Open access

Beyond the Black-and-White of Autism: How Cognitive Performance Varies with Context

Written By

Joseph L. Amaral, Susan Collins, Kevin T. Bohache and Heidi Kloos

Submitted: 04 April 2012 Published: 14 November 2012

DOI: 10.5772/53937

From the Edited Volume

Current Topics in Children's Learning and Cognition

Edited by Heidi Kloos, Bradley J. Morris and Joseph L. Amaral

Chapter metrics overview

4,747 Chapter Downloads

View Full Metrics

1. Introduction

A child’s cognitive performance can vary considerably from one task context to the next, even when only small details of the task are changed. Such context dependence has given rise to a new way of looking at the underlying processes of children’s thinking (e.g. Smith, Thelen, Titzer, &McLin, 1999; Spencer, Thomas, & McClelland, 2009). Rather than attributing performance to a particular competence (or a lack thereof), performance is attributed to a synergy between the actor and environment, highly sensitive to even seemingly irrelevant details of the task context. Though not complete, this view makes it possible to map out how changes in the context could be harnessed to bring about changes in a child’sbehavior. The current chapter looks at whether the same view can be applied to autism spectrum disorder (ASD). A first step in this direction is to explore the extent to which cognitive performance in ASD is affected by apparently irrelevant variations of the task context.

Autism spectrum disorder comprises of a cluster of disorders that include Autistic Disorder (also known as “classic” autism), Asperger’s Syndrome, and Pervasive Developmental Disorder-Not Otherwise Specified (DSM-IV-TR; American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Though there are important differences between these sub-groups, they share a common set of general symptoms that arise early in a child’s life: ASD is characterized by pronounced social difficulties and communication impairment, along with restricted, repetitive behaviors or interests (APA, 2000). In particular, children often demonstrate atypical eye contact, a lack of verbal speech or atypical language use, odd mannerisms such as arm flapping, and narrow, obsessive interests (e.g.: an encyclopedic knowledge of former U.S. Secretaries of the Interior; Klinger, Dawson, & Renner, 2003).

Importantly, despite extensive research into ASD, no causal factors have been isolated so far. For example, even though the patters of neurological activity show numerous differences between typical development and ASD, no single difference appears to capture the disorder (for a review see Fein, 2011). And even though ASD appears to have a strong genetic component, genome variations appear diffuse (e.g., Devlin & Scherer, 2012). This raises the possibility that the disorder is not reducible to a static causal factor that can differentiate between typical development and ASD. Instead, the disorder might be the result of complex interdependence among multiple factors that change each other’s effect as they interact over time. Take for example, the language impairments documented in ASD. Rather than being attributed to a stable factor (language-specific, neurological, genetic, or otherwise), these behaviors might have their origin in virtually undetectably minimal discrepancies in how the perceptual system combines information into higher-order patterns. The discrepancy from typical development might be minimal at first, but then get amplified by a variety of child-internal, environmental, and social factors (e.g., a difficulty detecting higher-order patterns, a low tolerance for over-stimulation, the hierarchical order inherent in a language, a disrupted communication synchrony between child and caregiver, etc). The coming-together of environmental factors further intensify the initially minimal difference in perceptual processes – which then in turn amplify environmental and social factors. In other words, what may start out as a barely noticeable difference in how information is integrated might enter a cycle of forces that amplify each other’s effect over time, an interdependence that heralds a major departure from typical development.

The view that ASD behavior is the result of interdependent factors that amplify each other’s effects over time is a stark departure from the view that overt behavior is reducible to a stable factor that marks autism. And while there is no conclusive evidence to support the former view, there is nevertheless strong support in the developmental literature of ASD. First, it is difficult to predict the developmental trajectories of individual children (for a review, see Seltzer, Shattuck, Abbeduto& Greenberg, 2004). For example, while overall language abilities can improve over time (Sigman& McGovern, 2005), various atypicalities sometimes remain, including echolalia or fixations on various topics of interest (Lord, Rici, & Pickles, 2004). Some children may even exhibit an increase in general symptom severity over time (e.g., Nordin&Gillberg, 1998).

Second, developmental patterns tracked over time sometimes show a non-linear trajectory. For example, differences in social behavior (e.g. eye contact, visual tracking, visual disengagement, imitation, social interest, and sensory-motor behaviors), apparent at 12 months of age, are missing in younger children (Zwaigenbaum et al., 2005; Bryson, Brian, Roberts, Szatmari, Rombough, & McDermott 2007). And while 6-month-old infants with a high risk of autism show less frontal Gamma power than low-risk children, this difference is negligible when infants are 24 months old (Keehn, Luyster, Vogel-Farley, Tager-Flusberg& Nelson, 2012). Explaining such non-linear trajectories under the reductionist viewpoint would require an additional assumption, namely that the isolated causal factor comes online at a certain point in time. These trajectories imply instead that the disorder has to be attributed to complex interactions among factors that change in nature over time.

The interdependence view on ASD has strong implications for how to go about studying the source of this disorder and its treatment. Rather than looking for black-and-white differences between ASD and typical development, and interdependence view advocates the study of trajectories, the stability of trajectories, and how stable cycles can be perturbed. Furthermore, an interdependence view implies that even small changes in the context can potentially have a large effect on behavioral outcomes. In the remainder of the chapter, we review performance variability of already published research to describe such context dependence in cognitive tasks. As such, this review differs from already existing reviews (e.g., Klinger, Dawson, & Renner, 2003; Rajendran & Mitchell, 2007) in one crucial way. Rather than emphasizing consistencies in findings to promote the idea of a stable difference in ASD (and discussing conflicting findings to undermine one over another reductionist ASD theory), our goal is to highlight context effects detected in ASD research.

The chapter is organized as follows: we will first highlight some findings in pattern perception, a large research field centered on the idea of the so-called weak central coherence (cf., Frith, 1989). We will then turn to findings related to learning, focusing specifically on learning of higher-order patterns and statistical information in sequences. Next, we will discuss ASD research on executive functioning, the child’s ability to control their actions to achieve a certain outcome. Finally, we will turn to research on social reasoning, discussing findings in joint attention and theory of mind.

