks Hellabrunn, München [Praktikumsarbeit zur Vorlesung Tierökologie und Verhalten, Sommersemester 2006, betreut durch Prof. Dr. Gerstmeier]. Munich: Lehrstuhl für Zoologie, Technische Universität München; 2006.
  92. 92. Heufelder S. Eltern-Kind Beziehung und juveniles Spielverhalten von Hylobates moloch im Tierpark Hellabrunn, München [Praktikumsarbeit zur Vorlesung Verhaltensbiologie, betreut durch Prof. Dr. R. Gerstmeier]. Munich: Lehrstuhl für Zoologie, Technische Universität München; 2007.
  93. 93. Schneider H. Verhaltensbiologische Beobachtung der Silbergibbons (Hylobates moloch) des Tierparks Hellabrunn, München [Praktikumsarbeit zur Vorlesung Verhaltensbiologie, betreut durch Prof. Dr. R. Gerstmeier]. Munich: Lehrstuhl für Zoologie, Technische Universität München; 2009.
  94. 94. Sankaran S. Social behaviour and duetting in hoolock gibbons in Gibbon Wildlife Sanctuary, Assam [Master’s thesis]. Rajkot: Wildlife Science, Saurashtra University; 2009.
  95. 95. Dunn PO, Whittingham LA, Pitcher TE. Mating systems, sperm competition, and the evolution of sexual dimorphism in birds. Evolution. 2001;55:161-175. DOI: 0.1111/j.0014-3820.2001.tb01281.x
  96. 96. Lukas D, Clutton-Brock TH. The evolution of social monogamy in mammals. Science. 2013;341:526-530. Doi: 10.1126/science.1238677
  97. 97. Brockelman WY, Srikosamatara S. Maintenance and evolution of social structure in gibbons. In: Preuschoft H, Chivers DJ, Brockelman WY, Creel N, editors. The Lesser Apes: Evolutionary and Behavioural Biology. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press; 1984. p. 298-323.
  98. 98. Brockelman WY, Reichard U, Treesucon U, Raemaekers JJ. Dispersal, pair formation and social structure in gibbons (Hylobates lar). Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology. 1998;42:329-339. DOI: 10.1007/s002650050445
  99. 99. van Schaik CP. Social evolution in primates: The role of ecological factors and male behaviour. Proceedings of the British Academy. 1996;88:9-31.
  100. 100. Cowlishaw G. Song function in gibbons. Behaviour. 1992;121:131-153. DOI: 10.1163/156853992X00471
  101. 101. Geissmann T, Bohlen-Eyring S, Heuck A. The male song of the Javan silvery gibbon (Hylobates moloch). Contributions to Zoology. 2005;74:1-25. DOI: 10.1163/18759866-0740102001
  102. 102. Roos C, Geissmann T. Molecular phylogeny of the major hylobatid divisions. Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution. 2001;19:486-494. DOI: 10.1006/mpev.2001.0939
  103. 103. Ellefson JO. A natural history of white-handed gibbons in the Malayan Peninsula. In: Rumbaugh DM, editor. Gibbon and Siamang, Vol. 3. Basel and New York: Karger; 1974. p. 1-136.
  104. 104. Yi Y. Intra-group and inter-group social interactions in a pair-living primate, the Javan gibbon (Hylobates moloch) [Ph.D. thesis]. Seoul: Ewha Womans University; 2020.
  105. 105. Ahsan MF. Socio-ecology of the hoolock gibbon (Hylobates hoolock) in two forests of Bangladesh. In: Chicago Zoological Society, editor. The Apes: Challenges for the 21st Century. Conference Proceedings; 10-13 May 2000; Brookfield Zoo. Brookfield, Illinois: Chicago Zoological Society; 2001. p. 286-299.
  106. 106. Whitten AJ. The Kloss gibbon in Siberut rain forest [Ph.D. thesis]. Cambridge: Sub-Department of Veterinary Anatomy, University of Cambridge; 1980.

Written By

Thomas Geissmann, Simone Rosenkranz-Weck, Judith J.G.M. Van Der Loo and Mathias Orgeldinger

Submitted: 26 November 2020 Reviewed: 27 November 2020 Published: 30 December 2020