Subject scores on the MMSE, dROCFT, mPFC gating generator activation (1-activated, 0-non-activated), clinical diagnosis, and cluster group.
Abstract
For years, it has been assumed that the cerebral accumulation of pathologic protein forms is the main trigger of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) pathology; however, recent studies revealed strong evidences that the alternations in synaptic activity precede and affect the homeostasis of amyloid-beta and tau, both of which aggregate during AD. Given that the neuropathological changes, characteristic for AD, start decades before the onset of the first symptoms, when alternations become irreversible, it is crucial to find a biomarker that can detect the preclinical signs of disease, presumably synaptic dysfunction of specific cerebral areas. Here is presented a novel, a high potential neuroimaging biomarker that can detect the postsynaptic dysfunction of specific neural substrate located in medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) during sensory gating processing of a simple auditory stimulus. The magnetoencephalography-based localization of mPFC gating activation has the potential not only to detect symptomatic AD but also to become a predictor of cognitive decline related to the pathophysiological processes of AD, both at the individual level. The strengths of proposed biomarker lie in the simplicity of using a binary value, i.e., activated or not activated a neural generator along with its potential to follow the evolution of the pathophysiological process of disease from preclinical phase. The novel biomarker does not require estimation of uniform cutoff levels and standardization processes, the main problems of so far proposed biomarkers. Ability to individually detect AD pathology during putative preclinical and clinical stages, absolute noninvasiveness, and large effect size give this biomarker a high translation capacity and clinical potential.
Keywords
- Alzheimer’s disease
- preclinical Alzheimer’s disease
- Alzheimer’s disease biomarker
- neuroimaging biomarker
- auditory sensory gating
- prefrontal cortex
- magnetoencephalography
1. Introduction
Alzheimer’s disease (AD), a long-lasting progressive neurodegeneration, characterized by synaptic dysfunction, an increase in extracellular amyloid plaques, intracellular tauopathy, extensive neuronal loss in several cerebral areas, and enhanced neuroinflammation. Highly disrupted cholinergic transmission is proving to be a featured biochemical sign of disease. Clinical manifestations include progressive loss of ability to encode new memories, impairment of both declarative and non-declarative memory, and finally the invincible decline of overall intellectual capacity. Today, worldwide spreading of Alzheimer’s type dementia is one of the major public health challenges confronting this generation.
Extensive AD research, especially in the last 40 years, brought a significant progress in the development of diagnostic approaches and understanding the etiology. However, despite accumulated knowledge, the cause of the disease has not been found, a postmortem histopathological evaluation is still required to confirm the clinical diagnosis, and finally, effective treatment that would at least slow progression of the disease has not yet been found. As a result, if Augusta Deter, the first patient who had been described with the hallmarks of the disease, was alive today, her prognosis would be the same as in 1906.
1.1. Amyloid hypothesis
Since the first documentation by Alois Alzheimer, spread of abnormal protein filaments (plaque) throughout the brain, the main histopathological sign of Alzheimer’s dementia, had a prominent role in a broad spectrum of proposed mechanisms related to disease pathogenesis. Amyloidogenic fragments were described structurally in extracellular plaques, and tau protein was documented as the main component of intracellular neurofibrillary tangles [1, 2].
According to amyloid hypothesis, the neurotoxic forms of amyloid-beta polypeptides, derived from amyloid precursor protein (APP), induce synaptic injuries followed by substantial intracellular damage in form of tauopathy and subsequently produce the pathological presentations of neurodegeneration leading to dementia [3]. There are reliable evidences, provided by the systematic work of Braak and Braak [4, 5], that the pathological progression of extracellular and intracellular deposits of pathological protein forms starts decades before the onset of clinical symptoms.
The genetic mutations identified on chromosome 21 are conferred to trigger AD through abnormal processing of APP, causing the elevated cerebral concentrations of amyloid-beta or increased production of its specific forms [6]. Also, three other different autosomal dominant mutations that might cause AD have been identified on chromosomes 14 and 1, leading to mutations of presenilin 1 and presenilin 2 proteins, respectively, along with late-onset apolipoprotein genotype 🄄 [7, 8, 9].
The discoveries of mutations in amyloid-beta-related proteins have had significant influence in promoting the amyloid theory. However, while these mutations account for the majority of early-onset AD (<65 years; familial AD), the risk genes linked with late-onset AD (> 65 years; 95% of all AD cases) are subtle, with no direct genetic relation to the APP gene or its enzymes [10]. In addition, recent neuroimaging results strongly indicate that some non-demented individuals can have amyloid-beta aggregates equivalent in concentration to those found in demented patients [11], and also symptoms of AD can emerge regardless of amyloid deposition [12].
The evidences, provided by the animal studies, that amyloid clearance produces decreased symptoms of disease in mouse models of AD [13, 14] had turned the drug development in the direction of targeting beta-amyloid in human brains. The deployment of drugs comprised active and passive immunization directly against amyloid-beta accumulation and inhibition of beta- and gamma-secretase APP cleaving enzyme. Regrettably, none of testing drugs have resulted in recovery of functional abilities or alleviating symptoms of the disease in humans. The treatments did not slow down the course of the disease; on the contrary, some drugs even accelerate the progression of the symptoms. The recently emerged evidences confirmed the low correlation of the cerebral distribution of beta-amyloid plaques with neuropathology, decrease of neural function, or cognitive deficits [3]. A possibility that insoluble plaque is not the primary cause of progressive AD pathology is a strong argument for failure of anti-amyloid vaccination trials to improve patient outcome, even when cerebral amyloid plaque was removed [15]. Consequently, failure of amyloid-based AD treatments shifts research hypothesis to the soluble amyloid beta oligomers rather than plaques to be the main cause of neuronal degeneration [16]. However, there is still not reliable evidence in human studies that soluble amyloid oligomers are toxic in vivo. Notably, the cause of pathological cascade, resulting in redundant amyloid forms production and ejection, is still poorly understood along with the issue concerning the role of different amyloid beta forms in AD pathogenesis [17].
1.2. Synaptic function failure hypothesis
Synapse loss generates a loss of dendritic mass [18] and promotes neuron loss [19], a hallmark of AD pathology. It is interesting that amyloid plaque can show up without synapse loss [20]; moreover, synapse loss can occur without associated amyloid deposition [21]. High quantitative correlations of postmortem cytopathology to premortem cognitive impairments suggest that the decrease in density of synapses and decrease in the number of synapses per neuron are more linked to AD symptoms than are the concentration of amyloid plaques, the number of intracellular tau-tangles, or cortical gliosis [22]. These results appoint synapses as the key feature of AD, strongly indicating that the disease pathogenesis could be the outcome of synaptic failure [23].
It has been shown that the early AD symptoms significantly correlate with a specific dysfunction of cholinergic synapses [24]. Interestingly, acetylcholine receptor agonists affect several of AD hallmarks, including cholinergic deficits, cognitive dysfunction, but also tau and amyloid-beta pathological burdens [25]. The electrophysiological measurements of basal synaptic transmission and long-term potentiation in transgenic mouse with mutations analog of human mutations causing AD suggest that change in synaptic function precedes amyloid plaque production. Increased synaptic activity increases amyloid secretion, while decreasing activity inhibits it [26, 27]. In turn, beta-amyloid burden inhibits synapses and alerts synaptic plasticity [28], implying the existence of a feedback loop between synaptic dynamics and associated amyloid production that might serve as a mechanism to prevent synaptic hyperactivation and excitotoxicity [26]. In addition, recent studies demonstrate an important physiological role of cortical amyloid secretion, showing that low concentrations of beta-amyloid peptides increase long-term potentiation, needed for successful memory formation [29, 30]. There are also evidences that synaptic dysfunction occurs before changes in synaptic morphology or the number of synapses per neuron [31, 32]. These breakthroughs in the field provide strong evidences that amyloid homeostasis is controlled by the synaptic functions and emphasize prominent amyloid involvement in healthy memory coding.
