PV (ir) positive cell densities in the prefrontal cortex and amygdala in WKY and SD rats. (N=4/strain)
1. Introduction
1.1. Anxiety
Anxiety disorders are the most common psychiatric disorders with a worldwide lifetime prevalence of 16-29% (Kessler et al., 2005; Somers et al., 2006). People with anxiety disorders are likely to suffer from depression and drug (or alcohol) abuse in an effort to gain relief from their symptoms, therefore, eliciting secondary disorders (Kessler et al., 2005). Although each subtype (i.e. generalized anxiety disorder, obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), panic disorder, post traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and social phobia) has unique features, the core symptom of all anxiety disorders is excessive avoidance. The etiology of anxiety disorders remains elusive (the presumed role of trauma in PTSD notwithstanding). What is abundantly clear is anxiety disorders arise as a complex interaction of genetic, epigenetic, sociocultural factors with life experiences; that is, anxiety disorders are best explained with diathesis models (Kendler et al., 2002; Mineka and Zinbarg, 2006; Zinbarg and Barlow, 1996).
Among a variety of neurobiological and neurobehavioral factors representing a source of risk for anxiety disorders, inhibited temperament is consistently linked to anxiety disorders (Biederman et al., 1993; Fox et al., 2005a; Hirshfeld et al., 1992; Hirshfeld-Becker et al., 2007; Kagan et al., 1987; Rosenbaum et al., 1993; Smoller et al., 2003). Behavioral inhibition is characterized as extreme withdrawal in the face of social and nonsocial challenges (Fox et al., 2005b; Kagan et al., 1989; Rosenbaum et al., 1991). Those with inhibited temperament exhibit excessive physiological reactivity to environmental challenges (Kalin et al., 2000; Kalin and Shelton, 2003; Keltikangas-Jarvinen et al., 1999; Perez-Edgar et al., 2007; Schwartz et al., 2003; Smoller et al., 2005; Tyrka et al., 2006; Tyrka et al., 2008).
Although there is support for temperament as a risk factor, the translation of risk to actualized disorder is unclear. Acquisition, expression and retention of avoidance may be the final common pathway to anxiety disorders. The particularly debilitating feature of avoidance is that, left untreated, avoidance increases over time and leads to a worsening of symptoms. Avoidance acquisition is more apparent in PTSD; the growth of avoidance traces the full expression of PTSD (Karamustafalioglu et al., 2006; Kashdan et al., 2006; North et al., 2004; O'donnell et al., 2006a). Given this prominent position, avoidance learning may represent an endophenotype for anxiety disorders (Gould and Gottesman, 2006).
Here, we present the case for inbred WKY rats to serve as a model for risk of anxiety disorders. Evidence is presented for the concordance of neurobehavioral, neuroendocrine, and neurochemical features to that observed in humans at risk for expressing an anxiety disorder. Particular emphasis is placed on enhanced avoidance acquisition and resistance to extinction as an endophenotype for vulnerability to anxiety disorders. Implications for treatment and efficacy are discussed.
2. Putative animal model for vulnerability to anxiety disorders - The WKY rat
The Wistar Kyoto (WKY) rat strain was first developed as a normotensive control strain for the spontaneously hypertensive rat (SHR) strain derived from the Wistar (WIS) rat (Okamoto and Aoki, 1963). Unlike its parent strain WIS rat, the WKY rat exhibits many unique behavioral characteristics differing from an out-bred rat strain. The most significant features are behavioral withdrawal, propensity to avoid, hyper-responsiveness to stress and hypervigilance (Drolet et al., 2002; Lemos et al., 2011; McAuley et al., 2009
2.1. Temperament: Behavioral inhibition
The WKY rat displays features of inhibited temperament in a variety of situations (Braw et al., 2008; Ferguson and Cada, 2004; Malkesman et al., 2005; Pare, 1992b; Pare, 1994; Pare, 1996; Pare et al., 2001; Servatius et al., 1998; Tejani-Butt et al., 2003). For example, Figure 1 depicts activity in the open field test comparing WKY rats to outbred SD rats. Upon placement into the center of the open field (which is brightly lit), the WKY rat remains immobile for a period of time; the latency to leave the center segment is often 2-3 times as long as exhibited by outbred strains (Drolet et al., 2002; Ferguson and Cada, 2003; Nosek et al., 2008). This reluctance to leave the center segment is followed by slow deliberate exploration. However, activity will generally increase over several minutes. Hypolocomotion is not
secondary to a motoric disturbance, in that WKY rats exhibit normal motor activity in a running wheel (Ferguson and Cada, 2003), a rotarod (Ferguson et al., 2003b) and a turning wheel avoidance task (Pare, 1992a). Together, these data suggest that the lack of movement is not physical, but psychological. Moreover, inhibited temperament is displayed by both female and male WKY rats compared to their outbred counterparts.
Inhibited temperament extends to more explicit nonsocial and social threats. In terms of social interactions, WKY rats generally exhibit normal play behaviors with conspecifics (Braw et al., 2006; Malkesman et al., 2006b), but reductions in play and more subordinate-type behaviors when faced with outbred rats (Ferguson and Cada, 2004). In response to an electrified probe, normal rats bury the probe; WKY rats simply freeze (Ahmadiyeh et al., 2005; Carr and Lucki, 2010; Gutiérrez-Mariscal et al., 2008; Pare, 1994). Thus, WKY rats represent an animal model of behavioral withdrawal in the face of social and non-social challenges.
2.2. Anxiety signs and symptoms
As stated earlier, the core feature of anxiety disorders is avoidance. There are a variety of expressions of avoidance; however, all will have a common process of acquisition and resistance to extinction. In addition to avoidance as a learned response, common features of anxiety disorders are altered arousal, social interaction, communication, attention, learning and memory. Still, each anxiety disorder has distinct features. Thus, endophenotypes may relate to the core features of avoidance learning, common characteristics (e.g., heighten arousal), or disorder-specific features (e.g., compulsions). Accordingly, behavioral assessments may be sensitive to a particular aspect or a general process concordant with anxiety.
2.2.1. Arousal
Arousal has two general aspects, the basal or undisturbed state and the relative magnitude of response to challenges. Moreover, arousal may be indexed through neuroendocrine or neurobehavioral measures. For each, the WKY has documented abnormalities.
Neuroendocrine and neurochemical. Within the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis (HPAA), levels of corticosterone (CORT) and adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) are measured to evaluate arousal levels as affected by circadian rhythms and stress (Ottenweller et al., 1994). WKY rats has been proposed as a model of stress vulnerability, exhibiting exaggerated HPAA responses to stress regimens compared to common rat strains (Pare et al., 1999b; Pare and Kluczynski, 1997; Pare and Redei, 1993b; Redei et al., 1994). Basal peripheral ACTH and CORT levels are generally higher in WKY rats and remained significantly higher after the diurnal peak as compared to WIS rats (Solberg et al., 2001). Moreover, WKY rats exhibit a sustained CORT response to acute stress and an enhanced plasma ACTH response to various stressors (De La Garza II and Mahoney III, 2004; Malkesman et al., 2006a; Pare and Redei, 1993a; Rittenhouse et al., 2002). Others reported that CRH content and mRNA binding are not different in WKY rats relative to other strains suggesting that a defective negative feedback system may contribute to hyperresponsive HPAA in WKY rats (Gomez et al., 1996; Redei et al., 1994). Together, neuroendocrine evidence suggests that WKY rats are inherently hyperresponsive to stress. Exaggerated HPAA activity is reminiscent of inhibited temperament (Smoller et al., 2003; Smoller et al., 2005).
In general the neurochemical profile of the WKY rat is aberrant compared to outbred rats. WKY rats have inherently low butyrylcholinesterase level (Figure 2) and activity (Servatius et al., 1998), leading to greater sensitivity to cholinomimetics (Beck et al., 2001). Among the neurotransmitters, WKY rats have altered levels of monoamines, namely norepinephrine (NE), dopamine (DA) and serotonin (5-HT), and their metabolites as compared to out-bred strains with a high degree of specificity in various regions (De La Garza II and Mahoney III, 2004; Ferguson et al., 2003a; Pardon et al., 2003; Scholl et al., 2010). Moreover, these monoamine systems show greater responsiveness in the face of acute stress (Pardon et al., 2002; Pardon et al., 2000) and chronic stress (Pardon et al., 2003). We will discuss this point in a later section (section 5).
