Open access peer-reviewed chapter

Understanding Neural Mechanisms of Action Observation for Improving Human Motor Skill Acquisition

Written By

Hideki Nakano and Takayuki Kodama

Submitted: 24 September 2016 Reviewed: 18 April 2017 Published: 29 November 2017

DOI: 10.5772/intechopen.69266

From the Edited Volume

Electroencephalography

Edited by Phakkharawat Sittiprapaporn

Chapter metrics overview

1,400 Chapter Downloads

View Full Metrics

Abstract

Action observation is a useful approach for improving human motor skill acquisition. This process involves the mirror neuron system that consists of the ventral premotor area, inferior parietal lobule, and superior temporal sulcus. The interaction between these areas produces the effect of action observation. This chapter presents neurophysiological and brain imaging studies of action observation, and their application to human motor learning. For action observation, the mirror system appears to map the intention in the ventral premotor area and the goal in the inferior parietal lobule. These features of action representation may be useful for refining conditions of practice, based on the mirror system, for acquiring new motor skills.

Keywords

  • action observation
  • electroencephalography
  • mirror neuron system
  • motor learning

1. Introduction

Previous neurophysiological and brain imaging studies have revealed that neural activity associated with observation of another person’s movement was elicited in the motor‐related cortical areas [13]. The motor‐related cortical areas that get active during such motion perception, constitute the mirror neuron system. Characteristically, this system is activated not only when a person performs a goal‐oriented movement by himself/herself, but also when the person observes the same movement performed by others (Figure 1) [4]. Action observation automatically creates a similar simulation of movement in the brain of the observer [5, 6]. In other words, action observation induces functional reorganization of the brain, and facilitates motor learning via the mirror neuron system (Figure 2) [7, 8]. Thus, observation of another person’s actions and behavior alters the neuronal activity of the observer. This chapter discusses neurophysiological and brain imaging studies of action observation, and their application to human motor learning.

Figure 1.

Mirror neurons in monkeys [4]. Top indicates the neural activity in area F5 when the monkey grasps food. Bottom indicates the neural activity in are F5 when the monkey observes the human grasping food.

Figure 2.

Mirror neuron system in humans [7]. Purple areas (PMD and SPL) are involved in reaching movements. Yellow areas (IFG, PMV, IPL and IPS) are involved in transitive distal movements. Blue areas (STS) are involved in observation of upper-limb movements. Green areas (A) are involved in intransitive movements. Orange areas (B) are involved in tool use. PMD indicates dorsal premotor cortex; SPL, superior parietal lobule; IFG, inferior frontal gyrus; PMV, ventral premotor cortex; IPL, inferior parietal lobule; IPS, intraparietal sulcus; STS, superior temporal sulcus.

Advertisement

2. Neural mechanism of action observation

The mirror neuron system is concerned with the neural mechanism of action observation. The mirror neuron system is a neural system that consists of the ventral premotor area, inferior parietal lobule, and superior temporal sulcus, and the interaction between these areas produces the effect of action observation (Figure 3) [9]. The mirror neuron system converts sensory information obtained through observation to a specific motion pattern, which is an objective [10]. Thus, decryption of the action and behavior of other individuals are facilitated. Additionally, the encoding of the meaning and objective of an action facilitates understanding of the purpose of the action performed by others, internally, without requiring higher cognitive processes, such as reasoning. The understanding of other’s intentions [11], empathy [12], and theory of mind [13] are known functional characteristics of the mirror neuron system, and dysfunction of the mirror neuron system is associated with autism [14].

Figure 3.

ALE meta‐analysis of action observation in the human brain [9]. Significant meta‐analysis results for action observation, summarized over all effectors. All results are displayed on the left and right lateral surface view of the MNI single subject template.

Fadiga et al. investigated the mirror neuron system using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) [5]. They measured the motor evoked potential (MEP), which is the index of excitatory change of the corticospinal tract, from finger muscles, by stimulating the motor area by means of TMS, while the subject was observing the action. The results showed that the MEP amplitude of the finger muscle involved in the observed action, increased significantly, and these phenomena were not observed in the MEP amplitude of another finger muscle that was not concerned with the observed action. Thus, the peripheral motor system also prepares for performing the observed action, and the temporal consistency between the muscle group concerned with the action that is an objective and the muscle activation patterns indicate that the mirror neuron system couples action execution and observation [15]. The same motor representations are activated during both action execution and observation; this indicates the possibility that the reorganization of the motor system network that is induced by action observation and that which is induced by actual physical practice involve the same mechanisms [16].

