Open access

Laterality Explored: Atypical Hemispheric Dominance in Down Syndrome

Written By

George Grouios, Antonia Ypsilanti and Irene Koidou

Submitted: 26 April 2012 Published: 06 March 2013

DOI: 10.5772/53829

From the Edited Volume

Down Syndrome

Edited by Subrata Kumar Dey

Chapter metrics overview

2,251 Chapter Downloads

View Full Metrics

1. Introduction

Down syndrome (DS) is a genetic disorder caused by an extra copy of chromosome 21 (trisomy 21), with an incidence in 1 in 700 live births. The third chromosome causes a series of physical, biological and behavioural characteristics that are syndrome-specific including intellectual disability, heart defects, problems in the endocrine and immune system and other medical conditions (Epstein et al., 1991). Moreover, there is established evidence for the language difficulties in people with DS particularly in expressive vocabulary and grammar. Research on language has documented a specific pattern of cerebral lateralization that commonly characterizes these individuals, that is unique to the syndrome compared to typically developing individuals and individuals with intellectual disability (ID) non-DS. This realization has triggered the interest of neuropsychologists to investigate atypical hemispheric dominance in DS.

Atypical hemispheric Dominance, or otherwise termed "anomalous dominance” or “anomalous cerebral organization”, refers to the atypical lateralization of language areas within the brain (Geschwind & Galaburda, 1985). Usually, most right-handed individuals (97%) exhibit left-hemisphere lateralization for language. The remaining 3% of right-handed individuals exhibit bilateral or right hemisphere lateralization for language (Bishop, 1990). In left-handed individuals this distribution is very different. About 60% of left-handed individuals exhibit left-hemisphere lateralization for language, 30% bilateral lateralization and 10% right-hemisphere lateralization for language (Bishop, 1990). Geschwind and Behan (1982) termed anomalous dominance that in which the pattern of language laterality differed from the “… standard dominance pattern” (pp. 70). Bryden, McManus and Bulman-Fleming (1994) criticized this definition, highlighting that if one accepts this description “… we run the risk of defining the majority of the population as being anomalous” (pp. 111). According to Geschwind and Galaburda (1985a; 1985b), atypical dominance may involve the inverse or weak dominance of three features; hand dominance, language dominance and visuospatial dominance. Previc (1994) distinguished the term atypical laterality into anatomical atypical asymmetry, which involves the decreased volume of the left hemisphere compared to the right hemisphere, particularly in the temporal region, and is observed in approximately 30-35% of the normal population, and functional atypical asymmetry, which relates to the bilateral or right hemisphere language dominance.

During the past decades atypical laterality has been studied in a number of pathological conditions, including individuals with intellectual disability (ID) (e.g., Grouios, Sakadami, Poderi, & Alevriadou, 1999), DS (e.g., Heath & Elliott, 1999), autism (Cornish & McManus, 1996), Turner syndrome (Ganou & Grouios, 2008), Klinefelter syndrome (Ganou, Grouios, koidou, & Alevriadou, 2010), Williams syndrome (Järvinen-Pasley, Pollak, Yam, Hill, Grichanik et al., 2010), fragile-X syndrome (Cornish, Pigram, & Shaw, 1997), developmental stuttering (Foundas, Corey, Angeles, Bollich, Crabtree-Hartman et al., 2003), developmental dyslexia (Illingworth & Bishop, 2009), disabled reading (Dalby & Gibson, 1981), attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (Hale, Zaidel, McGough, Phillips, & McCracken, 2006), depression (Pinea, Kentgena, Bruderb, Leiteb, Bearmana et al., 2000), schizophrenia (Giotakos, 1999) and epilepsy (Slezicki, Cho, Brock, Pfeiffer, McVearry et al., 2009). The aim of the present review is to present and discuss research on atypical cerebral laterality in DS.

Advertisement

2. Laterality measures

There are several techniques with which one can assess the laterality of cognitive functions. A broad division of these techniques is that between invasive and non-invasive laterality measures.

An invasive technique is one, which penetrates or breaks the skin or enters a body cavity. The only available invasive technique for the assessment of lateralization of cognitive functions is the intracarotid amobarbital procedure (IAP) or Wada test. The IAP is a procedure first described by Wada (1949) and Wada and Rasmussen (1960) for anaesthetizing cerebral hemispheres for the purpose of lateralizing language and memory functions. The procedure consists of unilateral injection of sodium amobarbital into the internal carotid, which temporarily anaesthetizes the hemisphere ipsilateral to the injection site. While one hemisphere is anaesthetized, language and memory functions of the hemisphere contralateral to the injection site can be tested. After the effect of the anaesthesia has dissipated, the process is repeated with the other hemisphere. Determining the lateralization of language and memory functions is of both theoretical and practical interest, establishing cerebral language lateralization, predicting patients who are at risk for developing a post-surgical amnestic syndrome and identifying lateralized dysfunction to help confirm seizure onset laterality (Loring & Meador, 2000). Scientific investigation of cerebral lateralization in individuals with ID using the IAP is generally hampered for obvious moral and ethical reasons.

A scientific procedure is strictly defined as non-invasive when no break in the skin is created and there is no contact with the mucosa, or skin break, or internal body cavity, beyond a natural or artificial body orifice. Νon-invasive techniques for the assessment of cerebral lateralization can be further subdivided into neuroimaging techniques and behavioural techniques.

Neuroimaging techniques include both anatomical techniques, which create "constructed" images of brain structure, and functional techniques, which generate a series of dynamic brain images reflecting ongoing brain activity (Ganou, Kollias, Koidou, & Grouios, 2012). The anatomical techniques, which are the classical methods to image the brain, comprise computed tomography and structural magnetic resonance imaging. The functional techniques contain both direct (electroencephalography and magnetoencephalography) and indirect (positron-emissiontomography, single photon emission computed tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging) measures of neural activity, which basically measure haemodynamic responses or differences in metabolic concentrations to cognitive stimulation (for more information see Cohen & Sweet, 2011; Hüsing, Jäncke, & Tag, 2006).

Neuroimaging have offered a broad range of investigative tools to basic (e.g., Aziz-Zadeh, Koski, Zaidel, Mazziotta, & Iacoboni, 2006; Jansen, Menke, Sommer, Forster, Bruchmann et al., 2006; Tomasi & Volkow, 2012) and clinical (e.g., Desmond, Sum, Wagner, Demb, Shear et al., 1995; khondi-Asi, Jafari-Khouzani, Elisevich, & Soltanian-Zadeh, 2011; Oertel, Knöchel, Rotarska-Jagiela, Schönmeyer, Lindner et al., 2010) laterality research that fulfill the popular fantasy of being able to ‘‘read the mind,’’ albeit in the form of ‘‘seeing the brain’’ both structurally and functionally (Kerr & Denk, 2008).

Over the past 20 years, evidence for atypical cerebral lateralization in individuals with DS has been adduced using various neuroimaging techniques (Azari, Horwitz, Pettigrew, Grady, Haxby, et al., 1994; Menghini, Costanzo, & Vicari, 2011; Pinter, Eliez, Schmitt, Capone, & Reiss, 2001). However, despite the large and growing literature describing patterns of brain structure and function in the healthy and diseased human brain, scientific research on Down syndrome has not been well integrated into the mainstream of human neuroimaging research. Nevertheless, a few investigators have demonstrated success in applying digital imaging technology in individuals with DS.

For example, Uecker, Mangan, Obrzut and Nadel (1993) argued that diffuse language lateralization in individuals with DS is likely to be a contributor to their poor visuospatial performance. Frangou, Aylward, Warren, Sharma, Barta et al. (1997) investigated whether the anatomic substrate for language are abnormal in DS. They examined volumetric Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) measures of the superior temporal gyrus and the planum temporale for community-dwelling individuals with DS and matched healthy comparison subjects. It was found that brain abnormalities in DS were not uniform. Specifically, the planum temporale volume of the individuals with DS was smaller than that of the healthy subjects. The volume of the superior temporal gyrus in the DS individuals was proportionally similar to that of the comparison group. For the subjects with DS, neither superior temporal gyrus nor planum temporale volume was significantly correlated with performance on language tests. Losin, Rivera, O'Hare, Sowell, and Pinter (2009) compared functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) activation patterns during passive story listening in young adults with DS and approximately age-matched, typically developing controls. They found that individuals with DS exhibited differences in blood oxygen level dependant activation patterns compared to a typically developing group during the fMRI story-listening task. In particular, their results indicated that the DS group showed almost no difference in activation patterns between the language (forward speech) and non-language (backward speech) conditions. Menghini, Costanzo and Vicari (2011) investigated regional grey matter density in adolescents with DS compared to age-matched controls and correlated MRI data with neuropsychological measures in the DS group. Their findings revealed that a number of brain regions subserved the neuropsychological abilities of participants with DS. Although adolescents with DS showed typical organization of brain structures related to some cognitive abilities, in particular spatial memory and visuoperception, they presented abnormal brain organization related to other cognitive domains, such as linguistic and verbal memory. Jacola, Byars, Chalfonte-Evans, Schmithorst, Hickey et al. (2011) used fMRI to investigate neural activation during a semantic-classification/object-recognition task in individuals with DS and typically developing control participants. A comparison between groups suggested atypical patterns of brain activation for the individuals with DS.