Neurobehavioral. The acoustic startle response (ASR) is a simple reflex used to index arousal and vigilance in mammals (Ardekani et al., 1989). The ASR can be used to reveal differences in sensitivity (threshold to elicit a reflex response), responsivity (the magnitude of response), latency, as well as nonassociative processes of habituation, dishabituation and sensitization.
We and others demonstrated that WKY rats made larger startle responses to a white noise within a wide range (92dB to 120dB) as compared to other inbred and outbred rat strains (Glowa and Hansen, 1994; McAuley et al., 2009; Servatius et al., 1998). Of 45 inbred and outbred rat strains including SD rats, male WKY rats exhibited the highest ASR magnitude when exposed to 8 trials of 110dB white noise (Glowa and Hansen, 1994). We found that WKY rats exhibited significantly higher startle responsivity at 92 and 102dB white noise after correction for each subject’s body weight (Figure 3a). In addition to greater startle responsivity, male WKY showed higher sensitivity compared to male SD rats, measured by a multi-intensity startle test (3-ASR) (Figure 3b). Although both male and female WKY exhibit substantially higher ASRs compared to SD rats, only male rats demonstrate habituation when single intensity startle test (1-ASR) was used (Figure 4). Yet others reported that WKY show similar or lower ASR magnitude compared to SD rats (Buuse, 2004; Palmer et al., 2000). We reasoned the inconsistency may be due to variant procedures used and whether subjects’ body weight was factored into the startle response.
WKY rats exhibited exaggerated stress response and elevated arousal following stress stimulation. Stress has been described as one of the key risk factors of anxiety disorders (Chantarujikapong et al., 2001; Grillon et al., 2007b; Mineka and Zinbarg, 1996 ). As described in previous literatures, WKY rats are behaviorally hyperresponsive to stress with the stress-induced exaggerated HPAA response (Redei et al., 1994; Solberg et al., 2001). Inasmuch as the basal behavioral state of WKY is abnormal, assessing the impact of stress on behavioral reactivity has been problematic. For example, assessing freezing to context or to cues is difficult given the propensity to freeze in novel situations. However, prior acute stress have been noted to increase freezing behavior and reduce activity in the OFT and elevated plus maze in WKYs (Nosek et al., 2008). When challenged in the forced swim test (FST), the WKY rat predominantly exhibits floating behavior and fewer struggling responses compared to other strains. The lack of struggling has been interpreted as ‘behavioral despair’, a sign of depression-like behavior in rodents (Malkesman and Weller, 2009; Pare, 1992a; Pare andRedei, 1993a).
The heightened stress reactivity is most clearly evident in the enhanced susceptibility to develop stress-induced ulcers in WKY rats compared to outbred strains (Pare, 1989a; Pare, 1989c; Pare and Schimmel, 1986). Pretreatment with drugs that elevate central monoamines reduce the severity of ulceration (Pare et al., 1999a; Tejani-Butt et al., 2003). Evidence from elevated arousal or exaggerated stress response in WKY rats may provide insight to the alterations in the CNS neurochemistry that may be responsible for anxiety. The effects of stress are discussed in the following sections in more detail.
2.2.2. Sleep disturbances
Sleeping disruption is one of the major symptoms of anxiety disorder and a hallmark of PTSD (Ross et al., 1989). WKY rats exhibit altered sleep-wake cycle and longer rapid eye movement sleep (REMS) episodes compared to other strains (Dugovic et al., 2000). REMS fragmentation was significantly altered following stress in WKY rats compared to control strain (DaSilva et al., 2011; Dugovic et al., 2000; Laitman et al.). Altered sleep patterns may also preexist as a vulnerability to anxiety, that are further disturbed after exposure to psychological distress.
2.2.3. Avoidance
As the core symptom of all anxiety disorders, avoidance behavior differs between patients with anxiety disorder and normal population (Foa et al., 2006; O'Donnell et al., 2006b). In humans avoidance is characterized in the form of emotions, ideations, and behaviors. In animal models, avoidance is characterized as passive (withholding a likely response to avoid aversive stimulation) and active (performing a target response to prevent aversive stimulation). Early work showed WKY rats exhibit superior acquisition in passive avoidance tasks compared to SD or WIS rats (Pare 1993; Pare 1996). Given that being immobile/freezing is the dominant coping strategy for WKY rats, superior acquisition of passive avoidance is not a surprise. However, when tested with a wheel-turning avoidance task, WKY performed equally well compared to WIS rat (Pare, 1992a). In contrast, Berger and Starzec found that WKY rats performed poorly in lever-press avoidance task compared to SHR rats (Berger and Starzec, 1988). As an arbitrary target response, a leverpress is not among the species specific defense reactions (Bolles, 1970). We reasoned that the inconsistency between studies and laboratories may due to the procedure applied, the nature of the test and the reference strain to which WKYs were compared.
In our institute, we utilized a signalled lever-press avoidance paradigm to study anxiety and its vulnerability in rats. It is known that anxiety is a disorder that develops over a period of time, so is the avoidance. Thus, a lever-press avoidance learning model allows the acquisition of avoidance to develop over an extended period of time, mimicking the developmental process of anxiety in humans. Our data indicated that the learning of WKY rats is superior in the lever-press avoidance task compared to a noninhibited reference strain, the SD rat (Beck et al., 2010; Jiao et al., 2011; Servatius et al., 2008). The superior active avoidance performance of WKY rats is in stark contrast to other rat strains with features of trait behavioral inhibition such as the Maudsley High Reactive (Blizard and Adams, 2002), which are generally poor in active avoidance. Moreover, rats bred for superior active avoidance are typically the least behaviorally inhibited (Syracuse high avoiders, Roman high avoiders and Australian high avoiders) (Aguilar et al., 2004; Brush, 2003; Driscoll, 1986; Overstreet et al., 1990; Overstreet et al., 1992).
Over the past several years we have amassed a substantial database concerning the avoidance performance of WKY rats. For one, the avoidance performance of WKY rats reaches asymptotic levels that approach unity; that is, once acquired WKY rats typically exhibit near perfect avoidance (Figure 5.). That perfect avoidance begins with the first trial of a session. Outbred rats display a typical pattern of avoidance performance in which each session begins with escape responses, although avoidance was expressed at the end of the previous session (i.e., warm up) (Hineline, 1978a; Hineline, 1978b). WKY rats generally do not exhibit warm up as acquisition progresses. This near perfect expression of avoidance resembles human avoidance. Facilitated avoidance acquisition is apparent in both female and male rats compared to their respective outbred counterparts. The near perfect avoidance behavior also insulates the rat from experiencing changes in shock contingencies. Accordingly, WKYs display perseveration of avoidance responding in the absence of shock, but continued presence of the explicit safety signal (Servatius et al., 2008). Perseveration/resistance to extinction has been implicated in neuropathology of anxiety (Barad, 2005; Myers and Davis, 2002).
WKY rats also display another interesting and potentially clinically-relevant feature. Each training session begins with a 60-s stimulus free period prior to the first warning signal. As WKY rats acquire avoidance they emit bar presses, which are not reinforced, during this period (Figure 6.). This pattern of response is only exhibited prior to the first trial; nonreinforced responses are rarely displayed on subsequent trials. These nonreinforced responses may be akin to worry (Mineka, 2004), accompanying avoidance acquisition only in those at risk.
Lever-press during the 1st min of each session in male and female SD and WKY rats
On the other hand, stress intensity is often cited as a contributing factor in the development of anxiety disorders (Braunstein-Bercovitz et al., 2001; Foa et al., 2006; Grillon et al., 2007b; Grillon et al., 2007a; Mineka and Zinbarg, 1996 ; Silver et al., 2002). Given the relationship between anxiety disorders and avoidance, one expects stress to accelerate avoidance acquisition, raise the asymptotic levels, or affect extinction. The results from a recent study suggest that stressor intensity only affects extinction. Training with a greater shock intensity than our standard, did not affect the acquisition curves of either SD or WKY rats (Figure 7.) (Jiao et al., 2011). Of course, there is little room to enhance asymptotic performance of WKY rats, but the rate to reach this level could differ. However, WKY rats trained with higher intensity shock exhibited perseveration of avoidance response during extinction compared to WKY rats trained with the lower intensity shock; extinction curves of SD rats resembled WKY rats trained with a lower intensity.