Cerebral dysfunction found in non-demented elderly individuals with amyloid plaques before any memory disturbances [33] points at impairment in neural function as a very early pathophysiological sign of AD. Interestingly, functional changes may be driven in both directions, increased [34, 35] or decreased [36, 37] neuronal excitability, usually depending on the disease stage and a specific brain area and its function. The increased synaptic activity, found in early stages of symptomatic AD, might be an adaptive response driving neuroprotection [38]. On the other hand, the decreased neural network excitability could be induced by activation of gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) receptors, which decrease glutamate excitatory transmission, implying that in vivo glutamate-mediated neuronal excitability is controlled by interactions between inhibitory systems [39]. The co-transmission of acetylcholine and GABA, first found within the cholinergic system only, recently was demonstrated as a common feature of nearly all cholinergic forebrain neurons [40, 41]. These results appoint GABA as a fast neurotransmitter utilized throughout the forebrain cholinergic system and emphasized acetylcholine-GABA co-release as a major modulation factor of cortical functions transmitted by cholinergic neurons. Moreover, there is evidence that GABA receptor agonists defend neurons in culture from the toxicity of beta amyloids and of different glutamate receptor agonists [42]. In conclusion, given their major role in both sensory processing and cognition, and high susceptibility to AD pathology, the ability of the cholinergic neurons to co-release GABA could explain the failure of specific synaptic inhibitory processes found in AD that may trigger the cascade of events resulting in characteristic neuropathology.
1.3. Dynamic model of AD pathogenesis
The presence of cerebral amyloid aggregation in cognitively normal individuals, the lack of systematic correlation between amyloid plaque deposition and cognition, insufficiently explained influence of soluble amyloid oligomeric
Failures of clinical trials of anti-amyloid drugs in symptomatic AD patients (mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and moderate Alzheimer’s disease patients) in addition to reliable evidence that the hallmarks of disease could be found in the brain decades before symptom onset set up a view that the onset of clinical symptoms is a sign of irreversible neural damage. Consequently, the concept of AD pathogenesis is evolving toward a view of the disease as a long-term continuum, which differs only by symptom appearance; that is, a non-symptomatic (preclinical) AD phase and an irreversible symptomatic AD phase [43, 44]. This dynamic concept of the disease emphasizes the neurobiological advantage of early intervention before of widespread neurodegeneration, during the preclinical stage of disease, when neural injuries may still be reversible. Consequently, it is crucial to detect very early, possibly reversible, pathological changes related to AD in cognitively intact individuals, before the occurrence of the first symptoms, i.e., to reveal the reliable preclinical biomarker of the disease.
1.4. Conclusion
A wealth of evidence suggests that the amyloid hypothesis of AD etiology is insufficient to explain all pathological changes associated with AD and their temporal evolution. Considering the wide spectra of data in AD research, it cannot be overlooked their common link: synaptic dysfunction and degradation as a very early characteristic of the disease. Unlike neuron death, changes in synaptic functions are very likely still reversible. These subtle neurofunctional alternations are likely detectable by functional neuroimaging techniques. The neuroimaging has the capability to provide an assessment of the altered neurophysiology, before anatomical abnormalities and divergent neuropathology of the later disease stages. Focusing the synaptic level, at the phase when the neuron cell potentially is still healthy, provides an excellent opportunity to intervene at a reversible stage of the disease when neural networks are vulnerable, but not lost. With regard to focal neural activation during the early phases of processing external inputs, studying the early sensory responses enable unique insight into the synchronized synaptic activity of functionally related neural substrates, which are on the larger scale recognized as sensory networks. The first manifestations of declining synaptic function could involve desynchronization of synaptic transmission, which may cause the “virtual” absence of activation when it is measured extracranially. These alterations in topology of sensory networks could be even associated with specific patterns of attention, memory, or behavioral disorder that could indicate the preclinical stage of disease.
2. Biomarkers of AD
A biomarker in medicine is conceptualized as a measurable detector of a physiological, anatomic, or biochemical alternation that can distinct normal biologic processes from pathological. Biomarker should be able to provide reliable diagnosis, follow the development of disease, and measure responses to a therapeutic intervention. The development of a new biomarker for any clinical condition is based on the ability to accurately detect specific pathophysiology against the gold or reference standard. This creates problem specifically to AD because there is no in vivo gold standard. Currently, the final diagnosis of AD requires both the presence of amnestic symptoms and postmortem histopathological confirmation. Premortem diagnosis of AD is only “probable” or “possible” based on symptoms characteristic of Alzheimer’s type of dementia and neuroimaging findings.
The growing body of evidences evolves AD research field toward the concept of disease pathogenesis as a continuum of long-term phases in which clinical symptomatology and underlying pathophysiological process have different temporal development rates [45]. The existing findings indicate that the onset of the first symptoms marked the already irreversible stage of the disease [44]. Therefore, only detection of preclinical phase, before the occurrence of the first symptoms, but with present neuropathological changes characteristic of AD, would provide a major opportunity for therapeutic intervention in possible still reversible stage of disease. The main problem of AD research lies in the fact that is not yet found a solid link between the specific biomarker occurrence in presymptomatic individuals and the subsequent appearance of clinical symptoms [44, 45]. Although major advances in brain imaging, neurochemistry, and genetic research that highly accelerated the field, there still remains a need for the establishment of accurate biomarkers of preclinical AD, which will differentiate subjects without the risk of progression to dementia from those at risk for developing symptomatic AD. Today, five state-of-the-art diagnostic measures of AD are proposed for diagnostic criteria [46, 47]. Three of these are neuroimaging measures, and the other two are the laboratory indicators related to cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) proteins. Laboratory measures of AD include a reduced level of amyloid-beta 42 or an increase in p-tau concentration in the CSF. Imaging measures include tracers that allow detection of fibrillary amyloid-beta deposits in the cortex by positron emission tomography (PET) and detectors, also known as injury or topographical biomarkers, of a medial temporal lobe atrophy and reduced glucose metabolism in temporoparietal regions, as determined by magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and fluorodeoxyglucose (FDG) PET, respectively. However, proposed biomarkers have limited efficiency in detecting preclinical changes associated with the disease [45, 46, 47, 48, 49, 50] and are invasive for subjects because there are risks associated with lumbar puncture (CSF), exposure to radiation (PET/CT), or claustrophobic time-consuming scanning (MRI). Moreover, the classification results of proposed biomarkers are based on the difference between group means of measured variables that generally cannot provide a clear boundary between normal and pathological responses. The underlying reason for the limited discriminatory accuracy of proposed biomarkers lies in the high individual heterogeneity and variability of neural responses, cerebral anatomy, and metabolism [51], especially in the elderly population. Consequently, there is an urgent need for additional, noninvasive, more accurate tool that can be used to differentiate presymptomatic and symptomatic AD from normal aging.
2.1. Topological biomarker of AD
Besides high sensitivity and specificity, ideal AD biomarker should be individual, robust, and must be able to follow the evolution of the pathophysiological process of AD from early preclinical changes to symptomatic aggravation of the disease. A promising, but so far underutilized line of biomarker research is the alternation of basic neural network topography as a consequence of AD pathology [52]. An efficient way to start topographic search for AD biomarker is studying the most fundamental, still focal neural mechanisms that occur very early in the processing stream of simple sensory inputs. Dominantly cholinergic modulation of a sensory gating processing [53, 54] indicates that sensory gating network would likely be alerted in AD pathophysiology because the leading neurochemical feature of AD is a deterioration of the cholinergic signal transmission by selective impact on the plasticity of nicotinic (nAChr) and muscarinic (mAChr) synaptic receptors [55]. On a large scale, this synaptic dysfunction is likely to cause subtle alterations in sensory gating processing years before meeting criteria for symptomatic AD [56, 57].
2.2. Sensory gating
Sensory gating is a fundamental process of sensory processing, arising within the first 50 ms of exposure to a stimulus, much earlier than conscious perception. It is a phenomenon by which the neural system rapidly adjusts its response to subsequent stimuli, a neural feature with an essential impact in everyday life [58, 60]. This fast inhibition or enhancement of the neural response provoked by the external stimuli refers to different gating mechanisms: by gating-out neural system selectively suppresses its responses to irrelevant or redundant stimuli and by gating-in reinforcing responses on task-relevant or novel stimuli [59, 60]. Gating-out has been proposed as a mechanism of habituating to redundant stimuli that protect working memory overload by preventing irrelevant information from recurrent sensory processing, while at the same time, gating-in processing enables recognition of relevant environment inputs that are essential for survival [59, 60, 61].