Many previous studies have investigated the excitatory changes in the corticospinal tract during a single action observation, and some of these have examined the excitatory change in the corticospinal tract during repetitive action observation. Stefan et al. revealed that repetitive action observation changes the motor representation in the cerebral cortex as it does with physical practice [17]. Moreover, they disclosed that these changes of the motor representation in the cerebral cortex, were induced by physical practice along with brief action observation. Furthermore, it has been reported that repetitive action observation increased the excitability of the corticospinal tract, and that there was a significant positive correlation between the increased excitability of the corticospinal tract and the change in motion patterns [16].

Watanabe et al. examined the effect of observation viewpoint on brain activity and performance [18]. There are two observation perspectives. One involves observing another person’s action from the same side as that of the subject’s perspective, termed the “first‐person perspective” and the other involves observing the other person’s action on the opposite side as that of the subject’s perspective, termed the “third‐person perspective” (Figure 4). Their study investigated the difference between reaction time and brain activity from first‐person and third‐person perspectives, while the subject observed the action and imitated the action of another, using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). They showed that the motor‐related areas involve the mirror neuron system, including the ventral premotor area, supramarginal gyrus, and supplementary motor area, in the first‐person perspective significantly more than in the third‐person perspective. Thus, action observation from the first‐person perspective activates the mirror neuron system advantageously, and facilitates the intracerebral simulation of the action that is the objective, and enhances motor learning. This information should be applied to motor learning, considering the various conditions of action observation.

Figure 4.

First‐person perspective (left) and third‐person perspective (right).

Advertisement

3. Mirror neuron system and EEG studies

Many studies have investigated the mirror neuron system using electroencephalography (EEG) [19]. A specific EEG rhythm, called the mu rhythm (8–13 Hz), is observed in the human sensorimotor cortex [20]. The characteristics of the mu rhythm are blocked during actual movement [20] as well as observation of action [21] and motor imagery [22]. Therefore, numerous EEG studies have used the mu rhythm as an electrophysiological marker of the mirror neuron system in humans, after Altschuler et al. [23] first investigated this possibility. Those studies found that the mu rhythm represents the activity of the mirror mechanism in humans. An fMRI study also showed a significant correlation between mu rhythm desynchronization (Figure 5) [24] and BOLD activity in typical mirror neuron system regions.

Figure 5.

Mu rhythm desynchronization [24]. Simulation of mu rhythm desynchronization in the 8–13 Hz frequency band. There is a decrease in amplitude in the electroencephalogram from baseline during action observation or execution.

Many studies have examined the reactivity of the mu rhythm during action observation. Avanzini et al. [25] investigated the dynamics of sensorimotor cortical oscillations during the observation of hand movements using EEG (Figure 6). A desynchronization of alpha and beta rhythms was observed in the central and parietal regions. Notably, there was a large post‐stimulus power rebound present in all bands. Furthermore, the velocity profile of the observed movement and beta band modulation correlated, indicating a direct matching of the stimulus parameter to motor activity.

Figure 6.

EEG rhythms during action observation [25]. The graph shows the EEG power time‐course for each frequency band: alpha band (8–13 Hz) in green, lower beta (13–18 Hz) in red, and upper beta (18–25 Hz) in cyan.

Advertisement

4. Action observation and motor learning

Schmidt defined that motor learning is a “process of acquiring the capability for producing skilled actions” [26], and “the changes associated with practice and experiences, in an internal process that determines a person’s capability for producing motor skill” [27]. Moreover, Guthrie stated that motor learning is a “relatively permanent change, resulting from practice or a novel experience, in the capability for responding” [28]. In other words, motor learning is the capability acquired from practice and experience, and the change is relatively permanent. How can then action observation contribute to motor learning?

Motor imagery is similar to action observation. It is the mental simulation of movement without physical movement of body parts [29]. Action observation and motor imagery have been shown to share the same neural basis as that used for the execution of the actual physical movement [30]. However, the process of action observation and motor imagery are different. Action observation is a bottom‐up process (process from perception), while motor imagery is a top‐down process (process from memory). Nevertheless, these processes are not clearly divided, and a feed‐forward model was constructed by complementing these processes [31]. In addition, action observation has an effect of promoting motor imagery [32].