A long standing discrepancy between the basic science literature and clinical descriptions concerns the relationship between avoidance acquisition and arousal. In rats, arousal decreases as avoidance is acquired (Coover et al., 1973). However, arousal is sustained in humans with anxiety disorders and who are employing avoidance. Therefore, we assessed ASRs prior to avoidance acquisition and toward the end of acquisition training. Whereas the ASRs of SD rats are virtually unchanged between the measures, the ASRs of WKY rats
3. Genetic components
Genetic components (trait vulnerability) play an important role in various psychiatric disorders, including anxiety disorders. Quantitative trait loci (QTL) analysis indicated that common loci, which influence certain behavioral characteristics tested by OFT and defensive bury test in rats, may represent genetic factors contributing to anxiety and depression (Ahmadiyeh et al., 2005; Boyle and Gill, 2001; Cloninger et al., 1998; Henderson et al., 2000; Solberg et al., 2004). Several QTL (
4. Brain anatomy and neuronal activity
Converging data from structural and functional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies suggest that differential patterns of anatomical brain abnormalities appear to be involved in mood and anxiety disorders (Brambilla et al., 2002; Lind et al., 2006; Milad et al. ; Ohara et al., 2004). Abnormalities were found in orbital frontal lobe, basal ganglia, temporal lobe and hippocampus in patients with various subtypes of anxiety (Brambilla et al., 2002; Bystritsky et al., 2001; McEwen, 2005). Moreover, imaging data from humans suggest that medial prefrontal cortex, amygdala, thalamus and periaquaductal gray are involved in avoidance acquisition (Mobbs et al., 2007; Samanez-Larkin et al., 2008; Simmons et al., 2006; Straube et al., 2009; Suslow et al., 2009). However, the neurocircuitry underlying avoidance extinction and its perseveration in anxiety states is largely a matter of speculation. Mainly supported by fear extinction studies, mPFC plays a key role in both humans and experimental animals (rodents). Anxiety patients exhibit reduced activity of the anterior cingulate cortex and thalamus during episodes of re-experiencing (Hopper et al., 2007; Lanius et al., 2003), leading to a suggestion that reduced cortical influence on structures such as the amygdala may explain the resistance to extinction. Reduced cortical influence on the amygdala is also presumed to underlie the persistent expression of heightened arousal (e.g., increased acoustic startle) observed in PTSD patients (Nutt and Malizia, 2004).
The knowledge of anatomical difference between WKY and other rats is quite limited. A recent volumetric study evaluated hippocampus in female rats in which the hippocampal volume of WKY rats is 20% less than that of WIS rats (Lemos et al., 2011). On the other hand, alterations in neuronal activation are demonstrated in the expression of c-Fos or brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) in various brain regions in WKY rats compared to out-bred rat strains (Ma and Morilak, 2004; O'Mahony et al., 2011). We recently reported that the c-Fos immunoreactivity is lower in the medial prefrontal cortex in the WKY rat than the SD rat at the end of extinction of a lever-press avoidance task, whereas a reduced GABAergic activation was found in basal amygdala in the WKY rat trained with higher shock intensity (Jiao et al., 2011).
However, the structures that are critical for acquisition and extinction of lever press avoidance are relatively unknown. In our initial work, we assessed c-Fos immunoreactivity in various regions in the SD rat at multiple time points within the phases of acquisition and extinction. Our results indicated that there is a trend of increased c-Fos expression in the prefrontal regions as extinction starts and proceeds. Interestingly, a similar pattern was observed in the lateral and basolateral amygdala where a reduced activity was expected during extinction. A further analysis targeting GABAergic neuron revealed that GABAergic activation arises during the extinction phase. An elevated GABAergic activation (detected by the double staining for c-Fos and parvalbumin (PV, a calcium binding protein that is expressed in 50-60% amygdalar GABAergic neurons (Kemppainen and Pitkanen, 2000)) in the basolateral and lateral amygdala would reduce the excitatory output from the amygdala as GABA neurons in the basolateral amygdala are mainly interneurons that make synaptic contact on projection neurons (Rosenkranz and Grace, 2001). Thus an elevated GABAergic activity may be responsible for the increased neuronal activation during extinction phase in the basolateral amygdala. However, this assumption needs further investigation since parvalbumin positive neurons represent 50-60% of GABAergic neurons in the basolateral amygdala (Kemppainen and Pitkanen, 2000). We do not as yet know whether these neuronal alterations are present or different in WKY rats.
From a different perspective, enhanced avoidance learning may be secondary to deficits in neurotrophins. Converging data supports the role of neurotrophins in mood disorders; one of the latest theories of the neuropathology of anxiety and depression disorders (Chen et al., 2006; Martinowich et al., 2007). For example, low levels of the brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) are found in stress-related disorders in humans (Duman and Monteggia, 2006). In general, lower BDNF is related to anxiety and depression disorders that are not responsive to serotonergic antidepressant treatment (Duman, 2004; Kalueff et al., 2006). Consistent with this perspective, a recent study illustrated that BDNF levels are significantly lower in the CA3 of hippocampus in WKY rats compared to SD rats (Malkesman et al., 2009).
5. Potential mechanisms in psychopharmacology
Alterations in neurochemistry in the CNS are implicated in various psychiatric disorders (i.e. depression, addiction and anxiety). Recent investigations targeting central neurochemical pathways have enhanced our understanding of anxiety disorders.
5.1. Serotonin (5-HT)
Evidence for the association of altered serotonergic activity in anxiety, that decreasing serotonergic function is anxiogenic and increasing it anxiolytic, is mostly supported by the use of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) in various sub-types of anxiety disorders (Bremner, 2006; Vaswani et al., 2003). WKY rats showed significant lower basal 5-HT tissue level in limbic regions and cell body area compared to WIS or SD rats (De La Garza II and Mahoney III, 2004; Scholl et al., 2010). Acute stress elicits an increased tissue level of 5-HT in the amygdala in WIS but not in WKY rats, it increased 5-HT turnover rate in the mPFC only in WKY rats (De La Garza II and Mahoney III, 2004). When exposed to chronic stress, WKY rats failed to show stress-induced reduction of 5-HT tissue level as SD rats did, whereas the turnover rate was increased in both WKY and SD rats (O'Mahony et al., 2011). Previous studies showed increased binding of 5-HT1a receptors in hippocampus and hypothalamus, but decreased binding of 5-HT transporters in the cell body area in WKY rats following chronic stress, compared to SD rats (Pare and Tejani-Butt, 1996). However WKY rats are insensitive to serotonergic drugs (e.g. SSRIs and receptor agonists) in terms of activity in the EPM and OFT, immobility in the FST, or severity of gastric ulceration after stress (Chaouloff et al., 1998; Griebel et al., 1999; Lahmame and Armario, 1996; Lopez-Rubalcava and Lucki, 2000; Pare et al., 2001; Tejani-Butt et al., 2003), suggesting serotonergic manipulation may not affect the temperamental behaviors of these rats.
5.2. Dopamine (DA)
Converging literatures demonstrate that an aberrant DA circuitry is associated with anxiety disorders (Gendreau et al., 1998; Hamner and Diamond, 1996; Mathew et al., 1981; Taylor et al., 1982).WKY rats exhibit altered dopaminergic function in various brain regions. DA levels in WKY rats do not differ between WIS or SD rats in most brain areas; DA turnover is higher in the nucleus accumbens shell in the WKY rat compared to the WIS rat (De La Garza II and Mahoney III, 2004; Ferguson et al., 2003a; Scholl et al., 2010). Receptor and transporter binding studies show that WKY rats have altered dopaminergic pathways compared to control strains. The results from those studies reveal a significant strain difference, with WKY rats exhibiting lower levels of D1 receptor binding in the caudate putamen and nucleus accumbens core, but higher binding levels in the substantia nigra pars reticulata compared to WIS rats (Novick et al., 2008). D1 receptors in the substantia nigra are involved in mediating the startle response (Meloni and Davis, 1999), thus a higher D1 receptor level in this region may lead to heightened ASR magnitude in WKY rats. Results from a recent study demonstrated that WKY rats exhibited higher D2 receptor binding levels in the nucleus accumbens shell and ventral tegmental area, but lower D2 receptor binding in the caudate putamen, nucleus accumbens core and hypothalamus compared to WIS rats (Yaroslavsky et al., 2006). It is known that the D1 and D2 receptors represent critical sites where DA acts to modify behavior related to anxiety and reward; the altered expression of this receptor in the WKY rat may be reflective of the anxiety susceptibility noted in this rat strain. Moreover, DA transporter binding levels were lower in the nucleus accumbens core, amygdala and cell body regions (ventral tegmental area and substantia nigra), but higher in the hippocampus and hypothalamus compared to SD and WIS rats (Jiao et al., 2003). The observed differences in the density and distribution of DAT sites in WKY rats may lead to altered modulation of synaptic DA levels in the cell body and mesolimbic regions, thereby contributing to the noted anxiety- and depression-like behaviors reported in this rat strain. This speculation was supported by a further study in which some of the alterations in DAT binding was reversed by chronic nomifensine (i.e. a dopamine transporter blocker) treatment (Jiao et al., 2006). Moreover, after 8-12 days of nomifensine administration, WKY rats showed significantly increased head poke responses in the emergence test, reduced latency to leave the center in an open field and increased activity in the FST (Tejani-Butt et al., 2003). Therefore, the WKY rat may represent a good model for a sub-type of anxiety disorder exhibiting imbalanced DA distribution in the CNS.