Advertisement

2. Perception

Perceiving meaningful configurations in the array of ever-changing information (visual or otherwise) requires the mind to combine separate bursts of sensation into an organized unit of perception. The mind has to detect or impose coherence (cf., Thagard, 1989) For example, in order to perceive a painting, the mind has to ignore the individual pixels of color and detect the higher-order organization of objects and scenes. The possible patterns of organization are nested hierarchically, ranging from a very local organization (e.g., an individual object in the painting) to a more global organization (e.g., the theme of the painting). The ability to detect patterns of organization at various levels of abstraction is commonly studied under the framework of local versus global perception, with the central question pertaining to the degree to which local and global perception interfere with one another (cf. e.g., Baylis& Driver, 1993; Herrmann & Bosch, 2001; Humphreys, Olson, Romani, & Riddoch, 1996; Kahneman & Henik, 1981; Kahneman &Treisman, 1984; Kimchi, 1992; Kramer & Jacobson, 1991; Maurer, Le Grand, & Mondloch, 2002; Moore & Egeth, 1997; Pellicano& Rhodes, 2003).

An essential difference between ASD and typical development is the degree to which the perception of global order interferes with the perception of local order. Rather than exhibiting a bias towards coherence, perception of individuals with ASD is typically characterized by what is known as weak central coherence. Best example of this difference was established with the classical Navon task, a task in which stimuli consist of many small letters configured in the arrangement of a large letter (cf., Navon, 1977). In typical development, results show a distinct interference of large letters on the perception of small letters, both in children (OzonoffS trayer, McMahon &Filloux 1994; Plaisted, Swettenham, and Rees, 1999) and in adults (Fagot & Deruelle, 1997; Navon, 1977). In particular, when participants are asked to focus on small letters, reaction time is longer for trials in which large and small letters differ than on trials in which large and small letters match. This global interference is non-detectable in participants with ASD: They perform equally fastin both letter-mismatch trials and letter-match trials – and that with high accuracy (e.g. Mottron, Burack, Stauder, &Robaey, 1999; Plaisted et al., 1999).

Another example of weak central coherences in ASD comes from face-perception tasks. The identity of a face is defined not only by its individual parts (e.g., nose, eyes, mouth), but also by the holistic configuration of these parts, something that appears to be disrupted when faces are presented upside down. For typically developing children, recognition accuracy decreases when faces are presented upside down, compared to trials in which faces are presented upright (Mondloch, Le Grand, & Maurer, 2002). In contrast, children with ASD do not perform differently as a function of face orientation (Langdell, 1978; Tantam, Monaghan, Nicholson, & Stirling, 1989). Along the same lines, participants with ASD could classify faces better when local rather than global features were exaggerated (through the use of a high-pass vs. low-pass filter; Deruelle, Rondan, Salle-Collemiche, Bastard-Rosset, & Da Fonséca, 2008). The inverse pattern of results was obtained for typically developing children.

A final example of preferential local focus comes from research involving auditory perception (Foxton Stewart, Barnard, Rodgers, Young, O‘Brien, & Griffiths 2003). Stimuli were 5-tone sequences that varied in specific tones, pattern of switch in pitch direction (e.g., a down sequence was followed by up sequence), and timing of the switch. In the crucial task (a global-interference condition), participants had to focus on only one of these features, ignoring changes in the other features. In particular, they had to decide on whether two sequences match in the patterns of switch, ignoring differences in specific tones or differences in timing. Result show superior performance for participants with ASD than matched controls. Vice versa, when sequences differed only in specific tones (the patterns and timing of the sequences being identical), ASD performance matched that of control participants (see also Mottron, Peretz& Menard, 2000). Further evidence for enhanced local processing of auditory information comes from the finding that individuals with ASD can label isolated tones better than TD controls and are more likely to have perfect pitch, meaning that they can replicate or identify individual musical tones without assistance (Bonnel, Mottron, Peretz, Trudel, Gallun, &Bonnel 2003; Heaton, Hermelin, &Pring, 1998).

In broad strokes, while typical development is characterized by a bias towards perceiving higher-order Gestalts over perceiving an isolated detail, this bias is thought to be missing or at least less prevalent in ASD (for reviews, see Happé, 2000; Happé& Booth, 2008; Happé& Frith, 2006). However, even though research generally supports the idea of a weak bias towards higher-order Gestalt in ASD, there are some interesting exceptions. For example, when individuals with ASD are told to look at relevant information in face processing tasks, they perform in a similar way to typically developing children (Lopez, Donnelly, Hadwin, &Leekam,2004). The task was to determine which of two test items matched with a target face. In configuration trials, the test items were faces, one of which always matched the target, while the other one differed in a single feature. And on feature trials, the test items were individual features (e.g., eyes), one of which match the respective feature of the target. Critically, participants were sometimes provided with a cue indicating on which feature to focus. For example, they were told “look at the eyes.” In cued configuration trials, the cue focused attention to the mismatched feature. On feature trials, it focused attention to the matching feature. The typically developing group demonstrated superior performance on configuration trials, compared to feature trials, regardless of cueing. The ASD group, in contrast, demonstrated a configural advantage only on cued trials, but not in un-cued trials.

A similar effect of instruction was found when the task was to read sentences that contained homographs (words that have the same spelling are a pronounced differently, depending on the context of a sentence). As one would predict from a weak-central-coherence assumption, individuals with ASD are less likely than typically developing individuals to use the context of a sentence to disambiguate homographs (Frith &Snowling, 1983; Happé, 1997; Jolliffe& Baron-Cohen, 1999; Lopez &Leekam, 2003). For example, when asked to read aloud a sentence containing the homograph “tear”, participants with ASD are less likely to take the sentence context into account when deciding on how to pronounce the word. However, this pattern of performance changes dramatically when attention is explicitly directed to the homographs (Snowling& Frith, 1986). That is, when explicitly told to look for homographs, their use of proper pronunciation approaches that of typically developing individuals.