Behavioural techniques that have frequently been used to assess cerebral lateralization include those that involve measurement of perceptual asymmetries, those that engage evaluation of sensory asymmetries and those that implicate determination of motor (or manual) asymmetries.

Studies of perceptual asymmetries have been utilized to explore lateral dominance of brain function and comprise dichotic, dichoptic and dichaptic stimulation. The rationale underlying the dichotic listening technique is that contralateral projections from each ear override ipsilateral projections when both ears are simultaneously presented with an auditory stimulus (e.g. a speech sound, digit or a musical tone) and the subject has to report what he/she has heard (Kimura 1967). Individuals with left hemisphere dominance for speech generally show a right-ear advantage for verbal stimuli. The stimuli, most commonly consonant vowel syllables or monosyllabic words, are presented to the participant via ear-phones. Right-handers commonly exhibit a right ear advantage for verbal stimuli (e.g., Elliot & Weeks, 1993; Hugdahl, 2005), although individual differences seem to affect performance (e.g., gender, age) (Cowell & Hugdahl 2000). Empirical research, using dichotic listening techniques, has stressed asymmetry at the perceptual level in individuals with DS (e.g., Bowler, Cufflin, & Kiernan, 1985; Bunn, Welsh, Simon, Howarth, & Elliott, 2003; Hartley, 1981).

In the dichoptic presentation technique (or divided visual field technique), the subject is asked to report verbal stimuli (letters, words) that are rapidly flashed tachistoscopically into one visual half-field, thereby, limiting visual input to the contralateral hemisphere (Banich, 2003). The very short tachistoscopic presentation time prevents possible eye movements and, thus, bilateral cortical projection of the stimuli. Speech stimuli presented in the right visual field and, thus, transmitted primarily to the left hemisphere are recognized and named more rapidly and certainly than stimuli presented in the left visual field (McKeever & Huling, 1970; Hines, 1972). The dominance of the left hemisphere is shown more distinctly in recognition of abstract rather than concrete nouns (Ellis & Shepard, 1974, Hines, 1978) and also of words that only elicit a visual imagination with difficulty (Day, 1979). Right-handers usually show a right visual field advantage for verbal stimuli, as determined by the speed and correctness of the responses (Belin, Jullien, Perrier, & Larmande, 1990). A limited body of literature, using dichoptic presentation techniques, has documented the existence of perceptual asymmetries in individuals with DS (e.g., Chua, Weeks, & Elliott, 1996; Weeks, Chua, Elliot, Lyons, & Pollock, 1995).

The dichaptic stimulation technique requires the subject to feel two different objects with meaningless shapes presented one to each hidden hand at the same time (Witelson, 1974). Upon dichaptic examination, the subject is asked to identify the two shapes from among a collection of six visually displayed shapes (Springer & Deutsch, 1981). Thus, hemispheric differences in haptic perception might be uncovered because of the complexity of the task, by making verbal mediation impossible, or by interfering with the interhemispheric transfer of information through the activation of homologous cortical areas. It has been shown that when meaningless stimuli are used, perceptual asymmetries are usually found in favor of the left hand for right-handed individuals (Benton, Harvey, & Varney, 1973; Dodds, 1978; Verjat, 1988), which reflects a better treatment of spatial information by the right hemisphere. Experimental data, using dichaptic stimulation techniques, have supported the existence of perceptual asymmetries in individuals with DS (e.g., Chua, Weeks, & Elliott, 1996; Elliott, Pollock, Chua, & Weeks, 1995; Weeks, Chua, Elliot, Lyons, & Pollock, 1995).

Laterality researchers have increasingly come to recognize the importance of sensory asymmetries in determining observed patterns of cerebral dominance (Dittmar, 2002). Lateral asymmetries in the use of sensory organs, based on their preferential use or/and functional primacy in a specific situation, are among the most obvious functional lateral preferences (Hellige, 1993), and they figure prominently in explanations of our evolutionary past (Corballis, 1989), of ontogenetic development (Best, 1988; Levy, 1981), and of various abnormalities (Geschwind & Galaburda, 1985). The rationale for using the sensory asymmetries paradigm in the n the context of brain laterality is based on the presumption that difference in sensory performance between sensory stimuli presented to a sensory organ contralateral or ipsilateral to the dominant hemisphere would reflect a hemispheric bias in their attribution strategy (Porac, Coren, Steiger, & Duncan, 1980). Sensory asymmetries are most prominent with respect to the auditory (e.g., Reiss & Reiss, 1998), visual (e.g., Porac & Coren, 1976), tactile (e.g., Harada, Saito, Kashikura, Sato, Yonekura et al., 2004) and chemical senses [taste (e.g., Faurion, Cerf, Van De Moortele, Lobel, MacLeod et al., 1999) and smell (e.g., Royet & Plailly, 2004)]. As far as we know, no study to date has examined sensory asymmetries in DS individuals.

Motor indices of laterality, namely hand and foot preference and performance, have been used extensively to explore fundamental properties of the human brain, such as lateralization of brain functions, both in typically developing individuals (e.g., De Agostini & Dellatolas, 2001; Reiss, Tymnik, Kogler, Kogler, & Reiss 1999) and individuals with DS (e.g., Porac, Coren & Duncan, 1980; Grouios, Sakadami, Poderi & Aleuriadou, 1999). The most commonly used index of laterality is handedness. The main consideration in the assessment of handedness is the use of different handedness measures, which produce different types of handedness. For example, hand preference can be assessed using questionnaires (e.g., Briggs & Nebes, 1972; Oldfield, 1971) on a five-scale continuum ranging from strong left-handers to strong right-handers. Alternatively, researchers have used preference measures to distinguish between left and right-handers (2 categories), excluding intermittent hand preferences (e.g., Coren & Porac, 1980), or right and non-right handers (2 categories) (e.g., Ypsilanti, 2009) or right-handers, left handers and ambiguous (or mixed) handers (3 categories) (e.g., Cornish & McManus, 1996).

In attempting to clarify both the conceptual and theoretical issues surrounding handedness assessment methodology, it is important to discriminate between “direction of hand preference”, “degree of hand preference” and “consistency of hand preference” (Cornish & McManus, 1996). Direction of hand preference refers to the degree of dexterity or sinistrality that an individual exhibits (Bishop, 1990). Degree of hand preference is determined by whether an individual consistently exhibits a specific hand preference across several tasks or behaviours (Cornish & McManus, 1996). Consistency of hand preference is ascertained by whether an individual exhibits a specific hand preference for the same task on several occasions (Cornish & McManus, 1996). Consistency of hand preference was previously described by Palmer (1964), which he termed “variable hand preference” and postulated to be increased in left-handers. Moreover, the degree of hand preference was also previously described by Palmer (1964) which he termed “ambidexterity or mixed motor preference” referring to the degree of hand differentiation across different tasks.

Classification of handedness is further complicated by the fact that a researcher may assess hand preference (be that the direction, degree, or consistency) by a self-reported questionnaire (e.g., Briggs & Nebes, 1972) or a behavioural measure of hand preference (e.g., Bryden, Pryde, & Roy, 2000) or observation of hand preference (Porac & Coren, 1981) and/or hand performance or hand skill, which evaluates the proficiency of one hand over the other in performing a specific task (e.g., pegboard). The advantage of accessing hand preference is that one can evaluate several tasks (e.g., writing, throwing, cutting and dealing cards), rather than assessing hand performance on one task. However, assessing hand performance assists in the more qualitative understanding of handedness by allowing individuals to document their relative proficiency of one hand over the other. Most researchers (e.g., Porac & Coren, 1981; Bishop, 1990) agree that the assessments of hand preference and hand skill are two qualitatively different measures (i.e., they measure different things) of handedness. The mechanisms that mediate preference and performance are different representing two dimensions of laterality. In essence hand preference is mediated more by cognitive mechanisms that support the choice of hand-use, while hand skill may be less mediated by cognitive mechanisms and more supported by motoric systems. Annett, Hudson and Turner (1974) have supported the use of performance measures, suggesting that the relative proficiency of one hand over the other would most likely lead to increased preference of the more skilled hand.

The assessment of preference in populations with DS using questionnaires has been scarce since most clinical groups document ID, which may interfere with the process of answering questionnaires (even if those are read to them). It has become very common during the pasts decades to use behavioural measures of hand preference (e.g., Bryden, Pryde, & Roy, 2000; Bishop, Ross, Daniel, & Bright, 1996) or observation of hand preference on a number of tasks (Porac & Coren, 1981). These tasks are comprised of 10-12 preference measures (to assess degree of hand preference), which are examined twice (to assess hand consistency) and handedness is usually evaluated on a three point scale of preference; left, right, mixed. However, studies have used the demonstration of hand preference based on the items of an inventory and a five-point scale has been used classifying individuals as strongly left, weak left, ambitexter, weak right, strongly right (Van Strein, Lagers, van Haselen, van Hagen, de Coo, Frens, & van der Geest, 2005). An alternative example of such a task is the WatHand Box Test (Bryden, Pryde, & Roy, 2000), which assesses direction and consistency of hand preference using a variety of unimanual tasks (e.g., lifting a cupboard door, using a toy hammer, placing rings on hooks and tossing a ball). In addition, Bishop’s card reaching task (Bishop, Ross, Daniel, & Bright, 1996) that provides a measure of the degree and the direction of hand preference has commonly been used in individuals with neurodevelopmental disorders (see Desplanches, Deruelle, Stefanini, Ayoun, Volterra, Vicari et al, 2006).