5.3. Norepinephrine (NE)
Defective noradrenergic function is one of the major mechanisms in the neuropathology of anxiety in human (Bremner et al., 1996; Charney and Redmond, Jr., 1983; Hamner and Diamond, 1996; Neumeister et al., 2005; Sullivan et al., 1999). The WKY appears to have normal tissue levels of NE (De La Garza II and Mahoney III, 2004; O'Mahony et al., 2011; Scholl et al., 2010) and tyrosine hydroxylase (TH, the rate-limiting enzyme in NE synthesis) (Mann and Bell, 1991; Vachette et al., 1993). In response to acute stress, noradrenergic reactivity appears to be blunted. For example, acute stress-induced increases of NE tissue levels in the lateral bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BSTL) were significantly lower in WKY compared to SD rats (Pardon et al., 2002; Pardon et al., 2003). Moreover, the acute stress-induced increases in neuronal activity (cFos expression) in medial amygdala and locus ceruleus (LC) was lower in WKY rats compared to SD rats (Ma and Morilak, 2004). Alternatively, NE reactivity may be delayed. Acute stress-induced increases of TH mRNA level in WKY rats were apparent, but delayed by 2-hr in WKY rats compared to control strains (Pardon et al., 2002; Sands et al., 2000). Although reactivity to acute stress is blunted, chronic stress appears to sensitize NE in WKY rats. After chronic exposure to cold stress, increased BSTL NE release was induced by acute stress challenge only in WKY rats, but not in SD rats (Pardon et al., 2003). Chronic stress decreases NE transporter binding in the cell body area and decreases α2 and β receptor binding in terminal regions, suggesting aberrant NE modulatory responses towards stress in WKY rats (Tejani-Butt et al., 1994; Zafar et al., 1997). Although the NE response to acute stress is blunted,, WKY rats may exhibit less habituation to stress appearing as larger levels of reactivity as the reactivity of SD rats declines. A sustained NE response may underlie hypervigilance and elevated arousal in response to specific or generalized stress (Cameron et al., 2004; Lahdesmaki et al., 2002; Maes et al., 2002; Schramm et al., 2001). The efficacy of NE-targeted drugs in altering behaviors relevant to anxiety is limited. Much of the past research has focused on altering behaviors in the FST. Drugs which act by blocking NE transporters reduced immobility in FST and increase activity in the OFT in WKY rats (Lahmame and Armario, 1996; Lucki and Nobler, 1985; Pare, 1992b; Pare
5.4. Others
There is a dearth of knowledge about strain difference between the WKY rat and comparison strains in the glutamatergic and the GABAergic pathways. A recent autoradiographic study demonstrated that WKY rats exhibited lower N-methyl-d-aspartate (NMDA) receptor binding in anterior cingulate cortex, caudate putamen, nucleus accumbens, CA1 of hippocampus and substantia nigra compared to WIS rats (Lei et al., 2009). Interestingly, chronic stress increased NMDA receptor binding in the prefrontal cortex, caudate putamen and nucleus accumbens only in the WKY rat (Lei and Tejani-Butt, 2010). Thus the authors speculated that NMDA receptors in these regions may be more sensitive to stress in WKY rats. Consistent with the previous report (Jiao et al., 2011), our recent preliminary data suggest that naïve WKY rats exhibited altered density of PV immunoreactive cells in the amygdala and prefrontal cortex compared to SD rats (Table 1.). Although basal tissue level of GABA does not differ between WKY and WIS rats, stress increased it only in WKY rats (O'Mahony
REGIONS | STRAIN | MEAN PV (ir) CELL DENSITY + S.E.M. (CELL COUNTS/mm3) |
Anterior Cingulate | SD | 2208.08 + 716.27 |
WKY | 3503.53 + 111.97 | |
Prelimbic Cortex | SD | 2364.65 + 281.96 |
WKY | 3181.51 + 241.74 | |
Infralimbic Cortex | SD | 2891.72 + 408.16 |
WKY | 2989.72 + 442.44 | |
Basal Amygdala | SD | 2939.07 + 211.16 |
WKY | 1786.03 + 445.31 | |
Lateral Amygdala | SD | 2294.93 + 251.59 |
WKY | 1427.58 + 265.70 |
5.5. Summary
The WKY rat exhibits a neurochemical profile reminiscent of anxiety disorders. The profile is also reminiscent of behavioral inhibition in nonhuman primates (Kalin, 2004; Kalin and Shelton, 2003; Kalin et al., 2007). Past research has focused on the WKY as animal model of depression owing to the stress reactivity, susceptibility to ulcers, and reduced FST. Faster avoidance acquisition, avoidance perseveration and sustained arousal induced by avoidance learning are strong signs of anxiety vulnerability. Thus, investigation of psychotropic drugs in their ability to affect avoidance may have relevance for treatment of human anxiety. Recent findings of GABAergic activity selective to avoidance acquisition and extinction in WKY rats suggest a novel target for this class of anxiolytics.
6. Overall summary
Anxiety disorders develop as an interaction of trait vulnerabilities (e.g., behavioral inhibition), early life experiences and environmental exposures. The interplay of these influences determines success or failures to effectively cope, especially under stress. WKY rats model trait inhibited temperament, a risk factor for anxiety disorders. Accordingly, WKY rats have neurobehavioral, neuroanatomical, neurochemical and neuropharmacological features consistent with inhibited temperament. In particular, WKY rats acquire active avoidance faster; avoidance that is resistant to extinction. Moreover, WKY rats display two behavioral features which are hallmarks of anxiety as avoidance develops: worry and increased arousal. In the respect of neurochemistry and neuroendocrine, WKY rats demonstrated over stimulated NE circuitry and exaggerated HPAA activity in response to stress stimulation, and innate 5-HT deficit together with altered receptors function in monoamine systems and GABAergic system, which all contribute to hyper-reactivity towards stress and the phenotypes that related to anxiety behavior in this rat strain. Together, these significant findings suggest that the WKY rats should be studied as an animal model of vulnerability to develop anxiety disorders.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by NIH grants and a Department of Veterans Affairs Merit Review research program and the Stress & Motivated Behavior Institute of the New Jersey Medical School. The described experimentations were conducted with approval by the VANJHCS Institutional Animal Care and Use and Research and Development Committees. The authors would like to thank the technical expertise of Toni Marie Dispenziere, Tracey Longo and Amanda Agley in the behavioral testing of the animals in these experiments.
References
- 1.
Aguilar R. Gil L. Fernandez-Teruel A. Tobena A. 2004 Genetically-based behavioral traits influence the effects of Shuttle Box avoidance overtraining and extinction upon intertrial responding: a study with the Roman rat strains.66 63 72 - 2.
Ahmadiyeh N. Churchill G. A. Solberg L. C. Baum A. E. Shimomura K. Takahashi J. S. Redei E. E. 2005 Lineage is an epigenetic modifier of QTL influencing behavioral coping with stress.35 189 198 - 3.
Ardekani A. M. Walker S. J. Donohue S. J. Stitzel R. E. Connors J. M. Vrana K. E. 1989 Adrenocorticotropin and corticosterone levels in pre-weanling spontaneously hypertensive rats.44 919 925 - 4.
Barad M. 2005 Fear extinction in rodents: basic insight to clinical promise.15 710 715 - 5.
Beck K. D. Zhu G. Beldowicz D. Brennan F. X. Ottenweller J. E. Moldow R. L. Servatius R. J. 2001 Central nervous system effects from a peripherally acting cholinesterase inhibiting agent: interaction with stress or genetics.933 310 314 - 6.
Berger D. F. Starzec J. J. 1988 Contrasting Lever-Press Avoidance Behaviors of Spontaneously Hypertensive and Normotensive Rats (Rattus norvegicus).102 279 286 - 7.