In the early stages of new complex motor learning, action observation is superior to motor imagery as a strategy for motor learning, as revealed by behavioral [33] and EEG data (Figure 7) [34]. Motor imagery is influenced by the environment and personal imaging ability and requires mental effort. In contrast, action observation is easier to apply than motor imagery, despite targeting activation of the same neural network as motor imagery [35]. We have also reported that the left sensorimotor and parietal areas of the high‐motor learning group showed a greater decrease in the alpha‐2 and beta‐2 rhythms than those of the low‐motor learning group during observation and execution. These results suggested that the decreases in the alpha‐2 and beta‐2 rhythms in these areas during observation and execution are associated with motor skill improvement (Figure 8) [36]. Accordingly, action observation may be an effective tool as an intervention method during the early stage of motor learning.

Figure 7.

Cortical activation patterns in the action observation (AO), motor imagery (MI), and control (C) groups [34]. Average TRPow (task‐related power) changes with respect to resting baseline, within the two frequency bands showing significant group effects, were interpolated and projected onto an average brain cortical surface.

Figure 8.

Changes in EEG activity durings observation and execution of a motor learning task [36]. Alpha-2 (A) and beta-2 (B) rhythms in the left sensorimotor and parietal areas during action execution from the 1st trial to the 5th trial were significantly decreased in the high‐motor learning group compared with in the low‐motor learning group.

Advertisement

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by JSPS KAKENHI Grant Number JP26750208 and ASTEM RI / KYOTO.