2.2.1. Clinical correlates of sensory gating
Sensory gating deficits have been associated with several clinical conditions. Patients with schizophrenia [62, 63, 64], Alzheimer’s disease [60, 64, 65], bipolar disorder [66], post-traumatic stress disorder [67], Parkinson’s disease [68], or Huntington’s disease [69] show alerted sensory gating dynamics compared to controls. Abnormality in extracranially measured auditory gating responses is recognized as one of the best established marks for schizophrenia [70, 71]. In addition, alerted sensory gating has been associated with impaired performance on tasks measuring sustained attention [72], inhibition of distractors [73], or performance on neurocognitive tasks [64].
2.2.2. Neurochemistry of sensory gating
Multiple neurotransmitters are found to be involved in sensory gating processing, including the cholinergic, dopaminergic, GABAergic, glutamatergic, noradrenergic, and serotonergic systems [74]. Pharmacological studies have emphasized the particular importance of the cholinergic system in regulating the decreased response to repeated stimuli (gating-out) through stimulation of the α-7 nicotinic receptor [75] and the muscarinic M1 receptor [76]. More recently, a marker of the gene for the α-7 nicotinic receptor has been strongly linked to sensory gating abnormalities [75]. Notably, α7-containing receptors are also known for fast desensitization in the presence of agonist and high Ca2+ permeability [77].
2.2.3. Assessment of sensory gating
Sensory gating is typically assessed using a paired stimulus or oddball paradigm during an electrophysiological recording. In a paired-click paradigm, the event-related potential (ERP) component P50 or its magnetic counterpart M50, elicited about 50 ms after stimulus presentation, is measured during the presentation of two identical stimuli (S1 and S2) with an interstimulus interval (ISI) of 500 ms [72, 78]. The habituation of the response to the redundant second stimulus, expressed as the ratio between responses amplitudes (S2/S1), indicates the strength of gating-out inhibition [79]. Auditory oddball paradigm is characterized by the varying occurrence probability of a deviant (novel) stimulus between a series of repeated standard stimuli and therefore evokes both gating mechanisms, habituation of redundant information (standard stimuli) and pre-attentive memory-based comparison processes (deviant stimuli) [60, 64].
2.2.4. Neuroimaging of sensory gating
Notably, there is a problem in noninvasively studying the gating function in vivo. Only functional neuroimaging technique with millisecond temporal resolution can capture the sensory gating dynamics, which occurs within the first 100 ms after stimulus input. Positron emission tomography (PET), functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), single-photon emission computed tomography (SPECT), and near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) are neuroimaging techniques that provide indirect look at the brain at work. These techniques provide measures of metabolic and vascular signals that are associated with neuronal activity, which are assumed to be linearly correlated, although reliable functional relations are not yet established. The temporal resolution of these functional techniques is rather low, on a minute scale, compared to underlying neural activity, which extends from 1 to 3 ms of action potential firing to a few tens of milliseconds of synchronous synaptic activity of thousands of postsynaptic neurons.
Unlike other functional neuroimaging techniques, electroencephalography (EEG) and magnetoencephalography (MEG) provide a critical opportunity to directly and noninvasively study the brain activity [80, 81]. Neurons in the human cortex generally process their information by means of electromagnetic signals and thus enable the direct recording of their activity. EEG and MEG are electrophysiological techniques that are both sensitive to the electrochemical currents within and between the brain cells. With sub-millisecond temporal resolution, these techniques can easily capture the dynamics of spontaneous and evoked neural responses. MEG and EEG provide a complimentary information about the underlying electromagnetic brain activity. MEG measures the magnetic field generated by the primary intracellular ionic currents through postsynaptic dendrites, while EEG measures the voltage scalp distribution generated by the secondary extracellular ionic flow, both produced by the normal brain activity [80, 81].
Though EEG can capture both radial and tangential components of a produced extracellular electrical field [80, 81], there was a widely accepted assumption that MEG detects only sources that are tangential to the scalp surface. However, recently, it was shown that source orientation is not a significant factor in limiting MEG sensitivity. Using both numerical simulation and empirical measurements, it has been demonstrated that source depth and spatial extent of activated assembly on convoluted cortical surface are the main factors that compromise the sensitivity of MEG to neural activity in the human cortex [82]. Although both techniques have high temporal resolution, MEG outperforms EEG in spatiotemporal localization of neural substrate underlying extracranially captured electromagnetic activity. Using modern signal analysis methods the centimeter spatial resolution of EEG approach that of conventional fMRI [83]. On the other hand, MEG technique has an excellent, millimeter spatial accuracy [82, 84]. Moreover, MEG can detect low-amplitude dipole current source in the deep tissue of dislocate cortex, which is simultaneously active with several times higher-magnitude dipole sources in the superficial regions of primary sensory areas [60], demonstrating a high spatial resolution of synchronously active sources within a neural network. Therefore, MEG can be used in two basic ways. The first, similar to the ordinary EEG recordings, utilizes studies of rhythmic brain activity, evoked neural processing, and clinical diagnostics to detect the presence or signs of abnormality in spontaneous or evoked brain activity. The second and more ambitious use of MEG is in the estimation of the location, strength, and time courses of neuronal current sources of spontaneous and evoked magnetic signals recorded outside the head. In conclusion, MEG is a unique method that enables the noninvasive spatiotemporal mapping of elementary sensory gating processing that rises within first 100 ms after stimulus presentation. Moreover, the sources of MEG signals, primarily cortical intracellular postsynaptic currents, make MEG the first choice technique for the noninvasive study of synaptic function in real time. Consequently, MEG has an advantage in the neuroimaging search for early neurodegenerative biomarkers associated with synaptic alterations.
2.2.5. Cortical network underlying sensory gating
In line with the hierarchical model of neural processing [85], sensory gating responses have been long conceptualized to originate in the primary sensory areas [86, 87], due to the very early appearance in a sensory process stream. However, a report of the gating response presence after bilateral damage of the primary auditory areas [88] suggested a contribution from at least one source related to arousal level or state [89]. Efforts to delineate the neuroanatomy of the sensory gating network long have yielded inconsistent findings [90, 91, 92, 93]. The recent works, relied on advanced MEG-based multi-dipole source localization, finally provide a reliable set of neural generators [60, 64, 94, 95, 96]. Moreover, our novel results revealed possible explanations concerning the problem of inconsistent reports regarding auditory gating network topology, suggesting that paired-click and oddball paradigms, which are often used to challenge sensory gating effects, evoke different gating generator topologies, even though they are both passive for the subjects [96].
2.2.6. Auditory sensory gating networks evoked by oddball and paired-click paradigm
Using the oddball paradigm, which evokes both gating-in and gating-out mechanisms, we have demonstrated that auditory gating network comprises a medial prefrontal (mPFC) generator along with the anticipated generators in the bilateral primary auditory cortices [60, 64, 96], as shown in panel A) of Figure 1. Our finding suggests the existence of a novel, very fast sensory processing stream (i.e., sensory gating loop), which links executive PFC to primary auditory cortex within the first 50 ms after stimulus presentation, alongside well-affirmed but slower limbic (dorsal) and somatic (ventral) sensory processing pathways [60, 96]. The existence of additional fast sensory processing stream, sensory gating loop, is anatomically supported by dense bidirectional connections between medial PFC and superior temporal cortices found in both primate and human anatomical studies [97, 98]. The mPFC possesses extensive cortico-cortical connections, including extensive local projections to and from other prefrontal regions, as well as with motor, limbic, and sensory cortices [99]. These structural properties of the connecting pathways provide the ability for localized primary auditory generators and PFC regions to work together as a large-scale neural network via fast sensory gating loop.

Figure 1.
Auditory sensory gating network evoked by the oddball paradigm (panel A) and paired-click paradigm (panel B). The best-fitting source locations are superimposed on volumetric MRI head data to achieve a spatial (3D) rendering of the auditory gating topology. In addition to anticipated bilateral generators in primary auditory areas (blue and green dots), both paradigms evoke a prefrontal gating generator (red dot). While oddball tones provoke medial PFC activation, the tones of a paired-click paradigm activate dorsolateral PFC areas.