Biederman J. Rosenbaum J. F. Bolduc-Murphy E. A. Faraone S. V. Chaloff J. Hirshfeld D. R. Kagan J. 1993 A 3-year follow-up of children with and without behavioral inhibition.32 814 821 - 8.
Blizard D. A. Adams N. 2002 The Maudsley Reactive and Nonreactive strains: a new perspective.32 277 299 - 9.
Bolles R. C. 1970 Species-specific defense reactions and avoidance learning.77 32 48 - 10.
Boyle A. E. Gill K. 2001 Sensitivity of AXB/BXA recombinant inbred lines of mice to the locomotor activating effects of cocaine: a quantitative trait loci analysis.11 255 264 - 11.
Brambilla P. Barale F. Caverzasi E. Soares J. C. 2002 Anatomical MRI findings in mood and anxiety disorders.11 88 99 - 12.
Braunstein-Bercovitz H. mentman-Ashkenazi I. Lubow R. E. 2001 Stress affects the selection of relevant from irrelevant stimuli.1 182 192 - 13.
Braw Y. Malkesman O. Dagan M. Bercovich A. Lavi-Avnon Y. Schroeder M. Overstreet D. H. Weller A. 2006 Anxiety-like behaviors in pre-pubertal rats of the Flinders Sensitive Line (FSL) and Wistar-Kyoto (WKY) animal models of depression.167 261 269 - 14.
Braw Y. Malkesman O. Merenlender A. Bercovich A. Dagan M. Overstreet D. H. Weller A. 2008 Withdrawal emotional-regulation in infant rats from genetic animal models of depression.193 94 100 - 15.
Bremner J. D. 2006 The relationship between cognitive and brain changes in posttraumatic stress disorder.1071 80 86 - 16.
Bremner J. D. Krystal J. H. Southwick S. M. Charney D. S. 1996 Noradrenergic mechanisms in stress and anxiety: II. Clinical studies.23 39 51 - 17.
Brush F. R. 2003 The Syracuse strains, selectively bred for differences in active avoidance learning, may be models of genetic differences in trait and state anxiety.6 77 85 - 18.
Buuse M. v. d. 2004 Prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle in spontaneously hypertensive rats.154 331 337 - 19.
Bystritsky A. Pontillo D. Powers M. Sabb F. W. Craske M. G. Bookheimer S. Y. 2001 Functional MRI changes during panic anticipation and imagery exposure.12 3953 3957 - 20.
Cameron O. G. Abelson J. L. Young E. A. 2004 Anxious and depressive disorders and their comorbidity: Effect on central nervous system noradrenergic function.56 875 883 - 21.
Carr G. V. Lucki I. 2010 Comparison of the kappa-opioid receptor antagonist DIPPA in tests of anxiety-like behavior between Wistar Kyoto and Sprague Dawley rats.210 295 302 - 22.
Chantarujikapong S. I. Scherrer J. F. Xian H. Eisen S. A. Lyons M. J. Goldberg J. Tsuang M. True W. R. 2001 A twin study of generalized anxiety disorder symptoms, panic disorder symptoms and post-traumatic stress disorder in men.103 133 145 - 23.
Chaouloff F. Aguerre S. Mormede P. 1998 GR 127935 and (+)-WAY 100135 do not affect TFMPP-induced inhibition of 5-HT synthesis in the midbrain and hippocampus of Wistar-Kyoto rats.37 1159 1167 - 24.
Charney D. S. Redmond D. E. Jr 1983 Neurobiological mechanisms in human anxiety. Evidence supporting central noradrenergic hyperactivity.22 1531 1536 - 25.
Chen Z. Y. Jing D. Bath K. G. Ieraci A. Khan T. Siao C. J. Herrera D. G. Toth M. Yang C. Mc Ewen B. S. Hempstead B. L. Lee F. S. 2006 Genetic variant BDNF (Val66Met) polymorphism alters anxiety-related behavior.314 140 143 - 26.
Cloninger C. R. Van E. P. Goate A. Edenberg H. J. Blangero J. Hesselbrock V. Reich T. Nurnberger J. Jr Schuckit M. Porjesz B. Crowe R. Rice J. P. Foroud T. Przybeck T. R. Almasy L. Bucholz K. Wu W. Shears S. Carr K. Crose C. Willig C. Zhao J. Tischfield J. A. Li T. K. Conneally P. M. and 1998 Anxiety proneness linked to epistatic loci in genome scan of human personality traits.81 313 317 - 27.
Coover G. D. Ursin H. Levine S. 1973 Plasma-corticosterone levels during active-avoidance learning in rats.82 170 174 - 28.
Da Silva. J. K. Lei Y. Madan V. Mann G. L. Ross R. J. Tejani-Butt S. Morrison A. R. 2011 Fear conditioning fragments REM sleep in stress-sensitive Wistar-Kyoto, but not Wistar, rats.35 67 73 - 29.
De La Garza I. I. R. Mahoney I. I. I. J. J. 2004 A distinct neurochemical profile in WKY rats at baseline and in response to acute stress: implications for animal models of anxiety and depression.1021 209 218 - 30.
Driscoll P. 1986 Roman high- and low-avoidance rats: present status of the Swiss sublines, RHA/Verh and RLA/Verh, and effects of amphetamine on shuttle-box performance.16 355 364 - 31.
Drolet G. Proulx K. Pearson D. Rochford J. Deschepper C. F. 2002 Comparisons of behavioral and neurochemical characteristics between WKY, WKHA, and Wistar rat strains.27 400 409 - 32.
Dugovic C. Solberg L. C. Redei E. Van R. O. Turek F. W. 2000 Sleep in the Wistar-Kyoto rat, a putative genetic animal model for depression.11 627 631 - 33.
Duman R. S. 2004 Role of neurotrophic factors in the etiology and treatment of mood disorders.5 11 25 - 34.
Duman R. S. Monteggia L. M. 2006 A neurotrophic model for stress-related mood disorders.59 1116 1127 - 35.
Ferguson S. A. Cada A. M. 2003 A longitudinal study of short- and long-term activity levels in male and female spontaneously hypertensive, Wistar-Kyoto, and Sprague-Dawley rats.117 271 282 - 36.
Ferguson S. A. Gough B. J. Cada A. M. 2003a In vivo basal and amphetamine-induced striatal dopamine and metabolite levels are similar in the spontaneously hypertensive, Wistar-Kyoto and Sprague-Dawley male rats.80 109 114 - 37.
Ferguson S. A. Gray E. P. Cada A. M. 2003b Early behavioral development in the spontaneously hypertensive rat: a comparison with the Wistar-Kyoto and Sprague-Dawley strains.117 263 270 - 38.
Ferguson S. A. Cada A. M. 2004 Spatial learning/memory and social and nonsocial behaviors in the Spontaneously Hypertensive, Wistar-Kyoto and Sprague-Dawley rat strains.77 583 594 - 39.
Foa E. B. Stein D. J. Mc Farlane A. C. 2006 Symptomatology and psychopathology of mental health problems after disaster. 67 Suppl2 15 25 - 40.
Fox N. A. Henderson H. A. Marshall P. J. Nichols K. E. Ghera M. M. 2005a Behavioral inhibition: linking biology and behavior within a developmental framework.56 235 262 - 41.
Fox N. A. Henderson H. A. Marshall P. J. Nichols K. E. Ghera M. M. 2005b Behavioral inhibition: linking biology and behavior within a developmental framework.56 235 262 - 42.
Gendreau P. L. Petitto J. M. Gariepy J. L. Lewis M. H. 1998 D2-like dopamine receptor mediation of social-emotional reactivity in a mouse model of anxiety: Strain and experience effects.18 210 221 - 43.
Glowa J. Hansen C. 1994 Differences in response to an acoustic startle stimulus among forty-six rat strains.24 79 84 - 44.
Gomez F. Lahmame A. de Kloet E. R. Armario A. 1996 Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal response to chronic stress in five inbred rat strains: differential responses are mainly located at the adrenocortical level.63 327 337 - 45.
Gould T. D. Gottesman I. I. 2006 Psychiatric endophenotypes and the development of valid animal models.5 113 119 - 46.
Griebel G. Cohen C. Perrault G. Sanger D. J. 1999 Behavioral Effects of Acute and Chronic Fluoxetine in Wistar-Kyoto Rats.67 315 320 - 47.