Performance measures of handedness are used less often to assess the relative proficiency of on hand over the other in individuals with neurodevelopmental disorders. Tasks that have commonly been utilized to assess hand skill are finger tapping (Elliott, Edwards, Weeks, Lindley, & Carnahan, 1987; Elliott, Weeks, & Jones 1986) and the pegboard (e.g., Cornish & McManus, 1996; Cornish, Pigram, & Paw, 1997).

Other laterality indexes, such as ear, eye and foot, are also assessed both as preference and as performance. For example, foot preference can be assessed using a questionnaire or using a demonstration of foot preference across a number of tasks (e.g., Porac & Coren, 1981). Moreover, foot performance can also be examined by assessing the relative proficiency of one foot over the other. Up until now, no study that we know has specifically addressed relative foot performance in individuals with DS.

Advertisement

3. Atypical laterality in individuals with Down syndrome (Dichotic listening studies)

In dichotic listening studies the participants selectively attend one of the two messages presented simultaneously in both ears indicating a left or right ear advantage for linguistic material. Most evidence agrees that right-handed individuals with DS exhibit a unique pattern of ear dominance that is syndrome-specific and cannot be attributed to the mental retardation per se (Heath & Elliot, 1999). Support for this dissociation in ear preference comes from various studies assessing individual with DS, individuals with mental retardation (non-DS) and typically developing participants (e.g., Hartley, 1981; Pipe, 1983; Elliot & Weeks, 1993; Heath & Elliot, 1999; Giencke & Lewandowski, 1989). There is increased evidence for left ear/right hemisphere dominance for language in right-handed individuals with DS, which is indicative of a reversed cerebral specialization for speech perception (see Elliot, Weeks & Chua, 1994 for a meta-analysis). This reversed pattern has been linked to the poor linguistic abilities of these individuals although dissociation between laterality for speech perception and speech production that involves oral motor systems has also been suggested (Elliot, Weeks, & Elliot, 1987; Giencke & Lewandowski, 1989; Heath & Elliot, 1999). During the past decade, studies explored the issue of the dissociation of lateralized systems for speech perception and speech production in individuals with DS using a verbal-motor task that tapped interhemispheric integration (Welsh, Elliot, & Simon, 2003). Their results supported their model of functional dissociation between perception and oral-motor production for speech stimuli that are typically supported by the same cerebral hemisphere in typically developing individuals. Moreover, this atypical pattern of cerebral specialization is specific to DS and is not observed in other populations with mental retardation (non-DS) of unknown etiology.

Unlike typically developing individuals, DS people exhibit right hemisphere lateralization for receptive language and a left hemisphere lateralization for the production of simple and complex movement. This separation of speech perception and motor movement in addition to the morphological callosal deficiencies (causing poor intrahemispheric communication) may be responsible for the verbal difficulties of DS individuals (Heath, Grierson, Binsted, & Elliott, 2007).

Pipe (1983) used dichotic listening tasks to assess language laterality in young children with DS, individuals with mental retardation (non-DS) and typically developing individuals. Their results indicated an atypical left-ear right-hemisphere advantage for speech stimuli in individuals with DS a pattern that was only observed in this clinical group. Non-DS individuals with mental retardation exhibited a right-ear left-hemisphere advantage for speech stimuli a pattern that was similar to typically developing individuals. In accordance with With Elliott, Edwards, Weeks, Lindley and Carnahan’s (1987) study, Pipe (1983) observed the unique pattern of ear preference in individuals with DS, which seems to be expressed over and above the degree of mental retardation and may be described as syndrome-specific. It should be noted here that most researchers (e.g., Pipe, 1983; Elliott, Edwards, Weeks, Lindley, & Carnahan, 1987 Heath & Elliot, 1999) have linked this unique pattern of cerebral laterality for language in individuals with DS with the weak linguistic abilities that they exhibit. However, further research assessing different clinical syndromes that also exhibit linguistic deficits (e.g., Williams syndrome) using dichotic listening tasks is needed to support this hypothesis.

On the other hand, Paquette, Bourassa and Peretz (1996) documented a left ear advantage in individuals with ID of unknown etiology. Their results indicated a left ear/ right hemisphere advantage for speech stimuli in both impaired groups and the opposite pattern in typically developing individuals. This pattern of ear preference supports the notion of atypical cerebral laterality in individuals with mental retardation as a consequence of the early brain damage that affects intellectual functioning and cerebral specialization.

The importance of studies using non-invasive techniques, such as dichotic listening and handedness, to assess cerebral laterality in individuals with mental retardation is of vast importance. Firstly, non-invasive measures are easy and safe to administer to such populations and produce significant information to researchers in this field. Secondly, such studies provide insight into the functioning of the brain and its lateralization. They also provide evidence for the representation of cognitive systems within the brain. For example, it may be suggested that the brains of individuals with DS may represent processing centers bilaterally causing a delay in the production of relevant cognitive and motor material. In addition, by combining neuroimaging with behavioral laterality techniques one can infer that certain brain areas are predominately involved in specific processes, while other areas are unable to execute their intended function. For instance, perhaps the weak collaboration of the two hemispheres is due to the thinner corpus callosum in individuals with DS (Wang, Doherti, Hesselink, & Bellugi, 1992) that may cause the isolation of the functions of the hemispheres enhancing weak intra-hemispheric integration at least for verbal-motor stimuli (Welsh, Elliot, & Simon, 2003).

Advertisement

4. Atypical laterality and Down syndrome (handedness studies)

Ear preference using dichotic listening tasks indicates a syndrome- specific pattern of cerebral laterality in individuals with DS. This pattern can perhaps be documented using other laterality indexes, such as hand, foot and eye preference. To date there has not been a study assessing individuals with DS on various laterality indexes using preference and performance measures and controlling for the effect of age, gender and degree of mental retardation. Such studies are currently been undertaken in our laboratory to assist further in the understanding of atypical laterality in individuals with DS.

However, handedness studies in individuals with DS have been reported since the 70’s. Pickersgill and Pank (1970) assessed the prevalence of left handedness in individuals with DS, individuals with mental retardation non-DS and typically developing individuals. They found a higher prevalence of left-handedness in individuals with mental retardation non-DS compared to individuals with DS and typically developing adults. More specifically, the prevalence of left-handedness in typically developing individuals in their sample was 15.6% and that of individuals with DS 18.7%, while individuals with mental retardation non-DS exhibited an almost twofold increased prevalence of left-handedness (31%).

In a later study, Batheja and Mc Manus (1985) explored the prevalence of left-handedness in individuals with DS, individuals with mental retardation (non-DS) and typically developing Individuals, matched for age, and found no difference between the two clinical groups (DS=27% left-handers, non-DS= 29% left-handers), although there was a marked difference in the non-clinical groups (age matched controls=11% left-handers).

In a similar study, Pipe (1987) assessed hand preference in individuals with DS, individuals with mental retardation non-DS and age-matched controls including her families to determine whether familial sinistrality is documented in these populations. Their results indicated that the two clinical groups, regardless of their etiology (DS or non-DS) exhibited 35-36% of non-right handedness (i.e., left and mixed handedness) and increased familial sinistrality compared to the non-clinical population. The authors explained that the increased prevalence of mixed handedness and familial sinistrality in individuals with mental retardation couldn’t support Satz’s (1973) model of pathological left-handedness. If non-right handedness is caused by early brain insult, as the model suggests, then there should not be an increased prevalence of familial sinistrality in these populations. Rather as Batheja and McManus (1985) suggested non-right handedness may be the result of any biological disturbance causing variability in cerebral asymmetry. Alternatively, specific hormones such as testosterone, delays the development of left-hemisphere functions resulting in increased prevalence of non-right handedness in clinical populations.

Lewin, Kohen and Mathew (1993) investigated handedness in individuals with DS, epilepsy and autism. Their results indicated a significantly increased prevalence of non-right handers in all three populations with no differences between the three groups and no differences associated with the level of mental retardation as reported elsewhere (e.g., Hicks & Barton, 1975). It was proposed that the theory of left-handedness (Satz, 1972) may explain the increased incidence of non-right handers in individuals with epilepsy in which focal brain damage may be assumed, however, it may not hold true for individuals with DS or autism. The theory of increased randomness (Palmer, 1964) may explain this pattern in individuals with learning disabilities, since the arrested development of the nervous system may lead to the undifferentiation of the two hemispheres documented by the increased prevalence of non-right handers in these populations. Table 1 below presents research using laterality indexes in DS and ID.

Findings from our laboratory confirm the existence of an atypical pattern of handedness preference in individuals with DS (n=50) and ID (n=50), compared to typically developing (TD) individuals (n=100) (Ypsilanti, 2009) (Figure 1). Specifically, our results demonstrate no significant differences between DS and ID individuals with similar level of intellectual functioning (mean IQ=43). However, they indicate statistically significant differences between both clinical groups and TD individuals (χ2= 46.86, d.f.=2, p<0.01).