Our novel research provides evidence that passive paired-click and oddball paradigms activate a different prefrontal generator within the auditory gating network [60, 96]. Spatiotemporal source localization of auditory gating responses evoked by the passive paired-click paradigm revealed a different gating topology consisting of a dorsolateral PFC (dlPFC) source in addition to the sources in the bilateral primary auditory cortices [78, 96], as shown in panel B) of Figure 1. This result implies the existence of early rerouting within prefrontal cortex by shifting prefrontal gating activation from dorsolateral to medial prefrontal region, depending on a paradigm [60, 96], suggesting that mPFC and dlPFC regions serve different functions involved during the early sensory processing. A passive paired-click paradigm characterized by the constant repetition of both short- and long-term patterns (S1–500 ms; S2–8 s) could result in long-term repetition suppression produced by the dlPFC. On the contrary, an oddball paradigm, characterized by the varying occurrence probability of a novel stimulus between a series of repeated stimuli, could put the neural network into a state of perceptual expectation (gating-in) while simultaneously suppressing redundant stimuli (gating-out). This phenomenon could be interpreted as the bottom-up, stimulus-driven initiation of attention during the very early sensory processing [100] executed by the mPFC region.
2.2.7. Functional mechanisms underlying auditory sensory gating
In addition to the spatial localization of auditory gating networks, we have disclosed the functional relation within network generators providing strong evidence of a modulatory role for the mPFC generator on bilateral superior temporal gyri (STG) sources dynamics during gating processing (Figure 2). This result discloses the long-sought mechanism underlying the auditory gating effect [60]. We demonstrated the complex form of estimated cortical morphology of the neural responses produced by the gating generators. In particular, gating response of STG generators is found to be composed of two consecutive cortical subcomponent, Mb1 peaking at 35–53 ms and Mb2 peaking at 75–95 ms post-stimulus, for both oddball paradigm tones [60]. We also provide the first estimates of cortical gating response produced by the mPFC generator, which has an analogous tandem form [60], as shown in Figure 2.

Figure 2.
Estimated cortical dynamics of auditory gating network generators evoked by the oddball paradigm. Cortical dynamics of medial prefrontal (PF) and STG gating generators for both tones (standard and deviant) are shown for eight representative subjects (four of each network type). Gating network type 1 comprised bilateral STG and the PF source, while network type 2 was characterized by the absence of PF activation. The time courses of the STG activity were individually averaged across hemispheres and displayed for 30–200 ms time window to include M100, the most prominent STG response, for the amplitude comparison with earlier Mb1 and Mb2 components. Both components of STG gating response (Mb1 and Mb2) are significantly increased when PF source activation is absent.
Taking advantage of the differences in the evoked gating networks between individuals whose PFC generator was activated (Type 1 in Figure 2) and cognitively impaired individuals whose PFC gating generator was not activated (Type 2 in Figure 2) by the tones of an oddball paradigm, we have carried out a differential analysis of the functional roles for each generator. Our results show that cortical gating dynamics of STG generators evoked by both the standard and deviant tones demonstrated highly increased strength of both gating components in all subjects lacking PFC generator activity [60]. This result is of extreme clinical importance because the prevalent view of the gating-related pathologies assumes that the larger amplitude of the extracranial gating response to the standard tone (i.e., redundant stimuli) reflects impaired gating out processing of neural substrate in primary auditory areas only (i.e., STG generators) [61, 62, 63, 66, 67, 68, 69]. Our novel results provide strong evidence of sustained inhibitory activity of the PFC generator that suppresses or modulates the activity of the STG generators as an underlying mechanism of both gating phenomena. Consequently, impaired activity of a PFC gating generator could be a primary cause of impaired extracranially measured sensory gating responses, found in numerous neurological and psychological clinical conditions.
3. Link between impaired sensory gating and AD pathology
The initial symptoms of AD include subtle decline of the ability to learn new information along with diverse amnestic disorders without present brain injuries. Clinically indicated AD signs are pointing to the existence of functional impairment of synapses that are involved in converting and forming new declarative memory [32]. Whereas longer retention of sensory memory traces derive more successful memory encoding [101], sensory gating process, conceptualized as the ability of the neural system to modulate its responses to subsequent stimuli, has a fundamental role in guiding successful encoding of new information. Impaired auditory gating processing may reduce pre-attentive signal-to-noise ratio and desynchronize synaptic consolidation in the initial phases of memory formation [58]. Augmentation of dysfunctional sensory gating process could yield to the first amnestic symptoms seen in AD neuropathology.
A range of EEG/MEG studies, measuring evoked sensory responses, have reported differences in early processing of auditory [102, 103, 104] stimuli in symptomatic AD patients, affirming the possibility of impaired inhibition of redundant information (gating-out) and processing of novel information (gating-in) in the initial phase of disease. Also, alerted connectivity among different brain regions and decline of long-distance synchronization are found to be responsible for some of the earliest cognitive changes in early phases of symptomatic AD [105]. Although the study of the extracranial neurophysiological (EEG/MEG) signals provides valuable information regarding the pathology-related changes in the amplitudes, latencies, frequency bands, spectral densities, and coherence of oscillatory brain dynamics, identified relations have received limited attention in the search for a biomarker of AD. The main reason is that classification based on the difference between group means of sensor-level measures generally cannot provide a clear individual boundary value between healthy and pathological responses and thus result in lower clinical significance. However, MEG spatiotemporal localization of cortical sources underlying extracranial magnetic field shows internal consistency and provides highly reliable and stable results of both cortical dynamics and topology of the activated network [60, 82, 84], enabling a search for an AD biomarker at the individual level.
3.1. Localization of auditory gating network generators: a topological biomarker of AD
Using MEG spatiotemporal source calculations, we have demonstrated [64] the potential of topological localization of mPFC generator within an auditory gating network as a discrete, binary, noninvasive tool for detection of AD at the individual subject level. We found three types of gating network topologies evoked by a simple auditory oddball paradigm across the research sample of elderly individuals, ranging from 63 to 87 years of age, which comprised patients with clinical diagnosis of symptomatic AD (MCI and moderate AD) and non-symptomatic elderly controls. Discrete localization/non-localization of mPFC gating generator absolutely discriminate symptomatic AD from controls confirm the indiscernibility between amnestic MCI (aMCI) and AD patients and differentiate two distinct gating network types within the elderly controls, one of which is suggested as preclinical AD. The lack of mPFC generator localization within an auditory gating network as a biomarker of symptomatic AD shows a large effect size (>0.9) and high accuracy, sensitivity, and specificity (100%) in respect of clinical diagnosis [64].
3.2. Sustained activation of mPFC gating generator: a sign of healthy cognitive aging
We have applied spectra of the multivariate analyses to disclose potentially hidden structure in complete set of our data, i.e., scores of wide neuropsychological screening and neurophysiological gating network topology results [64]. Clustering based on principal variables indicated the existence of three stable clusters across subjects as shown in Figure 3. Subsequent statistical review of group differences on neuropsychological tests confirmed the low-magnitude but significant differences in Mini Mental State Exam (MMSE) and delayed Rey-Osterreith Complex Figure Test (dROCFT) scores across subjects in different cluster groups [64]. The first group of controls, characterized by sustained gating activation of mPFC generator for both oddball tones, had the highest MMSE scores and the highest performance on the dROCFT, thus considered to be cognitively healthy elderly group.

Figure 3.
Dendrogram based on the neuropsychological and neurophysiological variables. Subjects were assigned numbers from 1 to 20 and grouped into clusters. Variables were normalized before using a clustering algorithm with Ward’s minimum variance method as amalgamation rule and with Euclidean distance as a measure of dissimilarity. Cluster grouping disclosed three groups across two clinical categories (H-healthy subjects, MCI/AD—symptomatic AD subjects) suggesting the potential of the proposed approach to detect subgroup of control (“healthy”) subjects who are in the possible preclinical phase of AD (cluster category 2).
3.3. Partial activation of mPFC gating generator: a biomarker of preclinical AD
The second cluster group of controls was characterized by the first signs of neurophysiological gating alternation, which emerged as a partial activation of the mPFC gating generator to the deviant tone only, as shown in Table 1. The standard tone did not evoke mPFC activation, suggesting that a very early stage of impairment in sensory gating processing is manifested by an absence of mPFC gating transmission that corrupts habituation to redundant stimuli [64]. We confirmed the presence of insidious cognitive decline in this subgroup of controls, demonstrating the low-amplitude but significantly lower both MMSE and dROCFT scores in comparison with the high-functioning subgroup of controls [64]. Overall, both neuropsychological and neurophysiological impairments characteristic of an AD type of dementia found in low-functioning control group, although they did not yet meet clinical criteria for aMCI, indicate a possible preclinical AD phase. The additional weight to our speculation gave Takayama, whose 10-year longitudinal study also discerns the dROCF test as highly assertive indicator of conversion to symptomatic phase of Alzheimer’s disease, i.e., found significant sensitivity of dROCFT scores to early, possible preclinical AD pathology [106].