Variations in instruction also affect ASD performance on tasks involving optical illusions, another area that attests to Gestalt interference in typical development. Typically, the task is to focus on a local piece of information and ignore the embedding context. For example, in the Muller-Lyer illusion, the task is to compare the length of the two lines, ignoring the arrows on each end of the lines. Findings show that typically developing participants are strongly affected by the embedded context, succumbing to the illusion to the expense of focusing on the local elements (for a review, see Changizi, Hsieh, Nijhawan, Kanai, &Shimojo, 2008). While individuals with ASD are far less affected by such visual configurations (e.g., Brosnan Scott, Fox, &Pye, 2004), important context effects are apparent. For example, participants with ASD were found to be more susceptible to Muller-Lyer illusions when asked, “which line looks longer,” versus “which line is longer” (Scott, Brosnan, & Wheelwright, in preparation, as cited in Happé& Frith, 2006). It appears that individuals with ASD can see both that the lines are equally long, and that the lines look like they differ in length.

In sum, while individuals with ASD differ from typical development in the degree to which they focus on the higher-Gestalt of a pattern (vs. the local elements), this difference is susceptible to variations in task context. Findings reviewed here pertain to the domains of shifting attention from local elements to global patterns (and vice versa), face perception (which depends on detecting relations between facial parts), reading homographs (which require the entire sentence to be taken into account), and optical illusions (which depends on children failing to ignore the embedding aspects of the target). Such context effects on perception have led to some revisions in the ASD theory of weak central coherence (e.g. Happé& Booth, 2008). Rather than positing an all-or-none competence of Gestalt processing, weak central coherence is now seen as a tendency, a preference of some sort that could be changed under ideal task contexts.

Advertisement

3. Learning of patterns

As mentioned above, ASD is characterized by delays in language learning, including the learning of new words, their use, the pragmatics of language, or the fluidity of use (see e.g., Lord, Risi, & Pickles, 2004, for a review). These delays, as well as other symptoms of autism, have been attributed to differences in how patterns of information are learned (e.g., L. G. Klinger, Klinger, &Pohlig, 2007). More specifically, individuals with autism might have difficulty learning underlying patterns of events when hypothesis-testing strategies cannot be applied. This kind of learning is commonly studied under the umbrella of implicit learning (see Perruchet, 2008; Shanks, 2005, for reviews), artificial-grammars learning (e.g., Reber, 1967), or pattern detection in category formation (e.g., Ashby & Maddox, 2005; Keri, 2003). Here, we use the term “implicit learning”, consistent with the term used in ASD research.

Studies of pattern learning have led to interesting findings in ASD. On the one hand, there are several findings that suggest impaired implicit learning in ASD (e.g., Romero-Munguía, 2008). Consider, for example, findings obtained with the so-called serial reaction time (SRT) task: Participants are asked to press a key to indicate a particular stimulus in a sequence. Learning is reflected in a decrease in reaction time for sequences that contain subtle repeated patterns, compared to random sequences (cf., Nissen&Bullemer, 1987). While typically developing children demonstrated such learning, participants with ASD did not (Mostofsky, Goldberg, Landa, &Denckla, 2000). Further support for compromised implicit learning comes from findings on prototype learning (Klinger & Dawson, 2001). The task was to categorize fictitious animals that differed in features like ear length, leg length, and neck length (cf., Younger, 1993). Children with ASD performed more poorly than control participants match in verbal age (see also Klinger et al., 2007). In fact, performance on implicit learning tasks was highly correlated with ASD symptomatology, including communication skills, social skills, and the occurrence of repetitive behaviors.

However, the difference in implicit-learning abilities between ASD and control participants is not stable across task context, even when tested in the same lab (cf., Klinger & Dawson 2001; Klinger et al., 2001). Consider the SRT task again: when the inter-stimulus interval was reduced to 120ms (Barnes, Howard, Howard, Gilotty, Kenworthy, Gaillard 2008) or omitted altogether (Travers, Klinger, Mussey, & Klinger 2010), there was no difference between ASD and control participants. Both groups of children could learn to anticipate the rule-based sequence, compared to a random sequence (see also Muller, Cauich, Rubio, Mizuno, &Couchesne, 2004). Similarly, there was evidence for sequence learning when the rule was greatly simplified and the training extended to multiple sessions (Gordon & Stark, 2007). Furthermore, children with ASD demonstrate repetition priming effects comparable to those of controls (i.e., they could identify studies items better than non-studied items; Renner, Klinger, & Renner, 2000). And they were found to have intact semantic priming for simple common words (Toichi&Kamio, 2002) – further evidence for implicit-learning abilities in ASD. Overall, these findings have undermined a claim that ASD is characterized by a general difficulty with implicit learning, in turn undermining an effort to explain social deficits, motor abnormalities, and language deficits associated with the disorder.

There are many ways in which context effects on implicit learning could be explained. For example, one could address the differences in findings by looking for differences in the groups of participants, whether in age, symtomatology, or co-morbidity. It is possible that the findings fail to univocally address the question of implicit-learning competence in ASD because participants differ across different tasks. Or one could look for differences in other internal processes that could explain the pattern of performance. Tasks might differ in the degree to which they tap a participant’s working memory. Or they differ in the extent to which they require the integration of gross-motor movements. Or they differ in whether they afford or undermine the use of explicit (i.e., hypothesis-testing) strategies. Indeed, ASD performance is comparable to that of typically developing children when the prototype learning task required a rule-based approach (Klinger & Dawson, 2001). And an exceedingly short inter-stimulus interval might have forced the minds of participants with ASD to abandon their bias to use a hypothesis-testing strategy and therefore make room for an implicit-learning process. There are multiple problems with this kind of reasoning, a major one being that it fails to address the entire list of context effects – beyond a comparison of a few studies.