In reviewing studies of atypical laterality in individuals with DS, compared to individuals with ID (non-DS) and typically developing individuals, two conclusions can be drawn. Firstly, in the existing literature there seems to be inconsistent findings even when similar methodologies are employed. For example, Pickersgill and Pank (1970) found no significant differences in laterality in individuals with DS and typically developing individuals, while other studies have found such differences consistently (e.g., Batheja & McManus, 1985; Pipe, 1987). The reason for this discrepancy may be linked to various laterality measures that have been used to assess hand preference in individuals with neurodevelopmental disorders as well as the different age groups that have been selected in each case. Moreover, differences in the degree of mental retardation may have interfered with the results of different studies. Secondly, few studies have taken into account the fact that individuals with DS do not exhibit focal brain lesion during fetal development, which has converted them from natural right-handers to pathological left-handers as in the cases of individuals with focal brain injury in the left hemisphere (Satz, 1972). As Batheja and McManus (1985) proposed it is more likely that the difference in the prevalence of hand preference may be due to “… any form of biological noise” (pp. 66) (Batheja & McManus, 1985) that disrupts the development of typical asymmetry in these individuals at its genesis.

Study no. Reference Participants N1 Preference/performance Indices Results
1 Gordon (1921) ID Preference Hand LH 18%
LH 7%
2 Merphy (1962) ID, DS 64 Preference Hand LH 31% of ID
LH 13% of DS
3 Lenneberg, Nickols and Rosenberger (1964) DS 61 Preference Hand M 42.6%
4 Clausen (1966) ID 276 Preference Hand LH 17%
5 Rengstroff (1967) ID 395 Preference Hand, eye 81.8% RH 18.2 LH - 48.5% RE 51.4 LE%
6 Peckersgill & Pank (1970) ID, DS 32 Performance Hand LH 18% in Ds
LH 31% in ID
7 Hicks & Barton (1975) ID 550 Preference Hand LH 20.7% *
Mild & Moderate: (13%)
Severe & Profound: (28%)
8 Silva & Satz (1979) ID 1409 Performance Hand LH 15.5
M 12.7%
9 Porac, Coren, & Duncan (1980) ID 128 Preference Hand, eye, ear, foot LH 15.9
M 44.2%
10 Burns & Zeaman (1980) ID 20 Preference Hand, eye, ear, foot Hand is more lateralized than foot, ear, eye in both groups.
11 Hartley (1981) ID, DS Performance Ear LEA in DS
12 McManus (1983) ID 68 Preference (mother’s report) Hand LH 13.2
13 Pipe (1983) ID, DS Performance Ear LEA in DS
14 Bradshaw, Hick & Kinsbourne (1984) ID 232 Performance Hand More LH
15 Elliot, D (1985) ID DS 38 Preference/ performance Hand
16 Batheja & McManus (1985) ID, DS 130 Performance Hand LH 27%
LH 29%
LH 11%
17 Elliott, Weeks & Jones (1986). DS Performance Hand DS same asymmetry on finger-tapping
18 Searleman, Cunningham & Goodwin (1987) ID 90 Preference/ performance Hand LH 17.8
M 5.6
19 Soper et al., (1987) ID 73 Preference
Hand LH 9.6%, M 45.2%, RH 45.2%
20 Pipe (1987) ID, DS 318 Preference Hand M 35%*
LRH 36%*
LRH 18%
21 Elliot et al. (1987) DS 12 Preference/ performance Hand
22 Lucas et al., (1989)
ID 238 Preference
Hand LH 17.4% mild
LH 28.0% severe
23 Morris & Romski, (1993) ID 50 Preference Hand LH 19%, LRH 32%, RH 49%
24 Paquatte et al (1996) ID 16 Performance
Ear LEA in ID
25 Mandal et al (1998) ID 50 Preference Hand Mixed handedness.
26 Grouios et al. (1999) ID 73 Preference Hand LH 17.8%, LRH 38.4%, RH 43.8%
LH 9.6%, LRH 4.1%, RH 86.3%
27 Vlachos & Karapetsas, (1999) DS 41 Preference Hand LH & LRH in DS
28 Heath & Elliot (1999) DS 10 Performance Ear
29 Carlier et al., (2006) DS, WS 79 Preference
30 Leconte and Fagard (2006) ID 30 Preference
Hand Eye Foot Crossed Eye-hand
31 Desplanches et al, 2006 Preference
32 Mulvey, Ringenbach & Jung, 2011 DS 25 Preference & perfromance Hand Reduced hand asymmetry in bimanual coordination
33 Carlier et al., 2011 DS, WS, DiGeorge syndrome Preference Hand Eye, Ear & Foot Increased mixed handedness and footedness in all groups, related to degree of ID.

Table 1.

Laterality indices in ID and DS.

1Sorted by year of study



Figure 1.

Frequencies of right and non-right handed individuals with DS, ID and TD.

Advertisement

5. Theoretical explanations of atypical laterality

Several accounts have been put forward to explain the increased incidence of atypical laterality in individuals with ID. It has been suggested that theories on atypical laterality fall into two categories; namely, pathological and natural (Satz, 1973). However, for the purposes of clarity this discrimination will not be adapted in the present paper. Rather a detailed analysis of all the theories will be presented including those that are scarcely discussed in the literature.

One of the most prominent theories has been put forward by Geschwind and Galaburda (1987), who implicated the levels of testosterone in the development of atypical laterality. According to the theory, several genetic factors, such as chromosomes and antigens, as well as environmental factors that affect fetal development, like the endocrine environment and the cyclic variation, alter the levels of testosterone to the fetus. This effect is directly linked to both the delayed growth of the left hemisphere and the increased growth of the right hemisphere particularly in the posterior regions. The decreased growth of the left hemisphere has been linked to mental retardation and poor verbal ability, which are some of the characteristics of individuals with neurodevelopmental disorders. In essence, the model predicts that the increased levels of testosterone will have an impact on the development of the left hemisphere, causing reduced language and visual-spatial dominance. Therefore, individuals with this condition will exhibit increased left and mixed handedness compared to the normal population. In support of their theory, Geschiwind and Galaburda (1985a, 1985b, 1985c) presented a series of studies associating atypical laterality (or “anomalous dominance”) with developmental learning disorders, autism and immune disorders.

Although Geschwind’s and Galaburda’s (1987) theory has been considered one of the most prominent theories in the field of cognitive neuropsychology, it has been strongly criticized for its complexity and its arbitrary predictions (e.g., McManus & Bryden, 1991; McManus, Bryden, & Bulman-Fleming, 1994; Annett, 1994; Previc, 1994). Bryden McManus and Bulman-Fleming (1994) suggest that the relationship between language dominance and handedness, as discussed by Geschwind and Galaburda (1987), is weak and the conclusions drawn based on this assumption are poorly supported by empirical findings. Moreover, the predictions made by Geshwind and Galaburda (1987) are farfetched and the experimental data cannot support the numerous associations that are predicted by theory. On the other hand, the theory, although long and complex, contributed greatly to the understanding of the biological factors (i.e., hormones) that may be linked to atypical laterality and triggered a large number of studies in atypical laterality and neurodevelopmental disorders.

Genetic theories have also been put forward to explain atypical laterality (Annett & Alexander, 1996; Bryden & McManus, 1985). The main focus of these genetic theories was to explain the origin of left and right-handedness in normal populations (Annett 1972, 1985). More specifically, Annett’s (1972) theory, referred to as “right-shift theory”, explained the exhibition of right and left handedness as the outcome of left hemisphere speech induced by a single gene. In the case of atypical handedness Annett (1994) suggested that atypical developmental effects could trigger randomness in the absence of the right-shift gene and inhibit the “natural” cerebral asymmetry that is observed in typical development. Moreover, individuals lacking the gene for right hemisphere speech (rs+ gene) are at risk for various difficulties that affect language expression and phonology such as dyslexia. In other words, Annett (1985) proposed that atypical laterality may be a “... natural variation in cerebral asymmetry” (pp. 241) triggered by the absence of the right-shift gene (Annett, 1994).

Previc (1991) postulated that cerebral asymmetry derives from the asymmetric development of the vestibular system (left ear dominance in approximately 70% of the population), which is established during prenatal life and is directly linked to the postural position of the fetus and the pattern of maternal movements during the final trimester of the pregnancy. Moreover, the anatomy of the female uterus induces fetuses in the final trimester of pregnancy to be positioned “… with their head to the left side of the mother’s midline and their right ear facing outward” (pp. 301) (Previc, 1991). This postural asymmetry of the fetus and the mother favours a sinistral vestibular dominance at birth, which is documented by the dextral lie preference of newborns and is correlated with the development of right hand preference later in life. The asymmetrical development of the two vestibular organs, the ear and the labyrinth, may be responsible for the asymmetry of the left and right hemisphere and the difference in ear preference documented in the literature using dichotic listening tasks (e.g., Heath & Elliott, 1999). Previc (1991) proposed a link between poor motoric lateralization (i.e., mixed or left handedness), the vestibular system and neurodevelopmental disorders that are associated with vestibular dysfunction; namely autism, dyslexia and deafness. In essence, Previc’s (1991) theory predicted increased percentages of non-right handedness in these disorders, in addition to other neurodevelopmental disorders, that exhibit abnormalities in the brain stem, the basal ganglia, the cerebellum and the temporal lobes, since these systems are directly affected or affect the vestibular system. Also, increased percentages of poor motoric dominance (i.e., non-right handedness) are likely to exist in pre-term infants, since they have not been exposed long enough to the right face position allowing for right handedness to be established. Previc’s (1991) theory initiated a new era in the research of human laterality. The presence of prenatal factors that affect and essentially define motoric dominance in humans in combination with genetic, environmental and cultural theories could provide an important framework for the development of a stronger and more inclusive theory that encompasses strengths of all other theories.