Subject | MMSE | dROCFT | mPFC standard | mPFC deviant | Clinical diagnosis | Cluster category |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
S1 | 30 | 26 | 1 | 1 | H | 1 |
S2 | 30 | 25 | 1 | 1 | H | 1 |
S3 | 30 | 23 | 1 | 1 | H | 1 |
S4 | 30 | 23 | 1 | 1 | H | 1 |
S5 | 30 | 22 | 0 | 1 | H | 2 |
S6 | 30 | 22 | 0 | 1 | H | 2 |
S7 | 30 | 17 | 0 | 1 | H | 2 |
S8 | 29 | 26 | 0 | 1 | MCI | 2 |
S9 | 29 | 25 | 0 | 1 | H | 2 |
S10 | 29 | 18 | 0 | 1 | H | 2 |
S11 | 26 | 20 | 0 | 1 | H | 2 |
S12 | 26 | 12 | 0 | 0 | MCI | 3 |
S13 | 26 | 7 | 0 | 0 | MCI | 3 |
S14 | 26 | 1.5 | 0 | 0 | MCI | 3 |
S15 | 25 | 13 | 0 | 0 | MCI | 3 |
S16 | 25 | 0 | 0 | 0 | AD | 3 |
S17 | 25 | 0 | 0 | 0 | AD | 3 |
S18 | 24 | 10.5 | 0 | 0 | AD | 3 |
S19 | 23 | 0 | 0 | 0 | AD | 3 |
S20 | 22 | 2 | 0 | 0 | AD | 3 |
Table 1.
The possibility that partial gating activation of the mPFC generator in lower-functioning controls may be associated with preclinical AD phase is also confirmed by several recent findings. There are evidences that reduced functional connectivity affecting the PFC is associated with amyloid-β-related hypersynchronization [107] and p-tau pathology [108] in a very early phase of AD-type memory impairment. The impaired mPFC activity during endogenous brain activity or memory tasks is found in cognitively normal individuals who were AD APOE ε 4 carriers [109]. Also, evidence of decreased extracranial gating dynamics as a predictor of cerebrospinal amyloid-β reduction is demonstrated in MCI patients [65]. Alerted synaptic function along with subsequent synaptic loss and transneuronal spread of pathological tau forms [108] through PFC regions could result in the topological gating deficit that we found in a low-functioning subgroup of controls. This topological gating deficit could reflect a possible preclinical phase of AD pathology before widespread of cognitive symptoms.
3.4. Complete absence of mPFC gating generator activation: a biomarker of symptomatic AD
Subjects within the cluster group characterized by the complete absence of mPFC gating generator activation regardless of the tone condition (i.e., standard and deviant tone) had the lowest neuropsychological test results and belonged to the symptomatic AD patients with clinical diagnosis [64], as shown in Figure 3. Clearly, a topological deficit of mPFC gating generator activation is augmented in symptomatic AD phase, taking place during both gating out (inhibition of standard tone) and gating in (enhanced processing of novel tone), suggesting complete disruption of sensory gating process. This absolute break of a fundamental sensory process is associated with the onset of the amnestic AD symptoms. It is possible that progressive failure in sensory gating-out, found in possible preclinical phase of disease, is likely to lead to an overload of working memory due to noise accretion and consequent to the first symptoms of memory impairment.
There are numerous evidences of association between symptomatic AD pathology and alerted physiology of PFC region. Decreased frontal-parietal correlations of glucose metabolism [110], prefrontal glucose hypometabolism on 18F-fluorodeoxyglucose PET scans [111], and frontal retention of 11C-Pittsburgh compound [112] are found in AD patients. Klupp and colleagues [113] found a significant hypometabolism in PFC regions that are not loaded with amyloid plaques, suggesting that the deficit in mPFC gating activation may be related to longitudinal amyloid deposition in different but functionally connected brain regions. For instance, absence or desynchronization of mPFC gating activity could induce atrophy across fast auditory sensory gating loop, which may result in increased amyloid vulnerability of synapses terminated within primary auditory areas involved in the gating process. Inactivity of the mPFC generator during processing of repetitive stimuli (gating-out) likely impairs the ability to distinguish novel from repetitive information, which is critical for long-term memory encoding. It has been demonstrated that activation of presynaptic α-7-nAChRs, involved in gating transmission, induces long-term potentiation of the excitatory input [114]. Therefore, a topological deficit of mPFC generator activation during gating transmission could be a consequence of impaired nAChR levels found in PFC regions of AD patients. Alerted function of α-7-nAChRs could induce lower levels of intracellular calcium, consequently impairing the calcium cascade in producing synchronized postsynaptic signals required for effective both gating processing and long-term memory encoding.
4. Conclusion
Identification of a novel biomarker of AD (Figure 4) with the potential to detect both putative preclinical and clinical stages at the individual subject level represents significant progress toward improving diagnosis of AD and accelerating the field toward the neurobiological advantage of earlier intervention. Although the study engaged only a research sample, the very large effect size (>0.98) of proposed test, thanks to its binary nature, provides high relevance to the finding. Such a large effect size enables this study with the research sample size to yield power greater than 85%.

Figure 4.
Proposed topological approach as biomarker of AD pathology. Three distinct types of mPFC auditory gating generator activation (red dot) were identified in healthy controls, possible preclinical AD and symptomatic AD patients. The healthy gating topology type requires mPFC activation in processing both oddball tones (standard and deviant). An altered gating topology, characterized by selective mPFC activation only by the deviant tone, presumably represents a presymptomatic phase of AD. Symptomatic AD gating topology lacks mPFC activation for both standard and deviant tones.
Novel topological biomarker, besides high accuracy, sensitivity, and specificity (100%) in identifying symptomatic AD patients in the research sample, shows the potential of following the evolution of the pathophysiological process of disease. The noninvasiveness and low sensitivity to individual heterogeneity and variability due to the discrete nature of impaired prefrontal gating activation are the most important properties of the novel biomarker. It is not based on the use of group means and is not associated with statistically significant changes in a continuous variable. The advantage of this biomarker lies in the simplicity of using a binary value, i.e., activated or not activated a prefrontal generator during gating processing of simple tones. The proposed biomarker is absolutely noninvasive; it is based on recordings of neuromagnetic fields that are produced by normal brain activity.
The new topographic tool certainly has properties, which place it within a group of high-potential biomarkers. The absolute noninvasiveness, individual detection of pathology, ability to detect the preclinical phase of the disease, discrete nature that does not require estimation of uniform cutoff levels and standardization processes, the low sensitivity to individual heterogeneity and variability, capability to follow the evolution of the pathophysiological process of AD, and finally high accuracy and sensitivity make it highly promising biomarker of AD.
However, despite mentioning highlights, the proposed biomarker requires to be tested in a large independent sample and requires assessment in longitudinal clinical MEG studies that would track nonsymptomatic elderly with partial activation of the prefrontal gating generator until the first clinical symptoms appear and finally to autopsy for confirmation of AD. It would also be necessary to investigate prefrontal gating dynamics in other dementias to determine the specificity of novel biomarker to discriminate AD from other etiologies of age-related cognitive decline.
References
- 1.
Roher A, Wolfe D, Palutke M, KuKuruga D. Purification, ultrastructure, and chemical analysis of Alzheimer disease amyloid plaque core protein. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 1986; 83 :2662-2666 - 2.
Grundke-Iqbal I, Iqbal K, Quinlan M, Tung YC, Zaidi MS, Wisniewski HM. Microtubule-associated protein tau. A component of Alzheimer paired helical filaments. The Journal of Biological Chemistry. 1986; 261 :6084-6089 - 3.
Morris GP, Clark IA, Vissel B. Inconsistencies and controversies surrounding the amyloid hypothesis of Alzheimer's disease. Acta Neuropathologica Communications. 2014; 2 :135 - 4.
Braak H, Braak E. Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathologica. 1991; 82 :239-259 - 5.
Braak H, Braak E. Staging of Alzheimer’s disease-related neurofibrillary changes. Neurobiology of Aging. 1995; 16 :271-278 - 6.