References

  1. 1. Rizzolatti G, Fadiga L, Gallese V, Fogassi L. Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research. 1996 Mar;3(2):131-41
  2. 2. Gallese V, Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Rizzolatti G. Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain. 1996 Apr;119(Pt 2):593-609
  3. 3. Hari R, Forss N, Avikainen S, Kirveskari E, Salenius S, Rizzolatti G. Activation of human primary motor cortex during action observation: A neuromagnetic study. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States. 1998 Dec 8;95(25):15061-15065
  4. 4. Iacoboni M, Dapretto M. The mirror neuron system and the consequences of its dysfunction. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2006 Dec;7(12):942-51
  5. 5. Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Pavesi G, Rizzolatti G. Motor facilitation during action observation: A magnetic stimulation study. The Journal of Neurophysiology. 1995 Jun;73(6):2608-11
  6. 6. Rizzolatti G, Craighero L. The mirror‐neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2004;27:169-92
  7. 7. Cattaneo L, Rizzolatti G. The mirror neuron system. Archives of Neurology. 2009 May;66(5):557-60
  8. 8. Buccino G, Vogt S, Ritzl A, Fink GR, Zilles K, Freund HJ, Rizzolatti G. Neural circuits underlying imitation learning of hand actions: An event‐related fMRI study. Neuron. 2004 Apr 22;42(2):323-34
  9. 9. Caspers S, Zilles K, Laird AR, Eickhoff SB. ALE meta‐analysis of action observation and imitation in the human brain. Neuroimage. 2010 Apr 15;50(3):1148-67
  10. 10. Rizzolatti G, Fogassi L, Gallese V. Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2001 Sep;2(9):661-70
  11. 11. Fogassi L, Ferrari PF, Gesierich B, Rozzi S, Chersi F, Rizzolatti G. Parietal lobe: From action organization to intention understanding. Science. 2005 Apr 29;308(5722):662-7
  12. 12. Singer T, Seymour B, O’Doherty J, Kaube H, Dolan RJ, Frith CD. Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science. 2004 Feb 20;303(5661):1157-62
  13. 13. Gallese V, Goldman A. Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind‐reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 1998 Dec 1;2(12):493-501
  14. 14. Oberman LM, Hubbard EM, McCleery JP, Altschuler EL, Ramachandran VS, Pineda JA. EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research. 2005 Jul;24(2):190-8
  15. 15. Small SL, Buccino G, Solodkin A. The mirror neuron system and treatment of stroke. Developmental Psychobiology. 2012 Apr;54(3):293-310
  16. 16. Ray M, Dewey D, Kooistra L, Welsh TN. The relationship between the motor system activation during action observation and adaptation in the motor system following repeated action observation. Human Movement Science. 2013 Jun;32(3):400-11
  17. 17. Stefan K, Cohen LG, Duque J, Mazzocchio R, Celnik P, Sawaki L, Ungerleider L, Classen J. Formation of a motor memory by action observation. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2005 Oct 12;25(41):9339-46
  18. 18. Watanabe R, Higuchi T, Kikuchi Y. Imitation behavior is sensitive to visual perspective of the model: An fMRI study. Experimental Brain Research. 2013 Jul;228(2):161-71
  19. 19. Rizzolatti G, Cattaneo L, Fabbri‐Destro M, Rozzi S. Cortical mechanisms underlying the organization of goal‐directed actions and mirror neuron‐based action understanding. Physiological Reviews. 2014 Apr;94(2):655-706
  20. 20. Gastaut H, Terzian H, Gastaut Y. Etude d’une activite electroencephalographique meconnue: le rythme rolandique en arceau. Mars Med. 1952;89(6):296-310
  21. 21. Gastaut HJ, Bert J. EEG changes during cinematographic presentation; moving picture activation of the EEG. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 1954 Aug;6(3):433-44
  22. 22. Pfurtscheller G, Neuper C. Motor imagery activates primary sensorimotor area in humans. Neuroscience Letters. 1997 Dec 19;239(2-3):65-8
  23. 23. Altschuler EL, Vankov A, Wang V, Ramachandran VS, Pineda JA. Person see, person do: Human cortical electrophysiological correlates of monkey see monkey do cells. Poster Session Presented at the 27th Annual Meeting of the Society for Neuroscience; New Orleans: LA. 1997
  24. 24. Fox NA, Bakermans‐Kranenburg MJ, Yoo KH, Bowman LC, Cannon EN, Vanderwert RE, Ferrari PF, van IJzendoorn MH. Assessing human mirror activity with EEG mu rhythm: A meta‐analysis. The Psychological Bulletin. 2016 Mar;142(3):291-313
  25. 25. Avanzini P, Fabbri‐Destro M, Dalla Volta R, Daprati E, Rizzolatti G, Cantalupo G. The dynamics of sensorimotor cortical oscillations during the observation of hand movements: an EEG study. PLoS One. 2012;7(5):e37534
  26. 26. Schmidt RA. Motor Control and Learning: A Behavioral Emphasis. 2nd ed. Champaign: Human Kinetics Publisher; 1988. p. 346
  27. 27. Schmidt RA. Motor Learning & Performance: From Principles to Practice. Champaign: Human Kinetics Publisher; 1991. p. 51
  28. 28. Guthrie ER. The Psychology of Learning. New York: Harper & Row; 1952
  29. 29. Decety J, Perani D, Jeannerod M, Bettinardi V, Tadary B, Woods R, Mazziotta JC, Fazio F. Mapping motor representations with positron emission tomography. Nature. 1994 Oct 13;371(6498):600-2
  30. 30. Grèzes J, Decety J. Functional anatomy of execution, mental simulation, observation, and verb generation of actions: a meta‐analysis. Human Brain Mapping. 2001 Jan;12(1):1-19
  31. 31. Holmes P, Calmels C. A neuroscientific review of imagery and observation use in sport. Journal of Motor Behavior. 2008 Sep;40(5):433-45
  32. 32. Conson M, Sarà M, Pistoia F, Trojano L. Action observation improves motor imagery: Specific interactions between simulative processes. Experimental Brain Research. 2009 Oct;199(1):71-81
  33. 33. Gatti R, Tettamanti A, Gough PM, Riboldi E, Marinoni L, Buccino G. Action observation versus motor imagery in learning a complex motor task: A short review of literature and a kinematics study. Neuroscience Letters. 2013 Apr 12;540:37-42
  34. 34. Gonzalez‐Rosa JJ, Natali F, Tettamanti A, Cursi M, Velikova S, Comi G, Gatti R, Leocani L. Action observation and motor imagery in performance of complex movements: Evidence from EEG and kinematics analysis. Behavioural Brain Research. 2015 Mar 15;281:290-300
  35. 35. Buccino G. Action observation treatment: A novel tool in neuro rehabilitation. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 2014 Apr 28;369(1644):20130185
  36. 36. Nakano H, Osumi M, Ueta K, Kodama T, Morioka S. Changes in electroencephalographic activity during observation, preparation, and execution of a motor learning task. International Journal of Neuroscience. 2013 Dec;123(12):866-75

Written By

Hideki Nakano and Takayuki Kodama

Submitted: 24 September 2016 Reviewed: 18 April 2017 Published: 29 November 2017