Advertisement

4. Executive functioning

Executive functioning (EF) is an umbrella term to describe various cognitive abilities assumed to be involved in conscious problem-solving. They pertain, for example, to inhibiting incorrect but dominant actions, planning a future action, and flexibly switching attention when instructed to do so (e.g., Zelazo&Mϋeller, 2002). EF plays an important role in cognitive development, as it leads to an improved ability to override automatic responses (Garon, Bryson, & Smith, 2008). A classical EF task – but by far not the only one – is the Stroop task, a task in which participants are asked to name the color of the ink used for a printed word, the word spelling a particular color (Stroop, 1935). The central finding is a slowing in reaction time when the ink color differs from the spelled-out color (compared to trials in which the ink color matches the spelled-out color), demonstrating the difficulty of inhibiting the automatic tendency to read the word.

EF is thought to be associated with typical ASD attributes, including the need for sameness, difficulty with switching attention, a tendency to perseverate, and a lack of impulse control.Indeed, there are tasks in which participants with ASD show difficulty with inhibition (for a review see Rajendran& Mitchell, 2007). Consider, for example, an inhibition task in which participants have to point to an empty window in order to receive the reward shown in a non-empty window (Hala, &Russel 2001). Unlike control participants, a majority of participants with ASD have difficulty inhibiting their natural response of pointing to the reward they desire, compared to controls matched on mental age. Other examples of EF difficulties consist of difficulties with planning (e.g., (Ozonoff& Jensen, 1999, Ozonoff, Pennington, & Rogers, 1991), mental flexibility (e.g., Hughes, Russell, & Robbins, 1994; Ozonoff, 1997), the generation of novel ideas (Turner, 1999), and self-monitoring (e.g., Hughes, 1996; Phillips, Baron-Cohen, &Rutter 1998; Russell &Jarrold, 1998, 1999).

However, there are findings that undermine a straightforward ASD theory surrounding EF differences. For example, participants with ASD do not have more difficulty with the Stroop task than control participants (e.g., Ozonoff& Jensen, 1999; Eskes, Bryson, & McCormick, 1990): participants with ASD were found to show a typical slowing in reaction time when naming the ink of a word that spells a different color. Similarly, context effects were found with planning task that involves keeping in mind a certain set of rules to produce an outcome (e.g., Tower of Hanoi task, Stockings of Cambridge task). ASD performance was equivalent to typically developing performance on trials with only a small number of required steps for completion. Performance only differentiated between groups on longer, more complex trials. Further, performance appeared modulated by each individual child’s nonverbal IQ, rather than symptomology (Hughes, Russell, & Robbins, 1994).

It appears that a claim about EF differences between typical development and ASD is not supported in all instances (for further review, see Hill, 2004). Performance seems instead dependent on specifics of the tasks and individual differences among children. Of course, it is always possible to interpret discrepant results consistent with a reductionist viewpoint. For example, one could argue that EF differences between typical and atypical development are most pronounced in so-called “hot” EF task, those that involve an emotional component (cf., Hongwanishkul, Happaney, Lee, &Zelazo, 2005). The differences might disappear in “cool” EF tasks, those that lack immediate rewards. These claims, though plausibly incorporating currently existing data, might not be able to capture context effects likely to accumulate as more data is being collected.

Advertisement

5. Social reasoning

Adaptive functioning includes social reasoning, or a child’s ability to engage in social interactions. ASD is characterized by major difficulties in this domain, ranging from attending to irrelevant features of social situations (e.g., Dawson, Meltzoff, Osterling, Rinaldi, & Brown, 1998; Klin, Jones, Schultz, Volkmar& Cohen 2002), giving atypical responses to social cues (e.g., Kjelgaard&Tager-Flusberg, 2001; Mottron, 2004; Mundy, Sigman&Kasari, 1990; Parish-Morris et al., 2007; Sigman& Ruskin, 1999; Stone, Ousley, Yoder, Hogan, & Hepburn 1997), having difficulty understanding the intentions of others (e.g., Baron-Cohen, 1995; Preissler& Carey, 2005; Warreyn, Roeyers, Oelbrandt, & De Groote 2005), and poor imitation skills (e.g., Hobson & Lee, 1999; Loveland, Tunali-Kotoski, Pearson, Brelsford, Ortegon, & Chen 1994). Here we describe findings for two of these areas, namely joint attention (i.e., the act of sharing another’s attentional focus) and of theory of mind (i.e., the understanding of others’ intentions).

5.1. Joint attention

The ability to share somebody else’s focus of attention, known as joint attention, is critical for successful social interactions, setting up a context in which a child can learn from others. For example, a child needs to know what a person is looking at to understand what a new label might refer to. Indeed, joint attention has been studied extensively in relation to children’s word learning (e.g., Baldwin, 1995; Mundy & Newell, 2007; Carpenter, Nagell, &Tomasello, 1998; Tomasello, 1995; see also Flom, Lee, & Muir, 2007). A common task involves presenting children with a set of objects, and an adult visibly looking at the one that is being named. Both the amount of time the participant follows the eye-gaze of the adult and the degree of labellingare thought to reflect the amount of joint attention that occurs between them.

Children with ASD have demonstrated difficulty following the gaze of an adult in joint attention tasks (for a review, see Meindl&Cannella-Malone, 2011). And this deficit is observed alongside difficulties with learning new object names (Baron-Cohen, Baldwin, &Crowson, 1997; McDuffie, Yoder, & Stone, 2006; Parish-Morris, Hennon, Hirsh-Pasek, Golinkoff, &Tager-Flusberg, 2007; Preissler& Carey, 2005). For example, there is a pronounced learning difference between children with ASD and typically developing children when the labeled object was held by the experimenter, versus by the child (Preissler& Carey, 2005). This difference cannot be attributed to general word-learning deficits because word learning did not differ between diagnostic groups when the labeled object was in the child’s hand. Similarly, learning did not differ between diagnostic groups when the labeled object was the only novel object.

Yet, despite strong evidence in favor of ASD impairments in joint attention, findings from other research complicate the picture: participants with ASD appear perfectly capable of joint attention in some contexts, if not even more skilled than their typically developing counterparts (Chawarska,Klin, Volkmar 2003; Kylliainen&Hietanen, 2004; Vlamings, Strauder, van Son, Mottron 2005). Consider, for example, a task in which participants have to press a corresponding button as soon as they see a target appear either at the top left or the bottom right of a monitor. A face was also shown in the center of the monitor. The gaze of the face was straight ahead, averted to the top left, or averted to the bottom right, 200ms before the target appeared. Findings show faster reaction time on trials in which the target appeared on the same side of the screen as the face’s gaze, with no difference between diagnostic groups (Kylliainen&Hietanen, 2004).