Grillon C. Avenevoli S. Daurignac E. Merikangas K. R. 2007a Fear-Potentiated Startle to Threat, and Prepulse Inhibition Among Young Adult Nonsmokers, Abstinent Smokers, and Nonabstinent Smokers. . - 48.
Grillon C. Duncko R. Covington M. F. Kopperman L. Kling M. A. 2007b Acute Stress Potentiates Anxiety in Humans. . - 49.
Guthrie R. M. Bryant R. A. 2005 Auditory startle response in firefighters before and after trauma exposure.162 283 290 - 50.
Gutiérrez-Mariscal M. de Gortari P. López-Rubalcava C. Martínez A. Joseph-Bravo P. 2008 Analysis of the anxiolytic-like effect of TRH and the response of amygdalar TRHergic neurons in anxiety.33 198 213 - 51.
Hamner M. B. Diamond B. I. 1996 Plasma dopamine and norepinephrine correlations with psychomotor retardation, anxiety, and depression in non-psychotic depressed patients: a pilot study.64 209 211 - 52.
Henderson A. S. Korten A. E. Jorm A. F. Jacomb P. A. Christensen H. Rodgers B. Tan X. Easteal S. 2000 COMT and DRD3 polymorphisms, environmental exposures, and personality traits related to common mental disorders.96 102 107 - 53.
Hineline P. N. 1978a Warmup in avoidance as a function of time since prior training.29 87 103 - 54.
Hineline P. N. 1978b Warmup in free-operant avoidance as a function of the response-shock = shock-shock interval.30 281 291 - 55.
Hirshfeld D. R. Rosenbaum J. F. Biederman J. Bolduc E. A. Faraone S. V. Snidman N. Reznick J. S. Kagan J. 1992 Stable behavioral inhibition and its association with anxiety disorder.31 103 111 - 56.
Hirshfeld-Becker D. R. Biederman J. Henin A. Faraone S. V. Davis S. Harrington K. Rosenbaum J. F. 2007 Behavioral inhibition in preschool children at risk is a specific predictor of middle childhood social anxiety: a five-year follow-up.28 225 233 - 57.
Hopper J. W. Frewen P. A. van der Kolk B. A. Lanius R. A. 2007 Neural correlates of reexperiencing, avoidance, and dissociation in PTSD: symptom dimensions and emotion dysregulation in responses to script-driven trauma imagery.20 713 725 - 58.
Jiao X. Pang K. C. Beck K. D. Minor T. R. Servatius R. J. 2011 Avoidance perseveration during extinction training in Wistar-Kyoto rats: An interaction of innate vulnerability and stressor intensity.221 98 107 - 59.
Jiao X. Pare W. P. Tejani-Butt S. 2003 Strain differences in the distribution of dopamine transporter sites in rat brain.27 913 919 - 60.
Jiao X. Pare W. P. Tejani-Butt S. M. 2006 Antidepressant drug induced alterations in binding to central dopamine transporter sites in the Wistar Kyoto rat strain.30 30 41 - 61.
Kagan J. Reznick J. S. Gibbons J. 1989 Inhibited and uninhibited types of children.60 838 845 - 62.
Kagan J. Reznick J. S. Snidman N. 1987 The physiology and psychology of behavioral inhibition in children.58 1459 1473 - 63.
Kalin N. H. 2004 Studying non-human primates: a gateway to understanding anxiety disorders. 38 Suppl1 8 13 - 64.
Kalin N. H. Shelton S. E. 2003 Nonhuman primate models to study anxiety, emotion regulation, and psychopathology.1008 189 200 - 65.
Kalin N. H. Shelton S. E. Davidson R. J. 2000 Cerebrospinal fluid corticotropin-releasing hormone levels are elevated in monkeys with patterns of brain activity associated with fearful temperament.47 579 585 - 66.
Kalin N. H. Shelton S. E. Davidson R. J. 2007 Role of the Primate Orbitofrontal Cortex in Mediating Anxious Temperament.62 1134 1139 - 67.
Kalueff A. V. Avgustinovich D. F. Kudryavtseva N. N. Murphy D. L. 2006 BDNF in anxiety and depression.312 1598 1599 - 68.
Karamustafalioglu O. K. Zohar J. Guveli M. Gal G. Bakim B. Fostick L. Karamustafalioglu N. Sasson Y. 2006 Natural Course of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: A 20-Month Prospective Study of Turkish Earthquake Survivors.67 882 889 - 69.
Kashdan T. B. Elhai J. D. Frueh B. C. 2006 Anhedonia and emotional numbing in combat veterans with PTSD.44 457 467 - 70.
Keltikangas-Jarvinen L. Kettunen J. Ravaja N. Naatanen P. 1999 Inhibited and disinhibited temperament and autonomic stress reactivity.33 185 196 - 71.
Kemppainen S. Pitkanen A. 2000 Distribution of parvalbumin, calretinin, and calbindin-D(28k) immunoreactivity in the rat amygdaloid complex and colocalization with gamma-aminobutyric acid.426 441 467 - 72.
Kendler K. S. Myers J. Prescott C. A. 2002 The etiology of phobias: an evaluation of the stress-diathesis model.59 242 248 - 73.
Kessler R. C. Chiu W. T. Demler O. Merikangas K. R. Walters E. E. 2005 Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication.62 617 627 - 74.
Lahdesmaki J. Sallinen J. Mac Donald. E. Kobilka B. K. Fagerholm V. Scheinin M. 2002 Behavioral and neurochemical characterization of alpha(2A)-adrenergic receptor knockout mice.113 289 299 - 75.
Lahmame A. Armario A. 1996 Differential responsiveness of inbred strains of rats to antidepressants in the forced swimming test: are Wistar Kyoto rats an animal model of subsensitivity to antidepressants?123 191 198 - 76.
Laitman B. M. Da Silva. J. K. Ross R. J. Tejani-Butt S. Morrison A. R. - 77.
Lanius R. A. Williamson P. C. Hopper J. Densmore M. Boksman K. Gupta M. A. Neufeld R. W. Gati J. S. Menon R. S. 2003 Recall of emotional states in posttraumatic stress disorder: an fMRI investigation.53 204 210 - 78.
Lei Y. Tejani-Butt S. M. 2010 N-methyl-d-aspartic acid receptors are altered by stress and alcohol in Wistar-Kyoto rat brain.169 125 131 - 79.
Lei Y. Yaroslavsky I. Tejani-Butt S. M. 2009 Strain differences in the distribution of N-methyl-d-aspartate and gamma ([gamma])-aminobutyric acid-A receptors in rat brain.85 794 799 - 80.
Lemos J. C. Zhang G. Walsh T. Kirby L. G. Akanwa A. Brooks-Kayal A. Beck S. G. 2011 Stress-Hyperresponsive WKY Rats Demonstrate Depressed Dorsal Raphe Neuronal Excitability and Dysregulated CRF-Mediated Responses.36 721 734 - 81.
Lind K. Jonsson M. Karlsson I. Sjogren M. Wallin A. Edman A. 2006 Depressive symptoms and white matter changes in patients with dementia.21 119 125 - 82.
Lopez-Rubalcava C. Lucki I. 2000 Strain differences in the behavioral effects of antidepressant drugs in the rat forced swimming test.22 191 199 - 83.
Lucki I. Nobler M. S. 1985 The acute effects of antidepressant drugs on the performance of conditioned avoidance behavior in rats.22 261 264 - 84.
Ma S. Morilak D. A. 2004 Induction of FOS expression by acute immobilization stress is reduced in locus coeruleus and medial amygdala of Wistar-Kyoto rats compared to Sprague-Dawley rats.124 963 972 - 85.
Maes M. Van G. A. Delmeire L. Kenis G. Bosmans E. Song C. 2002 Platelet alpha2-adrenoceptor density in humans: relationships to stress-induced anxiety, psychasthenic constitution, gender and stress-induced changes in the inflammatory response system.32 919 928 - 86.
Malkesman O. Asaf T. Shbiro L. Goldstein A. Maayan R. Weizman A. Kinor N. Okun E. Sredni B. Yadid G. Weller A. (200 Monoamines B. D. N. F. Dehydroepiandrosterone-Sulfate D. H. E. A. Childhood-An Depression. Animal Model. Study - 87.
Malkesman O. Braw Y. Zagoory-Sharon O. Golan O. Lavi-Avnon Y. Schroeder M. Overstreet D. H. Yadid G. Weller A. 2005 Reward and anxiety in genetic animal models of childhood depression.164 1 10 - 88.