An alternative model attempting to explain the increased incidence of atypical laterality in individuals with neurodevelopmental disorders is the theory of pathological left-handedness (Satz, 1973) According to this account, there is a subgroup of left-handed individuals which are described as pathological left handers. This subgroup was genetically natural right-handers, but suffered early brain insult to the left hemisphere causing a mild dysfunction of the contralateral hand for motor movements. The result of this dysfunction was a switch of hand dominance to the other hand (i.e., left hand) to perform complex motor tasks. Therefore, although these individuals were genetically programmed to become right-handers having left hemisphere dominance for language an early brain insult (before the age of six) caused a switch hand preference making them pathological left-handers. This subgroup is differentiated for natural left-handers who have no history of brain insult early in development and are naturally born with left hand dominance. In addition, the model describes a subgroup of pathological right-handers who were natural left-handers but an early brain injury in the right hemisphere caused them to switch hand preference to the opposite hand, thus becoming pathological right-handers. The account of pathological left-handedness can predict the increased incidence of left-handers in populations with ID and epilepsy, since both groups seem to have brain abnormalities exhibited early in development. Therefore, within a population of individuals with mental retardation, there will be an 8% of natural left-handers, as in the typical population, and approximately another 8-9% who are pathological left-handers. This model would explain the almost twofold percentage of manifest left-handers in individual with mental retardation.

Several studies have provided evidence for the model of pathological left-handedness, since the initial account was put forward (Satz, 1973). However, the theory has been tested in cross cultural studies (Satz, Baymure, & Van der Vlugt, 1979), in studies using EEG recordings (Silva & Satz, 1979), in studies with individuals with left or right congenital hemiplegia (Carlsson, Hugdahl, Uvenbrant, Wiklund, & Von Wentd, 1992), in relation to familial sinistrality (Orsini, Satz, Soper, & Light, 1985; Pipe 1987) and degree of ID (Bradshaw-McAnulty, Hicks, & Kinsbourne, 1984) and has been termed the pathological left handedness syndrome (Satz, Orsini, Saslow, & Henry, 1985). Since the original study (Satz, 1973) Soper and Satz (1984) incorporated one more type of pathological handedness in their model, termed ambiguous handedness, to explain the increased incidence of mixed handedness in individuals with early brain insult. The new explanatory model predicted increased incidence of ambiguous handedness in the more severe groups with neurodevelopmental disorders, such as infantile autism and severe ID (Soper & Satz, 1984), which has also been reported elsewhere (e.g., Tsai, 1982).

Although the above-mentioned theories contribute to the understanding of the increased incidence of non-right handers in individuals with neurodevelopmental disorders, the evidence for this link is far from conclusive. Satz’s (1973) theory of pathological left handedness could account for the increased incidence of left handers in individual with focal brain injury, but in clinical populations with defuse brain damage (e.g., DS, Williams syndrome) and lack of hand preference (i.e., increased mixed handedness) the theory seems inadequate. Particularly in individuals with ID, it has long been recognized that ambiguous handedness rather than left-handedness is most commonly observed (e.g., Porac, Coren, Steiger, & Duncan, 1980). This lack of handedness would be documented by random hand preference in preference measures.

Palmer (1964) termed this observation “increased randomness” referring to the increased ambiguous hand preference in individuals with mental retardation. In particular, he postulated that handedness is a developmental process and could be utilized as an index of typical motor development. This developmental process progresses from a bilateral undifferentiated state early in infancy to a unilateral state that is viewed as a “… differentiation from a whole” (pp. 258) (Palmer, 1964), since it initiates from the trunks before the shoulders and then the hands. Therefore, Palmer (1964) proposed a maturational process that is linked to typical cerebral laterality and one-sidedness. If this maturational process is arrested or lagged it could cause increased randomness, which would be documented by lack of hand preference (i.e., ambiguous handedness). One of the main conclusions that could be drawn from Palmer’s (1964) theory is that mixed and left-handedness has long been considered differentiated states and should be studied separately. Particularly in populations with neurodevelopmental disorders, “lack of hand preference” (i.e., mixed handedness) may be a more significant indicator of atypical cerebral laterality than left-handedness.

Along this vein, Bishop (1983, 1990) postulated that non right-handedness is an indicator of an immature development of the motor system, caused by diffuse brain abnormalities in individuals with mental retardation. In contrast to Satz’s (1973) theory and other genetic theories, Bishop (1990) suggests that differentiated hand preference indicates mature motor development. According to Bishop (1990), studies assessing hand preference in individuals with mental retardation should utilize a control group matched for motor development rather than chronological or mental age. The question remains whether there is correspondence between motor and mental age and whether measuring motor age when assessing handedness can further contribute to the existing literature. To our knowledge, there are no published data on of handedness in neurodevelopmental disorders that utilises a control group matched for motor age. On the other hand, mental age as assessed using the WISC III (Wechsler, 1992) may also be problematic because the verbal subtests of the WISC III (Wechsler, 1992) may undermine the motor development of an individual with mental retardation. The link between mental retardation and motor retardation has not been widely investigated. Perhaps using the performance subscales of the WISC III (Wechsler, 1992), or another measure of non-verbal intelligence (e.g., Raven, 1985), would be more appropriate for matching control groups. Further, research in the area of motor development and the assessment of handedness in relation to motor age are needed to clarify the issue.

A link has also been postulated between literacy and handedness, suggesting that cerebral organization may change as a result of schooling and literacy, although the evidence for this link is contradictory (Tzavaras, Kaprinis, & Gatzouas, 1981). In controversy with genetic theories, this approach suggests that literacy reinforces the left hemisphere dominance for language. According to the theory, there should be an increased number of individuals with atypical laterality among illiterate populations exhibiting right or bilateral dominance for language. Tzavaras, Kaprinis and Gatzouas (1981) examined this possibility using the dichotic listening technique as a measure of language dominance in an illiterate population and found an increased left-right ear difference in the illiterate population compared to the literate individuals. The authors suggested that this difference might be due to the poor strategic techniques used by illiterate subjects, which do not enhance bi-hemispheric participation for speech as in the educated brain. However, it has been found that aphasia is less severe and more provisional in illiterate patients suggesting a right hemisphere involvement of language in these individuals (Lecours, Mehler, Parente, Behrami, Tolipan, Cary, et al, 1988; Cameron, Currier, & Haerer, 1971). Castro-Caldas, Reis and Geurreiro (1997) in a review on literacy and laterality concluded that the empirical findings of studies from aphasic patients and dichotic listening tasks are inconclusive about the link between atypical laterality and literacy and further research is need to clarify this postulation. To the authors’ knowledge, no studies have been reported linking the observed atypical laterality of individuals with neurodevelopmental disorders with literacy and schooling. However, a number of researchers propose that lateral preferences may be affected by the type of task used and may be related to the level of experience and practice that a group of individuals have (e.g., Bishop, 1983). If one accepts this notion, it is probable that individuals with mental retardation are less skilled than typically developing individuals with objects like pencils, scissors and playing cards, which are commonly used to assess hand preference in these populations. In this case, inconsistent hand preference when manipulating such objects may be affected by the immature behaviour exhibited by these individuals due to decreased experience. More specifically, the effect of limited schooling and skilfulness in individuals with mental retardation may have an indirect impact on lateral preferences particularly when the preference measures presented are school-related utilities.

Another line of research suggests that individuals with DS exhibit atypical neural activation in left/right hemisphere regions compared to typically developing individuals (Jacola, Byars, Chalfonte-Evans, Schmithorst, Hickey, Patterson, et al., 2011). In an fMRI study with 13 DS individuals, there was a positive association between visual-spatial ability and occipito-parietal and dorso-frontal activation exclusively in individuals with DS compared to control counterparts.

Advertisement

6. Epilogue

Research in laterality in individuals with DS has been fruitful. Findings from dichotic listening studies suggest that individuals with DS exhibit a unique pattern of lateralization of language, which is syndrome specific. Specifically, it has been repeatedly supported that there is a left-ear, right-hemisphere advantage for speech stimuli, unlike that observed in typical populations or individuals with ID of other aetiologies. Moreover, handedness studies demonstrate that lateralization of language may be pathological with increased incidence of left-handedness, left- footedness, left-eyedness and cross eye-hand preferences. Several theories have been put forward to explain this atypicality, including, hormonal, structural and neural anomalies related to the syndrome. This atypical pattern of functional lateralization, most likely contributes to the linguistic difficulties observed in individuals with DS, which are rather permanent. At the same time, limited educational and motor training leaves little space for improvement in linguistic and motor efficiency in individuals with DS. Other developmental milestones that are fundamentally delayed in individuals with DS obstruct the developmental transition from an undifferentiated state to a lateralized state.