An argument could be made that different joint-attention tasks are not equally suited to capture the construct of joint attention. Maybe the reaction time task is a better reflection of joint-attention processes than a word-learning task. Such argument about what task might best reflect a stable factor is a common argument in the larger literature of cognition and cognitive development. However, it gets quickly overwhelmed as more context effects accumulate.

5.2. Theory of mind

Another aspect of social reasoning is the ability to understand someone else’s mental state, including their desires, motivation or beliefs. This kind of understanding is coined as theory of mind, with numerous studies investigating it and its development (Perner, 1991; Wellman, 1990). In a traditional theory of mind task, children are presented with two hiding locations, a basket and a box and two dolls, Sally and Anne. The story involves Sally placing a marble in a basket, which is then moved into the box by Anne – without Sally being present. The critical task is to determine the location where Sally would search for her marble upon her return. If children understand Sally’s mental state, they should pick the basket, because that is the marble’s location known to Sally. If, on the other hand, children go by their own beliefs, they should pick the box, because they know that Anne has moved the marble into the box.

Many studies have sought to identify general deficits in theory of mind reasoning in individuals with ASD, as a means of better understanding their social deficits (for reviews see Rajendran& Mitchell,2007). For example, Baron-Cohen et al. (1985) found that 80% of children with ASD failed the Sally-and-Anne test, compared to only 20% of matched controls. This difference cannot be attributed to general difficulties understanding the task instructions, given that children with ASD were able to answer control questions about the various locations of the marbles. Taken alone, these data appear to highlight a pathology specific impairment to theory of mind reasoning in individuals with ASD.

However, highly variable performance between similar theory of mind tasks has been observed (Grant, Grayson and Boucher, 2001; Baron-Cohen, Leslie, & Frith 1985; Yirmiya, Solomonica-Levi, Shulman &Pilowsky, 1996; Zelazo, Burack, Benedetto, & Frye 1996). Consider, for example, findings with the so-called deceptive-box task, a task in which the content of a box does not match with the label on the box (Grant, Grayson & Boucher, 2001). After being shown the content of the box, participants are asked about what another participant would predict about the contents of the box (without having seen inside the box). To answer accurately, the participant must understand that their knowledge of the contents of the box is not accessible to the other participant. Comparison of performance on the deceptive-box and Sally-and-Anne tasks revealed important differences in ASD: Performance was better on the deceptive-box task than the Sally-and-Anne task, despite the conceptual similarity.

In defense of a reductionist framework for ASD, one could go about dissecting the tasks in order to find the stable factor that could explain ASD. For example, one could argue that the two tasks differ in whether they involve real people (the deceptive box task) or puppets (the Salley-and-Anne task). Variable performance between these tasks may therefore reflect a difference in the perception of behavior associated with living versus inanimate actors. However, this explanation falls apart when the larger body of theory of mind research is considered: The inconsistent findings in theory of mind tasks (for a review, see Rajendran& Mitchell, 2007) to do separate on the fault line of inanimate vs. animate stimuli.

Advertisement

6. Summary and conclusions

ASD is diagnosed in about 1 in every 88 children (CDC, 2012), many of whom will have poor outcomes as adults, requiring some level of assistance throughout their lives (Seltzer, Shattuck, & Greenberg, 2004). In addition to this high prevalence of the disorder, there is a high heterogeneity, high co-morbidity, and the possibility of several subgroups of ASD. Together, these factors make it imperative to better understand the disorder and develop effective interventions. However, as more research accumulates, so do inconsistent findings and unexpected differences in patterns of performance on tasks that were designed to measure the same cognitive process or factor.

In the current chapter, we have reviewed some context effects taken from the domains of perception, learning, executive functioning, and social reasoning. On the one hand, while there are robust differences in performance in all of these domains, these differences can disappear under certain task contexts – rendering them less robust than initially thought. Specifically, while ASD is characterized by a focus on local details (vs. on an overall Gestalt), by difficulty with implicit learning, executive functioning, and social reasoning, these differences disappear as the variability in tasks increases. It is plausible that more context effects accumulate as ASD research expands in cognitive development, further exasperated by a focus on individual children.

Context effects are nothing new in the literature of cognitive development (e.g., Kloos& Van Orden, 2009). A plausible reaction is to dismiss them as isolated instances, leaving an existing hypothesis intact, or refining it to incorporate the context effects. The problem, however, is that both these solutions can only address context effects locally. Yet, context effects are not a local phenomenon. And they are not likely to disappear with more participants, more precise methods, or simply more data. In fact, if research with typically developing participants is any guide for predicting the patterns of findings, context effects are a necessary feature of the enterprise. And with more research we are likely to find more context effects (e.g., see Shanks, Rowland & Ranger, 2005, for a discussion on implicit learning in neuro-typical adults). An expanding of a reductionist theory of ASD to incorporate them all is unlikely to retain its usefulness. Instead, context effects undermine a reductionist theory altogether.

Rather than focusing on a binary interpretation of patterns of performance, context effects hint at the possibility of a complex interplay between factors that make a black-and-white approach to understanding performance insufficient. By shifting attention away from searching for a “smoking gun” of ASD, it may be possible to better understand the emergence of how the components are coordinated. It is possible that the coordination among components is compromised in individuals with ASD, reverberating through all areas of functioning, and amplifying itself with development. It gives rise to atypical perception patterns, implicit learning, planning, and social interactions – at least when the immediate task context does not support an adaptive coordination of interdependent components. This approach, while failing to reduce ASD to a single deficit, has important implications for training and teaching strategies. In particular, this approach makes it possible to map out how changes in support of coordination exist in the environment to bring about improved task performance.

Advertisement

Acknowledgement

Writing of this chapter was partially supported by a University of Cincinnati Research Council Summer Fellowship awarded to Joseph Amaral. We thank Samantha Linsky and Alexandra Matthews for editorial help.