Bird TD. Genetic factors in Alzheimer’s disease. The New England Journal of Medicine. 2005; 352 :862-864 - 7.
Corder EH, Saunders AM, Strittmatter WJ, Schmechel DE, Gaskell PC, Small GW, Roses AD, Haines JL, Pericak-Vance MA. Gene dose of apolipoprotein E type 4 allele and the risk of Alzheimer’s disease in late onset families. Science. 1993; 261 :921-923 - 8.
Sherrington R, Rogaev EI, Liang Y, Rogaeva EA, Levesque G, et al. Cloning of a gene bearing missense mutations in early-onset familial Alzheimer’s disease. Nature. 1995; 375 :754-760 - 9.
Rogaev EI, Sherrington R, Rogaeva EA, Levesque G, Ikeda M, et al. Familial Alzheimer’s disease in kindreds with missense mutations in a gene on chromosome 1 related to the Alzheimer’s disease type 3 gene. Nature. 1995; 376 :775-778 - 10.
Thies W, Bleiler L. Alzheimer's disease facts and figures. Alzheimer's and Dementia. 2011; 7 :208-244 - 11.
Rentz DM, Locascio JJ, Becker JA, Moran EK, Eng E, Buckner RL, Sperling RA, Johnson KA.Cognition, reserve, and amyloid deposition in normal aging. Annals of Neurology. 2010; 67 :353-364 - 12.
Tsai J, Grutzendler J, Duff K, Gan WB. Fibrillar amyloid deposition leads to local synaptic abnormalities and breakage of neuronal branches. Nature Neuroscience. 2004; 7 :1181-1183 - 13.
Schenk D, Barbour R, Dunn W, Gordon G, Grajeda H, et al. Immunization with amyloid-[beta] attenuates Alzheimer-disease-like pathology in the PDAPP mouse. Nature. 1999; 400 :173-177 - 14.
Lobello K, Ryan JM, Liu E, Rippon G, Black R. Targeting beta amyloid: A clinical review of immunotherapeutic approaches in Alzheimer’s disease. International Journal of Alzheimer's Disease. 2012; 2012 :628070 - 15.
Mullane K, Williams M. Alzheimer's therapeutics: Continued clinical failures question the validity of the amyloid hypothesis-but what lies beyond? Biochemical Pharmacology. 2013; 85 :289-305 - 16.
Ferreira ST, Klein WL. The Aβ oligomer hypothesis for synapse failure and memory loss in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2011; 96 :529-543 - 17.
Benilova I, Karran E, De Strooper B. The toxic Abeta oligomer and Alzheimer's disease: An emperor in need of clothes. Nature Neuroscience. 2012; 15 :349-357 - 18.
Uylings HBM, de Brabander JM. Neuronal changes in normal human aging and Alzheimer's disease. Brain and Cognition. 2002; 49 :268-276 - 19.
Conforti L, Adalbert R, Coleman MP. Neuronal death: Where does the end begin? Trends in Neurosciences. 2007; 30 :159-166 - 20.
Boncristiano S, Calhoun ME, Howard V, Bondolfi L, Kaeser SA, Wiederhold KH, Staufenbiel M, Jucker M. Neocortical synaptic Bouton number is maintained despite robust amyloid deposition in APP23 transgenic mice. Neurobiology of Aging. 2005; 26 :607-613 - 21.
Spires TL, Meyer-Luehmann M, Stern EA, McLean PJ, Skoch J, Nguyen PT, Bacskai BJ, Hyman BT. Dendritic spine abnormalities in amyloid precursor protein transgenic mice demonstrated by gene transfer and intravital multiphoton microscopy. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2005; 25 :7278-7287 - 22.
Terry RD, Masliah E, Salmon DP, Butters N, DeTeresa R, Hill R, et al. Physical basis of cognitive alternations in Alzheimer’s disease: Synapse loss is the major correlate of cognitive impairment. Annals of Neurology. 1991; 30 :572-580 - 23.
Mesulam MM. A plasticity-based theory of the pathogenesis of Alzheimer's disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2000; 924 :42-24 - 24.
Ferreira-Vieira TH, Guimaraes IM, Silva FR, Ribeiro FM. Alzheimer’s disease: Targeting the cholinergic system. Current Neuropharmacology. 2016; 14 (1):101-115 - 25.
Cacamo A, Oddo S, Billings LM, Green KN, Martinez-Coria H, Fisher A, LaFerla FM. M1 receptors play a central role in modulating AD-like pathology in transgenic mice. Neuron. 2006; 49 (5):671-682 - 26.
Kamenetz F, Tomita T, Hsieh H, Seabrook G, Borchelt D, Iwatsubo T, et al. APP processing and synaptic function. Neuron. 2003; 2003, 37 :925-937 - 27.
Cirrito JR, Yamada KA, Finn MB, Sloviter RS, Bales KR, May PC, et al. Synaptic activity regulates interstitial fluid amyloid-beta levels in vivo. Neuron. 2005; 48 :913-922 - 28.
Shankar GM, Li S, Mehta TH, Garcia-Munoz A, Shepardson NE, Smith I, et al. Amyloid-beta protein dimers isolated directly from Alzheimer’s brains impair synaptic plasticity and memory. Nature Medicine. 2008; 14 :837-842 - 29.
Puzzo D, Privitera L, Fa M, Staniszewski A, Hashimoto G, Aziz F, et al. Endogenous amyloid-beta is necessary for hippocampal synaptic plasticity and memory. Annals of Neurology. 2011; 69 :819-830 - 30.
Garcia-Osta A, Alberini CM. Amyloid beta mediates memory formation. Learning & Memory. 2009; 16 :267-272 - 31.
Watase K, Weeber EJ, Antalffy B, Yuva-Payor L, Hashimoto K, Kano M, Atkinson R, Sun Y, Armstrong DL, Sweatt JD, Orr HT, Paylor R, Zoghby HY. A long CAG repeat in the mouse Sca1 locus replicates SCA1 features and reveals the impact of protein solubility on selective neurodegeneration. Neuron. 2002; 34 :905-919 - 32.
Selkoe DJ. Alzheimer's disease is a synaptic failure. Science. 2002; 298 :789-791 - 33.
Sperling RA, Laviolette PS, O’Keefe K, O’Brien J, Rentz DM, Pihlajamaki M. Amyloid deposition is associated with impaired default network function in older persons without dementia. Neuron. 2009; 63 :178-188 - 34.
Reiman EM. QuirozYT, Fleisher AS, Chen K, Velez-Pardo C, Jimenez Del-Rio M, et al. Brain imaging and fluid biomarker analysis in young adults at genetic risk for autosomal dominant Alzheimer’s disease in the presenilin1 E280A kindred: Acase-control study. Lancet Neurology. 2012; 11 :1048-1056 - 35.
Marchetti C, Marie H. Hippocampal synaptic plasticity in Alzheimer’s disease: What have we learned so far from transgenic models? Reviews in the Neurosciences. 2011; 22 :373-402 - 36.
Trinchese F, Liu S, Battaglia F, Walter S, Mathews PM, Arancio O. Progressive age-related development of Alzheimer-like pathology in APP/PS1 mice. Annals of Neurology. 2004; 55 :801-814 - 37.
Menkes-Caspi N, Yamin HG, Kellner V, Spires-Jones TL, Cohen D, Stern EA. Pathological tau disrupts ongoing network activity. Neuron. 2015; 85 :959-966 - 38.
Tampellini D. Synaptic activity and Alzheimer’s disease: A critical update. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2015; 9 :423 - 39.
Rodriguez Manzanares PA, Isoardi NA, Carrer HF, Molina VA. Previous stress facilitates fear memory, attenuates GABAergic inhibition, and increases synaptic plasticity in the rat basolateral amygdala. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2005; 25 (38):8725-8734 - 40.
Lee S, Kim K Zhou ZJ. Role of ACh-GABA cotransmission in detecting image motion and motion direction. Neuron. 2010; 68 :1159-1172 - 41.
Saunders A, Granger AJ, Sabatini BL. Corelease of acetylcholine and GABA from cholinergic forebrain neurons. eLife. 2015; 4 :e06412. DOI: 10.7554/eLife.06412 - 42.
Paula-Lima AC, De Felice FG, Brito-Moreira J, Ferreira ST. Activation of GABA(A) receptors by taurine and muscimol blocks the neurotoxicity of beta-amyloid in rat hippocampal and cortical neurons. Neuropharmacology. 2005; 49 :1140-1148 - 43.