References

  1. 1. Annett, M. & Alexander, M. P. (1996). Atypical cerebral dominance: Predictions and tests of the right shift theory. Neuropsychologia, 34, 1215-1227.
  2. 2. Annett, M. (1972). The distribution of manual asymmetry. British Journal of Psychology, 65, 343-358.
  3. 3. Annett, M. (1972). The distribution of manual asymmetry. British Journal of Psychology, 63, 343-358.
  4. 4. Annett, M. (1985). Left, right hand and brain: The right shift theory. London: Erlbaum.
  5. 5. Annett, M. (1994). Commentary Geschwind’s legacy. Brain and Cognition, 26, 236-242.
  6. 6. Annett, M., Hudson, P. T. W., & Turner, A. (1974). The reliability of differences between the hands in motor skill. Neuropsychologia, 12, 527-531.
  7. 7. Azari, N. P., Horwitz, B., Pettigrew, K. D., Grady, C. L., Haxby, J. V., Giacometti, K. R., & Schapiro, M. B. (1994). Abnormal pattern of cerebral glucose metabolic rates involving language areas in young adults with Down syndrome. Brain and Language, 46, 1-20.
  8. 8. Aziz-Zadeh, L., Koski, L., Zaidel, E., Mazziotta, J., & Iacoboni, M. (2006). Lateralization of the human mirror neuron system. The Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 2964-2970.
  9. 9. Banich, M. T. (2003). The divided visual field technique in laterality and interhemispheric integration. In K. Hugdahl (ed.), Experimental methods in neuropsychology (pp. 47-63). New York, NY: Kluwer Publishers.
  10. 10. Batheja, M. & McManus, I.C. (1985). Handedness in the mentally handicapped. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 27, 63-68.
  11. 11. Belin, C., Jullien, S., Perrier, D., & Larmande, P. (1990). La tachistoscopie: une méthode expérimentale d’étude de la specialisation hémisphérique. Journal français d'ophtalmologie, 13, 293-297.
  12. 12. Benke, T., Koylu, B., Visani, P., Karner, E., Brenneis, C., Bartha, L., Trinka, E., Trieb, T., Felber, S., Bauer,G., Chemelli, A., & Willmes, K. (2006). Language lateralization in temporal lobe epilepsy: a comparison between fMRI and Wada Test. Epilepsia, 47, 1308-1319.
  13. 13. Benton, A. L., Harvey, S. L., & Varney, N. R. (1973). Tactile perception of direction in normal subjects. Neurology, 23, 1248-1250.
  14. 14. Best, C. T. (1988). The emergence of cerebral asymmetries in early human development: A literature review and a neuroembryological model. In D. L. Molfese and S. J. Segalowitz (eds.), Brain lateralization in children. Developmental implications (pp. 5-34). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
  15. 15. Bishop, D. V. M. (1990). Handedness and developmental disorders. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
  16. 16. Bishop, D. V. M., Ross, V., Daniels, M. S., & Bright, P. (1996). The measurement of hand preference. A validation study comparing three groups of right-handers. British Journal of Psychology, 87, 269–285.
  17. 17. Bourne, V. J. (2006). The divided visual filed paradigm: methodological considerations. Laterality, 11, 373-393.
  18. 18. Bowler, D. M., Cufflin, J., & Kiernan, C. (1985). Dichotic listening of verbal and nonverbal material by Down syndrome children and children of normal intelligence. Cortex, 21, 637-644.
  19. 19. Bradshaw- McAnulty, G., Hicks, R., & Kinsbourne, M. (1984). Pathological left-handedness and familial sinistrality in relation to degree of mental retardation. Brain and Cognition, 3, 349-356.
  20. 20. Briggs, G. G. & Nebes, R. D. (1975) Patterns of hand preference in a student population. Cortex 11, 230-238.
  21. 21. Bryden, M. P., McManus, I. C., & Bulman-Fleming, M. B. (1994). Evaluating the empirical support for the Geschwind-Behan-Galaburda model of cerebral lateralization. Brain and Cognition, 26, 103-167.
  22. 22. Bryden, P. J., Pryde, K. M., & Roy, E. A. (2000). A performance measure of the degree of hand preference. Brain and Cognition, 44, 402–414.
  23. 23. Bunn, L., Welsh, T. N., Simon, D. A., Howarth, K., & Elliott, D. (2003). Dichotic ear advantages in adults with Down's syndrome predict speech production errors. Neuropsychology, 17, 32-38.
  24. 24. Burns, B., & Zeaman, D. (1980). A comparison of laterality indices in college and retarded subjects. Journal of Psychology, 104, 241-247.
  25. 25. Camekon, R. F., Currier, R. D., & Haeker, A. F. (1971). Aphasia and literacy. British Journal of Disorders of Communication, 6, 161-163.
  26. 26. Carlier, M., Desplanches, A. G., Philip, N., Stefanini, S., Vicari, S., Volterra, V., Deruelle, C., Fisch, G., Doyen, A. L., Swillen, A. (2011). Laterality preference and cognition: cross-syndrome comparison of patients with trisomy 21 (Down), del7q11.23 (Williams-Beuren) and del22q11.2 (DiGeorge or Velo-Cardio-Facial) syndromes. Behavioral Genetics, 41, 413-422.
  27. 27. Carlier, M., Stefanini, S., Deruelle, C., Volterra, V., Doyen, A., Lamard, C., dePortzamparc, V., Vicari, S., & Fisch, G. (2006). Laterality in persons with intellectual disability. I-Do patients with trisomy 21 and Williams-Beuren syndrome differ from typically developing persons? Behaviour Genetics, 36, 365-376.
  28. 28. Carlsson, R., Hugdahl, K., Uvebrant, P., Wiklund, L.M., & VonWendt, L. (1992). Pathological left-handeness revisited: Dichotic listening in children with left vs right congenital hemiplegia. Neuropsychologia, 30, 471-481.
  29. 29. Castro-Caldas, A., Reis, A., & Guerreiro. M. (1997). Neuropsychological aspects of illiteracy. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 7, 327–338.
  30. 30. Cherry, E. C. (1953). Some experiments on the recognition of speech, with one and two ears. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 25, 975–979.
  31. 31. Chua, R., Weeks, D. J, & Elliott, D. (1996). A functional systems approach to understanding verbal-motor integration in individuals with Down syndrome. Down Syndrome Research and Practice, 4, 25-36.
  32. 32. Clausen, J. (1966). Ability, structure and subgroups in mental retardation. Washington DC: Spartan.
  33. 33. Cohen, A., R. & Sweet, H. L. (2011). Brain imaging in behavioral medicine and clinical neuroscience. In R. A. Cohen and L. H. Sweet (eds), Brain imaging in behavioral medicine and clinical neuroscience: an introduction (pp. 1-9). New York: Springer Science.
  34. 34. Corballis, M. C. (1989). Laterality and human evolution. Psychological Review, 96, 492-505.
  35. 35. Cornish, K. M., & McManus, I. C. (1996). Hand preference and hand skill in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26, 597-609.
  36. 36. Cornish, K. M., Pigram, J., & Shaw, K. (1997). Do anomalies of handedness with frazile -X. Laterality, 2, 91-101.
  37. 37. Cowell, P. & Hugdahl, K. (2000). Individual differences in neurobehavioral measures of laterality and interhemispheric function as measured by dichotic listening studies. Developmental Neuropsychology, 18, 95-112.
  38. 38. Dalby, J. T. & Gibson, D. (1981). Functional cerebral lateralization in subtypes of disabled readers. Brain and Language, 14, 34-48.
  39. 39. Day, J. (1979). Visual half field word recognition as a function of syntactic class and imageability. Neuropsychologia, 17, 515-519.
  40. 40. De Agostini, M. & Dellatolas, G. (2001). Lateralities in normal children ages 3 to 8 and their role in cognitive performances. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20, 429-444.
  41. 41. Desmond, J. E., Sum, M., Wagner, A. D., Demb, J. B., Shear, P. K., lover, G. H., Gabrieli, J. D., & Morrell, M. J. (1995). Functional MRI measurement of language lateralization in Wada-tested patients. Brain, 118, 1411-1419
  42. 42. Desplanches, A., Derualle, C., Stefanini, S., Ayoun, C., Volterra, V., Fisch, G., Carlier, M. (2006). Laterality in persons with intellectual disability II. Hand, foot, ear and eye laterality in persons with trisomy 21 and Williams-Beuren syndrome. Developmental Psychobiology, 48, 482-491.
  43. 43. Dittmar, M. (2002). Functional and postural lateral preferences in humans: Interrelations and life-span age differences. Human Biology, 74, 569-585.
  44. 44. Dodds, A. G. (1978). Hemispheric differences in tactuo-spatial processing. Neuropsychologia, 16, 247-254.
  45. 45. Elliott, D., & Weeks, D. J. (1993). Cerebral specialization for speech perception and movement organization in adults with Down’s syndrome. Cortex, 29, 103–113.
  46. 46. Elliott, D., Edwards, J. M., Weeks, D. J., Lindley, S., & Carnahan, H. (1987). Cerebral specialization in young adults with Down syndrome. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 91, 480-485
  47. 47. Elliott, D., Pollock, B., Chua, R., & Weeks, D J. (1995). Cerebral specialization for spatial processing in adults with Down syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 99, 605-615.
  48. 48. Elliott, D., Weeks, D. J., & Chua, R. (1994). Anomalous cerebral lateralization and Down syndrome. Brain and Cognition, 26, 191–195.
  49. 49. Elliott, D., Weeks, D. J., & Elliott, C. L. (1987). Cerebral specialization in individuals with Down syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 92, 263–271.