Malkesman O. Maayan R. Weizman A. Weller A. 2006a Aggressive behavior and HPA axis hormones after social isolation in adult rats of two different genetic animal models for depression.175 408 414 - 89.
Malkesman O. Weller A. 2009 Two different putative genetic animal models of childhood depression--A review.88 153 169 - 90.
Malkesman O. Braw Y. Maayan R. Weizman A. Overstreet D. H. Shabat-Simon M. Kesner Y. Touati-Werner D. Yadid G. Weller A. 2006b Two Different Putative Genetic Animal Models of Childhood Depression.59 17 23 - 91.
Mann R. Bell C. 1991 Neuronal metabolism and DOPA decarboxylase immunoreactivity in terminal noradrenergic sympathetic axons of rat.39 663 668 - 92.
Martinowich K. Manji H. Lu B. 2007 New insights into BDNF function in depression and anxiety.10 1089 1093 - 93.
Mathew R. J. Ho B. T. Taylor D. L. Semchuk K. M. 1981 Catecholamine and dopamine-[beta]-hydroxylase in anxiety.25 499 504 - 94.
Mc Auley J. D. Stewart A. L. Webber E. S. Cromwell H. C. Servatius R. J. Pang K. C. 2009 Wistar-Kyoto rats as an animal model of anxiety vulnerability: Support for a hypervigilance hypothesis. Behav. Brain Res. - 95.
Mc Ewen B. S. 2005 Glucocorticoids, depression, and mood disorders: structural remodeling in the brain.54 20 23 - 96.
Meloni E. G. Davis M. 1999 Enhancement of the acoustic startle response in rats by the dopamine D1 receptor agonist SKF 82958.144 373 380 - 97.
Milad M. R. Quirk G. J. Pitman R. K. Orr S. P. Fischl B. Rauch S. L. A. Role for. the Human. Dorsal Anterior. Cingulate Cortex. in Fear. Expression - 98.
Mineka S. Zinbarg R. 1996 Conditioning and ethological models of anxiety disorders: stress-in-dynamic-context anxiety models.43 135 210 - 99.
Mineka S. Zinbarg R. 2006 A contemporary learning theory perspective on the etiology of anxiety disorders: it’s not what you thought it was.61 10 26 - 100.
Mineka S. 2004 The positive and negative consequences of worry in the aetiology of generalized anxiety disorder: A learning theory perspective. [References].24 48 - 101.
Mobbs D. Petrovic P. Marchant J. L. Hassabis D. Weiskopf N. Seymour B. Dolan R. J. Frith C. D. 2007 When fear is near: threat imminence elicits prefrontal-periaqueductal gray shifts in humans.317 1079 1083 - 102.
Myers K. M. Davis M. 2002 Behavioral and neural analysis of extinction.36 567 584 - 103.
Neumeister A. Daher R. J. Charney D. S. 2005 Anxiety disorders: noradrenergic neurotransmission.205 223 - 104.
North C. S. Pfefferbaum B. Tivis L. Kawasaki A. Reddy C. Spitznagel E. L. 2004 The course of posttraumatic stress disorder in a follow-up study of survivors of the Oklahoma City bombing.16 209 215 - 105.
Nosek K. Dennis K. Andrus B. M. Ahmadiyeh N. Baum A. E. Solberg Woods. L. C. Redei E. E. 2008 Context and strain-dependent behavioral response to stress. 4, 23. - 106.
Novick A. Yaroslavsky I. Tejani-Butt S. 2008 Strain differences in the expression of dopamine D1 receptors in Wistar-Kyoto (WKY) and Wistar rats.83 74 78 - 107.
Nutt D. J. Malizia A. L. 2004 Structural and functional brain changes in posttraumatic stress disorder. 65 Suppl1 11 17 - 108.
O’donnell M. L. Elliott P. Lau W. Creamer M. 2006b PTSD symptom trajectories: From early to chronic response. Behav. Res. Ther. - 109.
O’donnell M. L. Elliott P. Lau W. Creamer M. 2006a PTSD symptom trajectories: From early to chronic response. Behav. Res. Ther. - 110.
O’Mahony C. M. Clarke G. Gibney S. Dinan T. G. Cryan J. F. 2011 Strain differences in the neurochemical response to chronic restraint stress in the rat: relevance to depression.97 690 699 - 111.
Ohara K. Nishii R. Nakajima T. Kokai M. Morita Y. 2004 Alterations of symptoms with borderline personality disorder after fronto-temporal traumatic brain injury. A case study].106 458 466 - 112.
Okamoto K. Aoki K. 1963 Development of a strain of spontaneously hypertensive rats.27 282 293 - 113.
Ottenweller J. E. Servatius R. J. Natelson B. H. 1994 Repeated stress persistently elevates morning, but not evening, plasma corticosterone levels in male rats.55 337 340 - 114.
Overstreet D. H. Rezvani A. H. Janowsky D. S. 1990 Impaired active avoidance responding in rats selectively bred for increased cholinergic function.47 787 788 - 115.
Overstreet D. H. Rezvani A. H. Janowsky D. S. 1992 Genetic animal models of depression and ethanol preference provide support for cholinergic and serotonergic involvement in depression and alcoholism.31 919 936 - 116.
Palmer A. A. Dulawa S. C. Mottiwala A. A. Conti L. H. Geyer M. A. Printz M. P. 2000 Prepulse Startle Deficit in the Brown Norway Rat: A Potential Genetic Model.114 374 388 - 117.
Pardon M. C. Gould G. G. Garcia A. Phillips L. Cook M. C. Miller S. A. Mason P. A. Morilak D. A. 2002 Stress reactivity of the brain noradrenergic system in three rat strains differing in their neuroendocrine and behavioral responses to stress: implications for susceptibility to stress-related neuropsychiatric disorders.115 229 242 - 118.
Pardon M. C. Joubert C. Perez-Diaz F. Christen Y. Launay J. M. Cohen-Salmon C. 2000 In vivo regulation of cerebral monoamine oxidase activity in senescent controls and chronically stressed mice by long-term treatment with Ginkgo biloba extrac3t (EGb 761).113 157 168 - 119.
Pardon M. C. Ma S. Morilak D. A. 2003 Chronic cold stress sensitizes brain noradrenergic reactivity and noradrenergic facilitation of the HPA stress response in Wistar Kyoto rats.971 55 65 - 120.
Pare A. M. T. Pare W. P. Kluczynski J. 1999a Negative Affect and Voluntary Alcohol Consumption in Wistar-Kyoto (WKY) and Sprague-Dawley Rats.67 219 225 - 121.
Pare W. P. 1992a Learning behavior, escape behavior, and depression in an ulcer susceptible rat strain.27 130 141 - 122.
Pare W. P. Blair G. R. Kluczynski J. Tejani-Butt S. 1999b Gender differences in acute and chronic stress in Wistar Kyoto (WKY) rats.34 227 241 - 123.
Pare W. P. Kluczynski J. 1997 Differences in the stress response of Wistar-Kyoto (WKY) rats from different vendors.62 643 648 - 124.
Pare W. P. Redei E. 1993a Depressive behavior and stress ulcer in Wistar Kyoto rats.87 229 238 - 125.
Pare W. P. Redei E. 1993b Sex differences and stress response of WKY rats.54 1179 1185 - 126.
Pare W. P. Tejani-Butt S. M. 1996 Effect of stress on the behavior and 5-HT system in Sprague-Dawley and Wistar Kyoto rat strains.31 112 121 - 127.
Pare W. P. 1989a Behavioral despair" test predicts stress ulcer in WKY rats.46 483 487 - 128.
Pare W. P. 1989b Behavioral despair" test predicts stress ulcer in WKY rats.46 483 487 - 129.
Pare W. P. 1989c Stress ulcer susceptibility and depression in Wistar Kyoto (WKY) rats.46 993 998 - 130.
Pare W. P. 1992b The performance of WKY rats on three tests of emotional behavior.51 1051 1056 - 131.
Pare W. P. 1993 Passive-avoidance behavior in Wistar-Kyoto (WKY), Wistar, and Fischer-344 rats.54 845 852 - 132.
Pare W. P. 1994 Open field, learned helplessness, conditioned defensive burying, and forced-swim tests in WKY rats.55 433 439 - 133.
Pare W. P. 1996 Enhanced retrieval of unpleasant memories influenced by shock controllability, shock sequence, and rat strain.39 808 813 - 134.
Pare W. P. Schimmel G. T. 1986 Stress ulcer in normotensive and spontaneously hypertensive rats.36 699 705 - 135.