Morris JC, Storandt M, Miller JP, McKeel DW, Price JL, Rubin EH, Berg L. Mild cognitive impairment represents early-stage Alzheimer disease. Archives of Neurology. 2001; 58 :397-405 - 44.
Sperling RA, Aisen PS, Beckett LA, Bennet DA, Craft D, et al. Toward definig the preclinical stages of Alzheimer's disease: Recommendetation from national institute on aging- Alzheimer's Asocciation workgroup on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimer's Dementia. 2011; 7 (3):280-292 - 45.
Lazarczyk MJ, Hof PR, Bouras C, Giannakopoul P. Preclinical Alzheimer disease: identification of cases at risk among cognitively intact older individuals. BMC Medicine. 2012; 10 :127. doi.org/10.1186/1741-7015-10-127 - 46.
Dubois B, Feldman HH, Jacova C, Dekosky ST, Barberger-Gateau P. Preclinical Alzheimer disease: Identification of cases at risk among cognitively intact older individuals. BMC Medicine. 2012; 10 :127. doi.org/10.1186/1741-7015-10-1276 - 47.
McKhann GM, Knopman DS, Chertkow H, Hyman BT, Jack CR Jr, et al. The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: Recommendations from the national institute on aging and the Alzheimer’s Association Workgroup. Alzheimer's & Dementia. 2011; 7 :263-269 - 48.
Morris JC, Blennow K, Froelich L, Nordberg A, Soininen H, Waldemar G, Wahlund LO, Dubois B. Harmonized diagnostic criteria for Alzheimer's disease: Recommendations. Journal of Internal Medicine. 2014; 275 (3):204-213 - 49.
Sperling RA, Jack CR Jr, Aisen PS. Testing the right target and right drug at the right stage. Science Translational Medicine. 2011; 3 (111):111-133 - 50.
Holtzman DM, Morris JC, Goate AM. Alzheimer's disease: The challenge of the second century. Science Translational Medicine. 2011; 3 (77):77sr1 - 51.
Poulson RS, Gadbury L, Allison DB. Treatment heterogeneity and individual qualitative interaction. The American Statistician. 2012; 66 (1):16-24 - 52.
Jack CR Jr, Knopman DS, Jagust WJ, Petersen RC, Weiner MW, Aisen PS, Shaw LM, Vemuri P, Wiste HJ, Weigand SD, et al. Tracking pathophysiological processes in Alzheimer's disease: An updated hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers. Lancet Neurology. 2013; 12 :207-216 - 53.
Adler LE, Hoffer LJ, Griffith J, Waldo MC, Freedman R. Normalization by nicotine of deficient auditory sensory gating in the relatives of schizophrenics. Biological Psychiatry. 1992; 32 :607-616 - 54.
Lucas-Meunier E, Fossier P, Baux G, Amar M. Cholinergic modulation of the cortical neuronal network. Pflügers Archiv. 2003; 446 :17-29 - 55.
Levin ED, McClernon FJ, Rezvani AH. Nicotinic effects on cognitive function: Behavioral characterization, pharmacological specification, and anatomic localization. Psychopharmacology. 2006; 184 :523-539 - 56.
Palop JJ, Mucke L. Amyloid-β–induced neuronal dysfunction in Alzheimer's disease: From synapses toward neural networks. Nature Neuroscience. 2010; 13 :812-818 - 57.
Knafo S, Sánchez-Puelles C, Palomer E, Delgado I, Draffin JE, et al. PTEN recruitment controls synaptic and cognitive function in Alzheimer's models. Nature Neuroscience. 2016; 19 (3):443-453 - 58.
Freedman R, Adler LE, Myles-Worsley M, Nagamoto HT, Miller C, et al. Inhibitory gating of an evoked response to repeated auditory stimuli in schizophrenic and normal subjects. Human recordings, computer simulation, and an animal model. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1996; 53 :1114-1121 - 59.
Gjini K, Arfken C, Boutros NN. Relationships between sensory “gating out” and sensory “gating in” of auditory evoked potentials in schizophrenia: A pilot study. Schizophrenia Research. 2010; 121 :139-145 - 60.
Josef Golubic S, Aine CJ, Stephen JM, Adair JC, Knoefel JE, Supek S. Modulatory role of the prefrontal generator within the auditory M50 network. NeuroImage. 2014; 92 :120-131 - 61.
Boutros NN, Torello MW, Barker BA, Tueting PA, Wu SC. The P50 evoked potential component and mismatch detection in normal volunteers: Implications for the study of sensory gating. Psychiatry Research. 1995; 57 :83-88 - 62.
Patterson JV, Hetrick WP, Boutros NN, Jin Y, Sandman C, Stern H, Potkin S, Bunney WE Jr. P50 sensory gating ratios in schizophrenics and controls: A review and data analysis. Psychiatry Research. 2008; 158 (2):226-247 - 63.
Yee CM, Williams TJ, White PM, Nuechterlein KH, Ames D, Subotnik KL. Attentional modulation of the P50 suppression deficit in recent-onset and chronic schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2010; 119 (1):31-39 - 64.
Josef Golubic S, Aine CJ, Stephen JM, Adair JC, Knoefel JE, Supek S. MEG biomarker of Alzheimer's disease: Absence of a prefrontal generator during auditory sensory gating. Human Brain Mapping. 2017; 38 (10):5180-5194 - 65.
Green DL, Payne L, Polikar R, Moberg PJ, Wolk D, Kounios J. P50: A candidate ERP biomarker of prodromal Alzheimer's disease. Brain Research. 2015; 624 :390-397 - 66.
Wang Y, Feng Y, Jia Y, Wang W, Xie Y, Guan Y, Zhong S, Zhu D, Huang L. Auditory M50 and M100 sensory gating deficits in bipolar disorder: A MEG study. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2014; 152-154 :131-138 - 67.
Neylan TC, Fletcher DJ, Lenoci M, McCallin K, Weiss DS, Schoenfeld FB, Fein G. Sensory gating in chronic posttraumatic stress disorder: Reduced auditory P50 suppression in combat veterans. Biological Psychiatry. 1999; 46 :1656-1664 - 68.
Teo C, Rasco L, al-Mefty K, Skinner RD, Boop FA, Garcia-Rill E. Decreased habituation of midlatency auditory evoked responses in Parkinson's disease. Movement Disorders. 1997; 12 (5):655-664 - 69.
Uc EY, Skinner RD, Rodnitzky RL, Garcia-Rill E. The midlatency auditory evoked potential P50 is abnormal in Huntington's disease. Journal of the Neurological Sciences. 2003; 212 :1-5 - 70.
Adler LE, Pachtman E, Franks RD, Pecevich MC, Waldo M, Freedman R. Neurophysiological evidence for a defect in neuronal mechanisms involved in sensory gating in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry. 1982; 17 :639-655 - 71.
Bramon E, Rabe-Hesketh S, Sham P, Murray RM, Frangou S. Meta-analysis of the P300 and P50 waveforms in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research. 2004; 70 :315-329 - 72.
Potter D, Summerfelt A, Gold J, Buchanan RW. Review of clinical correlates of P50 sensory gating abnormalities in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2006; 32 (4):692-700 - 73.
Erwin RJ, Turetsky BI, Moberg P, Gur RC, Gur RE. P50 abnormalities in schizophrenia: Relationship to clinical and neuropsychological indices of attention. Schizophrenia Research. 1998; 33 :157-167 - 74.
Adler LE, Olincy A, Waldo M, et al. Schizophrenia, sensory gating, and nicotinic receptors. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 1998; 24 :189-202 - 75.
Freedman R, Leonard S, Gault JM, et al. Linkage disequilibrium for schizophrenia at the chromosome 15q13-14 locus of the alpha7-nicotinic acetylcholine receptor subunit gene (CHRNA7). American Journal of Medical Genetics. 2001; 105 :20-22 - 76.
Klinkenberg IPM, Blokland AAJ, Riedel WJ, Sambeth A. Cholinergic modulation of auditory processing, sensory gating and novelty detection in human participants. Psychopharmacology. 2013; 225 (4):903-921 - 77.
Castro NG, Albuquerque EX. Alpha-Bungarotoxin-sensitive hippocampal nicotinic receptor channel has a high calcium permeability. Biophysical Journal. 1995; 68 (2):516-524 - 78.