References

  1. 1. American Psychiatric Association2000Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev). Washington, DC: Author.
  2. 2. AshbyF. GMaddoxW. T2005Human category learning.Annual Review of Psychology, 56149178
  3. 3. BaldwinD. A1995Understanding the link between joint attention and language. In C. Moore, & P. J. Dunham (Eds.) Joint attention: Its origin and role in development (131158Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  4. 4. BarnesK. AHowardJ. HJr., Howard, D. V., Gilotty, L., Kenworthy, L., Gaillard, W. D., et al. (2008Intact implicit learning of spatial context and temporal sequences in childhood autism spectrum disorder.Neuropsychology, 22(5), 563-570.
  5. 5. Baron-cohenS1995Mindblindness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  6. 6. Baron-cohenSBaldwinD. ACrowsonM1997Do children with autism use the speaker’s direction of gaze strategy to crack the code of language? Child Development, 684857
  7. 7. Baron-cohenSLeslieA. MFrithU1985Does the autistic-child have a theory of mind.Cognition, 213746
  8. 8. BaylisG. CDriverJ. S1993Visual attention and objects: Evidence for heirarchical coding of locations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19451470
  9. 9. BonnelAMottronLPeretzITrudelMGallunEBonnelA. M2003Enhanced pitch sensitivity in individuals with autism: A signal detection analysis.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15226235
  10. 10. BrosnanM. JScottF. JFoxSPyeJ2004Gestalt processing in autism: failure to process perceptual relationships and the implications for contextual understandingJournal of Child Psychology And Psychiatry459 EOF469 EOF
  11. 11. BrysonS. EZwaigenbaumLBrianJRobertsWSzatmariPRomboughVMcdermottC2007A prospective case series of high-risk infants who developed autismJournal of Autism and Developmental Disorders371224
  12. 12. CarpenterMNagellKTomaselloM1998Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age.Monographs of the Society forResearch in Child Development, 63 (4), serial255
  13. 13. Centers for Disease Control & Prevention (2012Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders: Autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 14 sites, united states, 2008. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report: Surveillance Summaries, 61(3), 1-18.
  14. 14. ChangiziM. AHsiehANijhawanRKanaiRShimojoS2008Perceiving the present and a systematization of illusionsCognitive Science32459503
  15. 15. ChawarskaKKlinAVolkmarF2003Automatic attention cueing through eye movement in 2year-old children with autismChild Development
  16. 16. DawsonGMeltzoffAOsterlingJRinaldiJBrownE1998Children with autism fail to orient to naturally occurring social stimuli.Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders479 EOF85 EOF
  17. 17. DeruelleCRondanCSalle-collemicheXBastard-rossetDDa Fonséca, D. (2008Attention to low- and high- spatial frequencies in categorizing facial identities, emotions and gender in children with autism. Brain and Cognition66115123
  18. 18. DevlinBS. WScherer2012Genetic architecture in autism spectrum disorder.Current Opinion in Genetics & Development, 22(3), 229-237.
  19. 19. EskesG. ABrysonS. EMccormickT. A1990Comprehension of concrete and abstract words in autistic children.Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,206173
  20. 20. FagotJDeruelleC1997Processing of global and local visual information and hemispheric specialization in humans (Homo sapiens) and baboons (Papiopapio).Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23(2), 429-442.
  21. 21. FeinD. A2011The neuropsychology of autismNew York: Oxford University Press.
  22. 22. FlomRLeeKMuirDEds.). (2007Gaze-following: Its development and significance.Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  23. 23. FoxtonJ. MStewartM. EBarnardLRodgersJYoungA. HOBrienGGriffithsT. D. (2003Absence of auditory global interference in autism. Brain, 12627032709
  24. 24. FrithU1989Autism: Explaining the enigma.Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishing.
  25. 25. FrithUSnowlingM1983Reading for meaning and reading for sound in autistic and dyslexic childrenBritish Journal of Developmental Psychology1329342
  26. 26. GaronNBrysonS. ESmithI. M2008Executive function in preschoolers: A reviewUsing an integrative framework. Psychological Bulletin, 1343160
  27. 27. GordonBStarkS2007Procedural learning of a visual sequence in individuals with autism.Focus on Autism & Other Developmental Disabilities, 221422
  28. 28. GrantCGraysonABoucherJ2001Using tests of false belief with children with autism: How valid and reliable are they?Autism, 5135145
  29. 29. HalaSRusselJ2001Executive control within strategic deception: A window on early cognitive development.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology112 EOF141 EOF
  30. 30. HappéF. G. E1997Central coherence and theory of mind in autism: Reading homographs in contextBritish Journal of Developmental Psychology15112
  31. 31. HappéF. G. E2000Parts and wholes, meaning and minds: Central coherence and its relation to theory of mind. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism and developmental cognitive neuroscience (2nd Ed.) (203221Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  32. 32. HappéF. G. EFrithU2006The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders.Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders36525
  33. 33. HappéF. G. EBoothR. D. L2008The power of the positive: Revisiting weak coherence in autism spectrum disorders.The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 615063
  34. 34. HeatonPHermelinBPringL1998Autism and pitch processing: A precursor for savant musical abilityMusic Perception15291305
  35. 35. HerrmannC. SCa, & Bosch, 2001Gestalt perception modulates early visual processing.Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuropsychology, 12(5), 901 EOF4 EOF
  36. 36. HillE. L2004Evaluating the theory of executive dysfunction in autismDevelopmental Review, 24189233
  37. 37. HobsonR. PLeeA1999Imitation and identification in autism.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 40(4), 649-659.
  38. 38. HongwanishkulDHappaneyK. RLeeW. S. CZelazoP. D2005assessment of hot and cool executive function in young children: Age-related changes and individual differencesDevelopmental Neuropsychology617 EOF644 EOF
  39. 39. HughesC1996Brief report: Planning problems in autism at the level of motor control.Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders2699107