  50. 50. Elliott, D., Weeks, D. J., & Jones, R. (1986). Lateral asymmetries in finger-tapping by adolescents and young adults with Down syndrome. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 90, 472-475.
  51. 51. Ellis, H. D., & Shepard, J. W. (1974). Recognition of abstract and concrete words presented in left and right visual fields. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 103, 1035-1036.
  52. 52. Faurion, A., Cerf, B., Van De Moortele, P. F., Lobel, E., MacLeod, P., & Le Bihan, D. (1999). Human taste cortical areas studied with functional magnetic resonance imaging: evidence of functional lateralization related to handedness. Neuroscience Letters, 277, 189-192.
  53. 53. Foundas, A. L., Corey, D. M., Angeles, V., Bollich, A. M., Crabtree-Hartman, E., & Heilman, K. M. (2003). Atypical cerebral laterality in adults with persistent developmental stuttering. Neurology, 61, 1378-1385.
  54. 54. Frangou, S., Aylward, E., Warren, A., Sharma, T., Barta, P., & Pearlson, G. (1997). Small planum temporale volume in Down's syndrome: A volumetric MRI study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 1424-1429.
  55. 55. Ganou, M., & Grouios, G. (2008). Cerebral laterality in Turner syndrome: a critical review of the literature. Child Neuropsychology, 14, 135-147.
  56. 56. Ganou, M., Grouios, G., Koidou, I., & Alevriadou, A. (2010). The concept of abnormal cerebral lateralization in Klinefelter syndrome. Applied Neuropsychology, 17, 144-152.
  57. 57. Ganou, M., Kollias, N., Koidou, I., & Grouios, G. (2012). Atypical cerebral laterality in autism: a review of current research. (Submitted for publication).
  58. 58. Geschwind, N. & Behan, E. (1982). Left-handedness: Association with immune disease, migraine, and developmental learning disorder. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 79, 5097-5100.
  59. 59. Geschwind, N. & Galaburda, A. M. (1985a). Cerebral lateralization: Biological mechanisms, associations, and pathology: I. A hypothesis and a program for research. Archives of Neurology, 42, 428-459.
  60. 60. Geschwind, N. & Galaburda, A. M. (1985b). Cerebral lateralization: Biological mechanisms, associations, and pathology: II. A hypothesis and a program for research. Archives of Neurology, 42, 521-552.
  61. 61. Giencke, S. & Lewandowski, L. (1989). Anomalous dominance in Down syndrome young adults. Cortex, 25, 93-102.
  62. 62. Giotakos, O. (2002). Crossed hand-eye dominance in male psychiatric patients. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 95, 728-732.
  63. 63. Goldstein, R. Z. & Volkow, N. D. (2011). Dysfunction of the prefrontal cortex in addiction: neuroimaging findings and clinical implications. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12, 652-669.
  64. 64. Gordon, H. (1921). Left-handedness and mirror writing, especially among defective children. Brain, 43, 313-368.
  65. 65. Grouios, G. & Ypsilanti, A. (2011). Language and visuospatial abilities in down syndrome phenotype: A cognitive neuroscience perspective. In S. K. Day (ed.), Genetics and etiology of Down syndrome (pp. 275-286). Rijeka, Croatia: InTech.
  66. 66. Grouios, G., Sakadami, N., Poderi, A. & Alevriadou, A. (1999). Excess of non-right handedness among individuals with intellectual disability: Experimental evidence and possible explanations. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 43, 306–313.
  67. 67. Hale, T. S., Zaidel, E., McGough, J. J., Phillips, J. M., & McCracken, J. T. (2006). Atypical brain laterality in adults with ADHD during dichotic listening for emotional intonation and words. Neuropsychologia, 44, 896-904.
  68. 68. Harada, T., Saito, D. N., Kashikura, K., Sato, T., Yonekura, Y., Honda, M., & Sadato, N. (2004). Asymmetrical neural substrates of tactile discrimination in humans: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. The Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 7524-7530.
  69. 69. Hartley, X. Y. (1981). Lateralization of speech stimuli in young Down’s syndrome children. Cortex, 17, 241–248.
  70. 70. Heath, M. & Elliott, D. (1999). The cerebral specialization for speech production in persons with Down syndrome. Brain and Language, 69, 193–211.
  71. 71. Heath, M., Grierson, L., Binsted, G., & Elliott, D. (2007). Hemispheric transmission time in persons with Down Syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 51, 972-981.
  72. 72. Hellige, J. B. (1993). Hemispheric asymmetry: What’s right and what’s left. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  73. 73. Hicks, R. E. & Banton, K. A. (1975). A note on left-handedness and severity of mental retardation. Journal of Generic Psychology, 127, 323-324.
  74. 74. Hines, D. (1972). Bilateral tachistoscopic recognition of verbal and nonverbal stimuli. Corex, 7, 313-322.
  75. 75. Hines, D. 1978). Visual information processing in the left and right hemispheres. Neuropsychologia, 16, 593-600.
  76. 76. Hugdahl, K. (2005). Symmetry and asymmetry in the human brain. European Review, 13, 119-133.
  77. 77. Hüsing, B., Jäncke, L., & Tag, B. (2006). Impact assessment of neuroimaging. Amsterdam: IOS Press.
  78. 78. Illingworth, S. & Bishop, D. V. M. (2009). Atypical cerebral lateralisation in adults with compensated developmental dyslexia demonstrated using functional transcranial Doppler ultrasound. Brain and Language, 111, 61-65.
  79. 79. Jacola, L., Byars, A., Chalfonte-Evans, M., Schmithorst, V., Hickey, F., Patterson, B., Hotze, S., Vannest, J., Chiu, C., Holland, S, & Schapiro, M. (2011). Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging of Cognitive Processing in Young Adults With Down Syndrome. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 116, 344-359.
  80. 80. Jansen, A., Menke, R., Sommer, J., Forster, A. F., Bruchmann, S., Hempleman, J., Weber, B., & Knecht, S. (2006). The assessment of hemispheric lateralization in functional MRI: Robustness and reproducibility. Neuroimage, 33, 204-217.
  81. 81. Järvinen-Pasley, A., Pollak, S. D., Yam, A., Hill, K. J., Grichanik, M., Mills, D., Reiss, A. L., Korenberg, J. R., & Bellugi, U. (2010). Atypical hemispheric asymmetry in the perception of negative human vocalizations in individuals with Williams syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 48, 1047-1052.
  82. 82. Kerr, J. N. D. & Denk, W. (2008). Imaging in vivo: Watching the brain in action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 195-205.
  83. 83. khondi-Asi, A., Jafari-Khouzani, K., Elisevich, K., & Soltanian-Zadeh, H. (2011). Hippocampal volumetry for lateralization of temporal lobe epilepsy: Automated versus manual methods. Neuroimage, 54 (Supplement 1), S218-S226.
  84. 84. Kimura, D. (1967). Functional asymmetry of the brain in dichotic listening. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 15, 156-65.
  85. 85. Leconte, P. & Fagard, J. (2006). Lateral preferences in children with intellectual deficiency of idiopathic origin. Developmental Psychobiology, 48, 492-500.
  86. 86. Lecours, A. R., Mehler, J., Parente, M. A., Behrami, M. C., Tolipan, L. C., Cary, L., Castro, M. J., Carrono, V., Chagastelles, L., Dehaut, F., Delgado, R., Evangelista, A., Faigenbaum, S., Fontoura, C., Karmann, D., Gurd, J., Torne, C. H., Jakybovicz, R., Kac, R., Lefevre, B., Lima, C., Maciel, J., Mansur, L., Martinez, R., Nobrega, M. C., Osorio, Z., Paciornik, J., Papaterram F., Penedo, M. A. J., Saboya, B., Scheuer, C., da Silva, A. B., Spinardi, M., Teixeira, M. (1988). Illiteracy and brain damage 3: contribution to the study of speech and language disorders in illiterates with unilateral brain damage (initial testing). Neuropsychologia, 26, 575-589.
  87. 87. Lennenberg, E. H., Nichols, I. A., & Rosenberger, E. F. (1967). Primitive stages of language development in mongolism. In Disorders of Communication, Vol. XLII: Research Publications," A.R.N.M.D., pp. 119-37.
  88. 88. Lewin, J., Kohen, D., & Mathew, G. (1993). Handedness in mental handicap: investigation into populations of Down’s syndrome, epilepsy and autism. British Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 674-676.
  89. 89. Loring, D. W. & Meador, K. J. (2000). Pre-surgical evaluation for epilepsy surgery. Saudi Medical Journal, 21, 609-616.
  90. 90. Losin, E. A., Rivera, S. M., O'Hare, E. D., Sowell, E. R., & Pinter, J. D. (2009). Abnormal fMRI activation pattern during story listening in individuals with Down syndrome. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 11, 369-380.
  91. 91. Lucas, J. A., Rosenstein, L. D., & Bigler, E. D. (1989). Handedness and language among the mentally retarded: implications for the model of pathological left handedness and gender differences in hemispheric specialization. Neuropsychologia, 27, 713-723.
  92. 92. Mandal M. K., Pandey G., Tulsi Das C., & Bryden M. P. (1998). Handedness in mental retardation. Laterality 3, 221-225.