Josef Golubic S, Susac A, Huonker R, Haueisen J, Supek S. Early attentional modulation of the neural network evoked with the auditory paired-click paradigm: An MEG study. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences. 2014; 126 :195-196 - 79.
Freedman R, Adams CE, Adler LE, Bickford PC, Gault J, Harris J, Leonard S. Inhibitory neurophysiological deficit as a phenotype for genetic investigation of schizophrenia. American Journal of Medical Genetics. 2000; 97 :58-64 - 80.
Baillet S, Mosher JC, Leahy RM. Electromagnetic brain mapping. IEEE Signal Processing Magazine. 2001; 18 :14-30 - 81.
Barkley GL. Controversies in neurophysiology. MEG is superior to EEG in localization of interictal epileptiform activity. Pro. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2004; 115 (5):1001-1009 - 82.
Josef Golubic S, Susac A, Grilj V, Ranken D, Huonker R, Haueisen J, Supek S. Size matters: MEG empirical and simulation study on source localization of the earliest visual activity in the occipital cortex. Medical & Biological Engineering & Computing. 2011; 4 :545-554 - 83.
Michel CM, Murray MM, Lantz G, Gonzalez S, Spinelli L, Grave de Peralta R. EEG source imaging. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2004; 115 (10):2195-2222 - 84.
Supek S, Stingl K, Josef Golubic S, Susac A, Ranken D. Optimal spatio-temporal matrix subdivision for cortical neurodynamics estimation. In: Proceedings of the 15th International Conference on Biomagnetism (BIOMAG 2006); Vancouver. 2006. pp. 180-181 - 85.
Felleman DJ, Van Essen DC. Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. Cerebral Cortex. 1991; 1 :1-147 - 86.
Lu BY, Edgar JC, Jones AP, Smith AK, Huang MX, Miller GA, Canive JM. Improved test–retest reliability of 50 ms paired-click auditory gating using magnetoencephalopgaphy source modeling. Psychophysiology. 2007; 44 :86-90 - 87.
Edgar JC, Huang MX, Weisend MP, Sherwood A, Miller GA, Adler LE, Canive JM. Interpreting abnormality: An EEG and MEG study of P50 and the auditory paired-stimulus paradigm. Biological Psychology. 2003; 65 :1-20 - 88.
Woods DL, Knight RT, Neville HJ. Bitemporal lesions dissociate auditory evoked potentials and perception. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 1984; 57 :208-220 - 89.
Korzyukova O, Pfliegere ME, Wagnerf M, Bowyer SM, Rosburgb T, Sundaresana K, Elgerb CE, Boutros NN. Generators of the intracranial P50 response in auditory sensory gating. NeuroImage. 2007; 35 :814-826 - 90.
Reite M, Teale P, Zimmerman J, Davis K, Whalen J. Source localization of a 50 msec latency auditory evoked field component. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 1988; 70 :490-498 - 91.
Thoma RJ, Hanlon FM, Moses SN, et al. M50 sensory gating predicts negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research. 2005; 73 :311-318 - 92.
Grunwald T, Boutros NN, Pezer N, von Oertzen J, Fernandez G, Schaller C, Elger CE.Neuronal substrates of sensory gating within the human brain. Biological Psychiatry. 2003; 53 :511-519 - 93.
Boutros NN, Trautner P, Rosburg T, Korzyukov O, Grunwald T, Schaller C, Elger CE, Kurthen M. Sensory gating in the human hippocampal and rhinal regions. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2005; 116 :1967-1974 - 94.
Josef Golubic S, Aine C, Supek S. Lack of the neuromagnetic activity in the frontal lobe of AD subjects. In: Proceedings of the 6th znanstveni sastanak Hrvatskog fizikalnog društva. 2009; Primosten. Zagreb: HFD; 2009. pp. 156-157 - 95.
Supek S, Golubic SJ, Bryant J, Donahue C, Montaño R, Adair J, Hart B, Knoefel J, Stephen J,Aine CJ. Neuromagnetic auditory activity reflects differences between normal aging, MCI and AD subjects: An oddball study. Biomagnetism: Transdisciplinary Research and Exploration. 2008; 1 :34-36 - 96.
Josef Golubic S, Jurasic MJ, Susac A, Huonker R, Gotz T, Haueisen J. Attention modulates topology and dynamics of auditory sensory gating. (Forthcoming; In submission) - 97.
Barbas H, Ghashghaei H, Dombrowski SM, Rempel-Clower NL. Medial prefrontal cortices are unified by common connections with superior temporal cortices and distinguished by input from memory-related areas in the rhesus monkey. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1999; 410 :343-367 - 98.
Croxson PL, Johansen-Berg H, Timothy EJ, Behrens M, Robson D, Pinsk MA, Gross CG, Richter W, Richter MC, Kastner S, Rushworth MPS. Quantitative investigation of connections of the prefrontal cortex in the human and macaque using probabilistic. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2005; 25 :8854-8866 - 99.
Cavada C, Company T, Tejedor J, Cruz-Rizzolo RJ, Reinoso-Suarez F. The anatomical connections of the macaque monkey orbitofrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex. 2000; 10 :220-242 - 100.
Summerfield C, Egner T. Expectation (and attention) in visual cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2009; 13 :403-409 - 101.
Atkinson RC, Shiffrin RM. Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In: Spence W, Spence JT, editors. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation. Vol. 2. New York: Academic Press; 1968. pp. 89-195 - 102.
Irimajiri R, Golob EJ, Starr A. Auditory brain-stem, middle- and long-latency evoked potentials in mild cognitive impairment. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2005; 116 (8):1918-1929 - 103.
Thomas C, VomBerg I, Rupp A, Seidl U, Schröder J, Roesch-Ely D. P50 gating deficit in Alzheimer dementia correlates to frontal neuropsychological function. Neurobiology of Aging. 2010; 31 :416-424 - 104.
Cheng CH, Pei-Ning W, Wan-Yu H, Yung-Yang L. Inadequate inhibition of redundant auditory inputs in Alzheimer’s disease: An MEG study. Biological Psychology. 2012; 89 :365-373 - 105.
Bajo R, Maestú F, Nevado A, Sancho M, Gutiérrez R, et al. Functional connectivity in mild cognitive impairment during a memory task: Implications for the disconnection hypothesis. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease. 2010; 22 (1):183-193 - 106.
Takayama Y. A delayed recall battery as a sensitive screening for mild cognitive impairment: Follow-up study of memory clinic patients after 10 years. Journal of Medical and Dental Sciences. 2010; 57 :177-184 - 107.
Canuet L, Pusil S, López ME, Bajo R, Pineda-Pardo JA, et al. Network disruption and cerebrospinal fluid amyloid-beta and phospho-tau levels in mild cognitive impairment. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015; 35 (28):10325-10330 - 108.
Wu JW, Hussaini SA, Bastille IM, Rodriguez GA, Mrejeru A, et al. Neuronal activity enhances tau propagation and tau pathology in vivo. Nature Neuroscience. 2016; 9 (8):1085-1092 - 109.
Cuesta P, Garcés P, Castellanos NP, López ME, Aurtenetxe S, et al. Influence of the APOE ε4 allele and mild cognitive impairment diagnosis in the disruption of the MEG resting state functional connectivity in sources space. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease. 2015; 44 (2):493-505 - 110.
Rapoport SI, Horwitz B, Haxby JV, Grady CL. Alzheimer's disease: Metabolic uncoupling of associative brain regions. The Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences. 1996; 13 (4):540-545 - 111.
Coleman RE. Positron emission tomography diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroimaging Clinics of North America. 2005; 15 :837-846 - 112.
Klunk WE, Engler H, Nordberg A, Blomqvist G, Holt DP, et al. Imaging brain amyloid in Alzheimer’s disease with Pittsburgh compound-B. Annals of Neurology. 2004; 55 :306-319 - 113.
Klupp E, Grimmer T, Tahmasian M, Sorg C, Yakushev I, et al. Prefrontal hypometabolism in Alzheimer disease is related to longitudinal amyloid accumulation in remote brain regions. Journal of Nuclear Medicine. 2015; 56 :399-404 - 114.
Sharma G, Vijayaraghavan S. Modulation of presynaptic store calcium induces release of glutamate and postsynaptic firing. Neuron. 2003; 38 (6):929-939