  40. 40. HughesCRussellJRobbinsT1994Evidence for executive dysfunction in autism.Neruopsychologia, 32(4), 477-492.
  41. 41. HumphreysG. WOlsonARomaniCRiddochM. J1996Competitive mechanisms of selection by objects and space.In A. Kramer & N. Coles (Eds.), Converging operationsin the study of visual selective attention.Hillsdale, N. J.: Erlbaum.
  42. 42. JolliffeTBaron-cohenS1999The strange stories test: a replication with high-functioning adults with autism or asperger syndrome.Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders395 EOF406 EOF
  43. 43. KahnemanDHenikA1981Perceptual organization and attention.In M. Kubovy& J.R. Pomerantz (Eds.), Perceptual organization (181211Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  44. 44. KahnemanDTreismanA1984Changing views of attention and automaticity.In R. Parasuraman, D.R. Davies & J. Beatty (Eds.), Variants of attention (2961New York: Academic Press.
  45. 45. KeehnBLuysterRVogel-farleyVTager-flusbergHNelsonC2012Gamma-Band Activity and Coherence in Response to Familiar and Unfamiliar Faces in Infants at Risk for Autism Spectrum Disorder. Paper presented at the International Meeting for Autism Research, May 19, Toronto.
  46. 46. KeriS2003The cognitive neuroscience of category learning.Brain ResearchReviews, 4385109
  47. 47. KimchiR1992Primacy of wholistic processing and global/local paradigm: A critical review.Psychological Bulletin1122438
  48. 48. KjelgaardM. MTager-flusbergH2001An investigation of language impairment in autism: Implications for genetic subgroupsLanguage and Cognitive Processes287 EOF308 EOF
  49. 49. KlinAJonesWSchultzRVolkmarFCohenD2002Defining and quantifying the social phenotype in autism.American Journal of Psychiatry, 159895908
  50. 50. KlingerL. GDawsonGPRenner2003Autistic disorder. In E.J. Mash & R.A. Barkley (Eds.), Child Psychopathology (409-454). New York: Guilford.
  51. 51. KlingerL. GKlingerM. RPohligR2007Implicit learning impairments in autism spectrum disorders: Implications for treatment. In J. M. Perez, P. M. González, M. L. Comí, & C. Nieto (Eds.), New developments in autism: The future is today76103London: Jessica Kingsley.
  52. 52. KloosHVan OrdenG. C2009Soft-assembled mechanisms for the unified theory. In J.P. Spencer, M. Thomas, & J. McClelland (Eds.) Toward a Unified Theory of Development: Connectionism and Dynamic Systems Theory Re-Considered,253267Oxford University Press.
  53. 53. KramerA. FJacobsonA1991Perceptual organization and focused attention:the role of objects and proximity in visual processing.Perception & Psychophysics,50267284
  54. 54. KylliainenAHietanenJ2004Attention orienting by another’s gaze direction in children with autismJournal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry435 EOF444 EOF
  55. 55. LangdellT1978Recognition of faces : an approach to the study of autismJournal of Child Psychology and Psychiatryand Allied Disciplines, 193255268
  56. 56. LopezBDonnellyNHadwinJ. ALeekamS. R2004Face processing in high-functioning adolescents with autism: Evidence for weak central coherenceVisualCognition, 11(6), 673 EOF688 EOF
  57. 57. LopezBLeekamS. R2003Do children with autism fail to process information in context? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(2), 285-300.
  58. 58. LordCRisiSPicklesA2004Trajectory of language development in autistic spectrum disorders. In M. L. Rice & S. F. Warren (Eds.) Developmental Language Disorders: From Phenotypes to Etiologies(7-29). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  59. 59. LovelandK. ATunali-kotoskiBPearsonD. ABrelsfordK. AOrtegonJChenR1994Imitation and expression of facial affect in autismDevelopment and Psychopathology6433444
  60. 60. MaurerDLe Grand, R., &Mondloch, C. (2002The many faces of configural processing.Trends in Cognitive Science,6255260
  61. 61. McduffieAYoderPStoneW2006Fast-mapping in young children with autism spectrum disorders.First Language, 26421436
  62. 62. MeindlJ. NCannella-maloneH. I2011Initiating and responding to joint attention bids in children with autism: A review of the literatureResearch in Developmental Disabilities14411454
  63. 63. MondlochC. JLe Grand, R., & Maurer, D. (2002Configural face processing develops more slowly than featural face processing.Perception31553566
  64. 64. MooreC. MEgethH1997Perception without attention: Evidence of grouping under conditions of inattention.Journal of Experimental PsychologyHuman Perception and Performance, 23339352
  65. 65. MostofskyS. HGoldbergM. CLandaR. JDencklaM. B2000Evidence for a deficit in procedural learning in children and adolescents with autism: Implications for cerebellar contribution.Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6(7), 752 EOF9 EOF
  66. 66. MottronL2004Matching strategies in cognitive research with individuals with high-functioning autism: Currentpractices, instrument biases, and recommendations.Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 341927
  67. 67. MottronLBurackJ. AStauderJ. E. ARobaeyP1999Perceptual processing among high-functioning persons with autism.Journal of Child Psychology &Psychiatry, 40203211
  68. 68. MottronLPeretzIMenardE2000Local and global processing of music in high-functioning persons with autism: beyond central coherence? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 4110571065
  69. 69. MüllerRCauichCRubioM. AMizunoACourchesneE2004Abnormal activity patterns in premotor cortex during sequence learning in autistic patients.Biological Psychiatry56323332
  70. 70. MundyPNewellL2007Attention, joint attention, and social cognition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(5), 269-274.
  71. 71. MundyPSigmanMKasariC1990A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children.Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20115128
  72. 72. NavonD1977Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception.Cognitive Psychology9353383
  73. 73. NissenM. JBullemerP1987Attentional requirements of learning:Evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology19132
  74. 74. NordinVGillbergC1998The long-term course of autistic disorders: Updateon follow-up studies. ActaPsychiatricaScandinavica, 97(2), 99-108.
  75. 75. OzonoffS1997Components of executive function in autism and other disorders.In J. Russell (Ed.) Autism as an Executive Disorder (179211Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  76. 76. OzonoffSJensenJ1999Brief report: Specific executive function profiles in threeneurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(2), 171-177.