  93. 93. McKeever,, W. F. & Huling, M. D. (1970). Lateral dominance in tachistoscopic word recognition as a function of hemisphere stimulation and interhemispheric transfer time. Neuropsychologia, 9, 291-299.
  94. 94. McManus, I. C. (1983). The interpretation of laterality. Cortex, 19, 187-214
  95. 95. Menghini, D., Costanzo, F., & Vicari, S. (2011). Relationship between brain and cognitive processes in Down syndrome. Behavior Genetics, 41, 381-393.
  96. 96. Morris, C.A. & Mervis, C.B. (2000). Williams syndrome and related disorders. Annual Review of Genomics and Human Genetics, 1, 461-484.
  97. 97. Mulvey, M., Ringenbach, S. D. R., & Jung, M. L. (2011). Reversal of handedness effects on bimanual coordination in adults with Down syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research 55, 998-1007.
  98. 98. Murphy, M. M. (1962). Hand preference in three diagnostic groups of severely deficient males. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 14, 508.
  99. 99. Oertel, V., Knöchel, C., Rotarska-Jagiela, A., Schönmeyer, R., Lindner., M., van de Ven, V., Haenschel, C., Uhlhaas, P., Maurer, K., & Linden, D. E. J. (2010). Reduced laterality as a trait marker of schizophrenia: Evidence from structural and functional neuroimaging. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 2289-2299.
  100. 100. Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97-113.
  101. 101. Orsini, D. L., Sazt, P., Soper, H. V., & Light, R. K. (1985). The role of familial sinistrality in cerebral organization. Neuropsychologia, 23, 223-232.
  102. 102. Palmer, R. D. (1964). Development of differentiated handedness. Psychological Bulletin, 62, 257-272.
  103. 103. Paquette, C., Bourassa, M., & Peretz, I. (1996). Left-ear advantage in pitch perception of complex tones without energy at the fundamental frequency. Neuropsychologia , 34, 153-157.
  104. 104. Pickersgill, M. & Pank, P. (1970). Relation of age and mongolism to lateral preferences in severely subnormal subjects. Nature, 228, 1342-1344.
  105. 105. Pinea, D. S., Kentgena, L. M., Bruderb, G. E., Leiteb, P., Bearmana, K., Maa, Y., & Kleinc, R. G. (2000) Cerebral laterality in adolescent major depression. Psychiatry Research, 93, 135-144.
  106. 106. Pinter, J. D., Eliez, S., Schmitt, J. E., Capone, G. T., & Reiss, A. L. (2001). Neuroanatomy of Down's syndrome: a high-resolution MRI study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 1659-1665.
  107. 107. Pipe, M. E. (1983). Dichotic-listening performance following auditory discrimination training in Down’s syndrome and developmentally retarded children. Cortex, 19, 481–491.
  108. 108. Pipe, M. E. (1987). Pathological left-handedness: is it familial? Neuropsychologia, 25, 571-577.
  109. 109. Porac, C. & Coren, S. (1976). The dominant eye. Psychological Bulletin, 83, 880-897.
  110. 110. Porac, C. & Coren, S. (1981). Lateral preferences and human behavior. New York: Springer.
  111. 111. Porac, C., Coren, S., & Duncan, P. (1980). Lateral preferences in retardates: Relationships between hand, eye foot and ear preference. Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 2, 173-188.
  112. 112. Porac, C., Coren, S., Steiger, J. H., & Duncan, P. (1980). Human laterality: a multidimensional approach. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 34, 91-96.
  113. 113. Previc, F. H. (1994). Assessing the legacy of the GBG model. Brain and Cognition, 26, 174-180.
  114. 114. Raven, J. C., Court, J. H., Javen, J. (1985). A manual for Raven's progressive matrices and vocabulary scales. London: H. K. Lewis.
  115. 115. Reiss, M. & Reiss, G. (1998). Ear preference: Association with other functional asymmetries of the ears. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 86, 399-402.
  116. 116. Reiss, M., Tymnik, G., Kogler, P., Kogler, W., & Reiss, G. (1999). Laterality of hand, foot, eye, and ear in twins. Laterality, 4, 287-297.
  117. 117. Rengstorff, R. H. (1967). The types and incidence of hand-eye preference and its relationship with certain reading abilities. American Journal of Optometric Archives of the American Academy of Optometry, 44, 233.
  118. 118. Royet, J. P, & Plailly, J. (2004): Lateralization of olfactory processes. Chemical Senses, 29, 731-745.
  119. 119. Salmaso, D. & Longoni, A.M. (1985). Problems in assessing hand preference. Cortex, 21, 533-549.
  120. 120. Satz, P. (1973). Left-handedness and early brain insult: An explanation, Neuropsychologia, 11, 115-117.
  121. 121. Satz, P., Baymur, L., & Van der Vlugt, H. (1979). Pathological left-handedness: Crosscultural tests of a model. Neuropsychologia, 17, 77-81.
  122. 122. Satz, P., Orsini, D.L., Saslow, E., & Henry, R. (1985). The pathological left-handedness syndrome, Brain and Cognition, 4, 27-46.
  123. 123. Searleman, A., Cunningham, T. F., & Goodwin, W. (1988). Association between familial sinistrality and pathological left-handedness: A comparison of mentally retarded and nonretarded subjects. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 10, 132-138.
  124. 124. Silva, D. A. & Satz, P. (1979). Pathological left-handedness: evaluation of model. Brain and Language, 7, 8-16.
  125. 125. Slezicki, K. I., Cho, Y. W., Brock, M. S., Pfeiffer, M., McVearry, K., & Tractenberg, R. E., Motamedi, G. K. (2009). Incidence of atypical handedness in epilepsy and its association with clinical factors. Epilepsy and Behavior, 16, 330-334.
  126. 126. Soper, H. V., Satz, P., Orsini, D. L., Henry, R. R., Zvi, R., & Sehulman, M. (1986). Handedness patterns in autism suggest subtypes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 16, 155-167.
  127. 127. Soper, H. V. & Satz, P. (1884). Pathological left-handedness and ambiguous handedness: A new explanatory model. Neuropsychologia, 22, 511-515.
  128. 128. Soper, H. V., Satz, P., Orsini, D. L., Vangorp, W. G., & Green, M.F., (1987). Handedness distribution in a residential population with severe or profound mental-retardation. American Journal of Mental Deficiency. 92, 94– 102.
  129. 129. Springer, S. & Deutsch, G. (1981). Left brain, right brain. San Francisco, CA: W. H. Freeman.
  130. 130. Tomasi, D. & Volkow, N. D. (2012). Laterality patterns of brain functional connectivity: Gender effects Cerebral Cortex, 22, 1455-1454.
  131. 131. Tzavaras , A., Kaprinis, G., & Gatzoyas, A. (1981). Literacy and hemispheric specialisation for language: Digit dichotic listening in illiterates. Neuropsychologia, 19, 565-570.
  132. 132. Uecker, A., Mangan, P. A., Obrzut, J. E. & Nadel, L. (1993). Down syndrome in neurobiological perspective: An emphasis on spatial cognition. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 22, 2, 266-276.
  133. 133. Van Strien, J. W., Lagers-van Haselen, G. C., van Hagen, J. M., de Coo, I. F. M., Frens, M. A., & van der Gest, J. N. (2005). Increased prevalences of left-handedness and left-eye sighting dominance individuals with Williams-Beuren syndrome. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 27, 967-976.
  134. 134. Verjat, I. (1988). Dissymmetry in manual perception by children on form recognition tasks with tactually presented forms. European Bulletin of Cognitive Psychology, 8, 223-239.
  135. 135. Vlachos, F. & Karapetsas, A. (1999). A developmental study of handedness in Down syndrome pupils. Perceptual and Motor Skills,88, 427-428.
  136. 136. Wada, J. & Rasmussen, T. (1960). Intracarotid injection of sodium amytal for the lateralization of speech dominance. Journal of Neurosurgery, 17, 266-282.
  137. 137. Wada, J. (1949). A new method for determination of the side of cerebral speech dominance: A preliminary report on the intracarotid injection of sodium amytal in man. Igaku Seibutsugaku, 4, 221-222.
  138. 138. Wang, P. P., Doherty, S., Hesselink, J. R., & Bellugi, U. (1992). Callosal morphology concurs with neurobehavioral and neuropsychological findings in two neurodevelopmental disorders. Archives of Neurology, 49, 407–411.
  139. 139. Wechsler D. (1992) Wechsler intelligence scale for children (3rd ed). Kent UK: The Psychological Corporation, Harcourt Brace & Co, Sidcup.
  140. 140. Weeks, D. J., Chua, R, Elliot, D, Lyons, J, & Pollock, B. J. (1995). Cerebral specialisation for receptive language in individuals with Down syndrome. Australian Journal of Psychology, 47, 137-140.
  141. 141. Welsh, T. N., Elliott, D., & Simon, D. (2003). Cerebral specialization and verbal-motor integration in adults with and without Down syndrome. Brain and Language, 84, 152-169.
  142. 142. Witelson, S. F. (1974). Hemispheric specialization for linguistic and nonlinguistic tactual perception using dichotomous stimulation technique. Cortex, 10, 3-17.
  143. 143. Ypsilanti, A. (2009). Investigating functional cerebral asymmetry with preference and performance measures in individuals with intellectual disability. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation. Aristotle University of Thessaloniki. Thessalon;iki, Greece.

Written By

George Grouios, Antonia Ypsilanti and Irene Koidou

Submitted: 26 April 2012 Published: 06 March 2013