Pare W. P. Tejani-Butt S. Kluczynski J. 2001 The emergence test: effects of psychotropic drugs on neophobic disposition in Wistar Kyoto (WKY) and Sprague Dawley Rats.25 1615 1628 - 136.
Pearson K. A. Stephen A. Beck S. G. Valentino R. J. 2006 Identifying genes in monoamine nuclei that may determine stress vulnerability depressive behavior in-Kyoto Wistar rats31 2449 2461 - 137.
Perez-Edgar K. Roberson-Nay R. Hardin M. G. Poeth K. Guyer A. E. Nelson E. E. Mc Clure E. B. Henderson H. A. Fox N. A. Pine D. S. Ernst M. 2007 Attention alters neural responses to evocative faces in behaviorally inhibited adolescents.35 1538 1546 - 138.
Redei E. Pare W. P. Aird F. Kluczynski J. 1994 Strain differences in hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal activity and stress ulcer. 266, R353 R360. - 139.
Rittenhouse P. A. Lopez-Rubalcava C. Stanwood G. D. Lucki I. 2002 Amplified behavioral and endocrine responses to forced swim stress in the Wistar-Kyoto rat.27 303 318 - 140.
Rosenbaum J. F. Biederman J. Bolduc-Murphy E. A. Faraone S. V. Chaloff J. Hirshfeld D. R. Kagan J. 1993 Behavioral inhibition in childhood: a risk factor for anxiety disorders.1 2 16 - 141.
Rosenbaum J. F. Biederman J. Hirshfeld D. R. Bolduc E. A. Faraone S. V. Kagan J. Snidman N. Reznick J. S. 1991 Further evidence of an association between behavioral inhibition and anxiety disorders: results from a family study of children from a non-clinical sample.25 49 65 - 142.
Rosenkranz J. A. Grace A. A. 2001 Dopamine attenuates prefrontal cortical suppression of sensory inputs to the basolateral amygdala of rats.21 4090 4103 - 143.
Samanez-Larkin G. R. Hollon N. G. Carstensen L. L. Knutson B. 2008 Individual differences in insular sensitivity during loss anticipation predict avoidance learning.19 320 323 - 144.
Sands S. A. Strong R. Corbitt J. Morilak D. A. 2000 Effects of acute restraint stress on tyrosine hydroxylase mRNA expression in locus coeruleus of Wistar and Wistar-Kyoto rats.75 1 7 - 145.
Scholl J. L. Renner K. J. Forster G. L. Tejani-Butt S. 2010 Central monoamine levels differ between rat strains used in studies of depressive behavior.1355 41 51 - 146.
Schramm N. L. Mc Donald M. P. Limbird L. E. 2001 The {alpha}2A-Adrenergic Receptor Plays a Protective Role in Mouse Behavioral Models of Depression and Anxiety.21 4875 4882 - 147.
Schwartz, C. E., Wright, C. I., Shin, L. M., Kagan, J., and Rauch, S. L. (2003 ) Inhibited and uninhibited infants "grown up": adult amygdalar response to novelty. Science 300, 1952-1953. - 148.
Servatius R. J. Jiao X. Beck K. D. Pang K. C. H. Minor T. R. 2008 Rapid avoidance acquisition in Wistar-Kyoto rats.192 191 197 - 149.
Servatius R. J. Ottenweller J. E. Beldowicz D. Guo W. Zhu G. Natelson B. H. 1998 Persistently exaggerated startle responses in rats treated with pyridostigmine bromide.287 1020 1028 - 150.
Silver R. C. Holman E. A. Mc Intosh D. N. Poulin M. Gil-Rivas V. 2002 Nationwide longitudinal study of psychological responses to September 11.288 1235 1244 - 151.
Simmons A. Strigo I. Matthews S. C. Paulus M. P. Stein M. B. 2006 Anticipation of aversive visual stimuli is associated with increased insula activation in anxiety-prone subjects.60 402 409 - 152.
Smoller J. W. Rosenbaum J. F. Biederman J. Kennedy J. Dai D. Racette S. R. Laird N. M. Kagan J. Snidman N. Hirshfeld-Becker D. Tsuang M. T. Sklar P. B. Slaugenhaupt S. A. 2003 Association of a genetic marker at the corticotropin-releasing hormone locus with behavioral inhibition.54 1376 1381 - 153.
Smoller J. W. Yamaki L. H. Fagerness J. A. Biederman J. Racette S. Laird N. M. Kagan J. Snidman N. Faraone S. V. Hirshfeld-Becker D. Tsuang M. T. Slaugenhaupt S. A. Rosenbaum J. F. Sklar P. B. 2005 The corticotropin-releasing hormone gene and behavioral inhibition in children at risk for panic disorder.57 1485 1492 - 154.
Solberg L. C. Baum A. E. Ahmadiyeh N. Shimomura K. Li R. Turek F. W. Churchill G. A. Takahashi J. S. Redei E. E. 2004 Sex- and lineage-specific inheritance of depression-like behavior in the rat.15 648 662 - 155.
Solberg L. C. Olson S. L. Turek F. W. Redei E. 2001 Altered hormone levels and circadian rhythm of activity in the WKY rat, a putative animal model of depression. 281, R786 R794. - 156.
Somers J. M. Goldner E. M. Waraich P. Hsu L. 2006 Prevalence and incidence studies of anxiety disorders: a systematic review of the literature.51 100 113 - 157.
Straube T. Schmidt S. Weiss T. Mentzel H. J. Miltner W. H. 2009 Dynamic activation of the anterior cingulate cortex during anticipatory anxiety.44 975 981 - 158.
Sullivan G. M. Coplan J. D. Kent J. M. Gorman J. M. 1999 The noradrenergic system in pathological anxiety: a focus on panic with relevance to generalized anxiety and phobias.46 1205 1218 - 159.
Suslow T. Kugel H. Rauch A. V. Dannlowski U. Bauer J. Konrad C. Arolt V. Heindel W. Ohrmann P. 2009 Attachment avoidance modulates neural response to masked facial emotion. Hum. Brain Mapp. - 160.
Taylor D. P. Riblet L. A. Stanton H. C. Eison A. S. Eison M. S. Temple D. L. 1982 Dopamine and antianxiety activity.17 25 35 - 161.
Tejani-Butt S. M. Pare W. P. Yang J. 1994 Effect of repeated novel stressors on depressive behavior and brain norepinephrine receptor system in Sprague-Dawley and Wistar Kyoto (WKY) rats.649 27 35 - 162.
Tejani-Butt S. Kluczynski J. Pare W. P. 2003 Strain-dependent modification of behavior following antidepressant treatment.27 7 14 - 163.
Tyrka A. R. Mello A. F. Mello M. F. Gagne G. G. Grover K. E. Anderson G. M. Price L. H. Carpenter L. L. 2006 Temperament and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis function in healthy adults.31 1036 1045 - 164.
Tyrka A. R. Wier L. M. Price L. H. Rikhye K. Ross N. S. Anderson G. M. Wilkinson C. W. Carpenter L. L. 2008 Cortisol and ACTH responses to the Dex/CRH test: influence of temperament.53 518 525 - 165.
Vachette C. Bourde O. Gillon J. Y. Pujol J. F. Renaud B. 1993 Decrease in the reactivity of locus coeruleus neurons to hypotension after an increase in their tyrosine hydroxylase content: a subregional in vivo voltammetry study in the rat.5 261 268 - 166.
Vaswani M. Linda F. K. Ramesh S. 2003 Role of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors in psychiatric disorders: a comprehensive review.27 85 102 - 167.
Walker J. R. Su A. I. Self D. W. Hogenesch J. B. Lapp H. Maier R. Hoyer D. Bilbe G. 2004 Applications of a rat multiple tissue gene expression data set.14 742 749 - 168.
Will C. C. Aird F. Redei E. E. 2003 Selectively bred Wistar-Kyoto rats: an animal model of depression and hyper-responsiveness to antidepressants.8 925 932 - 169.
Yaroslavsky I. Colletti M. Jiao X. Tejani-Butt S. 2006 Strain differences in the distribution of dopamine (DA-2 and DA-3) receptor sites in rat brain.79 772 776 - 170.
Zafar H. M. Pare W. P. Tejani-Butt S. M. 1997 Effect of acute or repeated stress on behavior and brain norepinephrine system in Wistar-Kyoto (WKY) rats.44 289 295 - 171.
Zinbarg R. E. Barlow D. H. 1996 Structure of anxiety and the anxiety disorders: a hierarchical model.105 181 193