  77. 77. OzonoffSPenningtonBRogersS1991Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: relationship to theory of mind.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatryand Allied Disciplines, 32(7), 1081 EOF105 EOF
  78. 78. OzonoffSStrayerD. LMcmahonW. MFillouxF1994Executive function abilities in autism and Tourette syndrome: An information processing approach.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry3510151032
  79. 79. Parish-morrisJHennonEHirsh-pasekKGolinkoffRTager-flusbergH2007Children with autism illuminate the role of social intention in word learningChild Development1265 EOF1287 EOF
  80. 80. PellicanoERhodesG2003Holistic processing of faces in preschool children and adultsPsychological Science14618622
  81. 81. PernerJ1991Understanding the representational mindCambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  82. 82. PerruchetP2008Implicit learning.In H. Roediger III (Ed.), Cognitive psychology of memory. Oxford, UK: Elsevier.
  83. 83. PhillipsWBaron-cohenSRutterM1998Understanding intention in normal development and autism.British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16337348
  84. 84. PlaistedKSwettenhamJReesL1999Children with autism show local precedence in a divided attention task and global precedence in a selective attention taskJournal of Child Psychology and Psychiatryand Allied Disciplines, 40(5), 733 EOF742 EOF
  85. 85. PreisslerMCareyS2005The role of inferences about referential intent in word learning: evidence from autism.CognitionB13B23.
  86. 86. RajendranGMitchellP2007Cognitive theories of autism.Developmental Review, 27(2), 224-260.
  87. 87. ReberA. S1967Implicit learning of artificial grammars.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 77317327
  88. 88. Romero-munguíaM. A2008Amnesic imbalance: A cognitive theory about autism spectrum disorders. Annals of General Psychiatry, 7, 20.
  89. 89. RussellJJarroldC1998Error-correction problems in autism: Evidence for a monitoring impairment.Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders28177188
  90. 90. RussellJJarroldC1999Memory for actions in children with autism: Self versus other.Cognitive Neuropsychiatry4303331
  91. 91. SeltzerMShattuckPAbbedutoLGreenbergJ2004Trajectory of development in adolescents and adults with autism.Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Review, 10(4), 234-247.
  92. 92. ScottF. JBrosnanMWheelwrightSin preparation) Perception of Illusions by People with Autism: isthere a low-level central coherence deficit? University of Cambridge, UK.
  93. 93. ShanksD. RRowlandL. ARangerM. S2005Attentional load and implicit sequence learning.Psychological Research, 69(5-6). 369382
  94. 94. ShanksD. R2005Implicit learning. In K. Lamberts & R. L. Goldstone (Eds.), Handbookof Cognition (202220London, UK: Sage.
  95. 95. SigmanMMcgovernC2005Improvement in cognitive and language skills from pre-school to adolescence in autism.Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 351523
  96. 96. SnowlingMFrithU1986Comprehension in hyperlexic readers.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 42392415
  97. 97. SmithL. BThelenETitzerRMclinD1999Knowing in the context of acting: The task dynamics of the A-not-B-error.Psychological Review, 106(2), 235-260.
  98. 98. SigmanMRuskinE1999Continuity and change in the social competence of children with autism, Down syndrome, and developmental delaysMonographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 64, 114
  99. 99. SpencerJ. PThomasM. SMcclellandJ. LEds.) (2009Toward a Unified Theory ofDevelopment: Connectionism and Dynamic Systems Theory Re-Considered. New York: Oxford University Press.
  100. 100. StoneWOusleyOYoderPHoganKHepburnS1997Nonverbal communication in two- and three-year-old children with autism.Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27677696
  101. 101. StroopJ. R1935The basis of Ligon’s theory.The American Journal of Psychology, 47(3), 499-504.
  102. 102. TantamDMonaghanLNicholsonHStirlingJ1989Autistic children’s ability to interpret faces: A researchnote. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 30(4), 623-630.
  103. 103. ThagardP1989Explanatory coherence.Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12435502
  104. 104. TraversB. GKlingerM. RMusseyJ. LKlingerL. G2010Motor-linked implicit learning in persons with autism spectrum disorders.Autism Research, 36877
  105. 105. ToichiMKamioY2002Long-term memory and levels-of-processing in autism.Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 3148390
  106. 106. TomaselloM1995Joint attention as social cognition. In C. Moore, & P. J. Dunham (Eds.) Joint attention: Its origins and rolein development (103129New Jersey: Erlbaum, Hillsdale.
  107. 107. VlamingsPStrauderJVan SonIMottronL2005Atypical visual orienting to gaze-and arrow-cues in adults with high functioning autism.Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(3), 267-277.
  108. 108. WarreynPRoeyersHOelbrandtTDe GrooteI2005What are you looking at? Joint attention and visual perspective taking in young children with autism spectrum disorder.Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 175573
  109. 109. WellmanH. M1990The child’s theory of mindCambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  110. 110. YirmiyaNSolomoncia- LeviDShulmanCPilowskyT1996Theory of mind Abilities in individuals with autism, Down syndrome, and mental retardation of unknown etiology: The role of age and intelligence, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 378100314
  111. 111. YoungerB1993Understanding category members as “the same sort of thing”: Explicit categorization in ten-month infants.Child Development64309320
  112. 112. ZelazoP. DBurackJ. ABenedettoEFryeD1996Theory of mind and rule usein individuals with Down’s syndrome: A test of the uniqueness and specificityclaims, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37447984
  113. 113. ZelazoP. DMüllerU2002Executive function in typical and atypical development. In U. Goswami (Ed.), Handbook of childhood cognitive development. Oxford: Blackwell.
  114. 114. ZwaigenbaumLBrysonSRogersTRobertsWBrianJSzatmariP2005Behavioral manifestations of autism in the first year of life International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 23143152

Written By

Joseph L. Amaral, Susan Collins, Kevin T. Bohache and Heidi Kloos

Submitted: 04 April 2012 Published: 14 November 2012