Open access peer-reviewed chapter

Emergent Reading and Brain Development

Written By

Yingying Wang

Submitted: 21 October 2018 Reviewed: 06 November 2018 Published: 05 December 2018

DOI: 10.5772/intechopen.82423

From the Edited Volume

Early Childhood Education

Edited by Donna Farland-Smith

Chapter metrics overview

1,824 Chapter Downloads

View Full Metrics

Abstract

Emergent reading emphasizes the developmental continuum aspect of learning to read and advocates the importance of reading-related behaviors occurring before school. Brain imaging evidence has suggested high plasticity of young children’s brains, and emergent reading experience can shape the brain development supporting fluent reading. The brain imaging evidence elucidates our understanding of the importance of emergent reading from a neurobiological point of view. Future studies are needed to understand how emergent reading experience can become protective factor for children at risk for reading impairments. Future studies need to design early interventions to improve emergent reading experience which is a crucial period.

Keywords

  • emergent reading
  • brain responses
  • neural basis
  • development
  • shared book reading
  • developmental dyslexia

1. Introduction

Reading is a complex process involving multiple regions in the brain communicating with each other to facilitate effective reading. Learning to read can play an important role in academic success. There is a reciprocal relationship between language and reading learning where improvements in one can lead to an increased understanding of the other [1, 2]. This chapter focuses on the concept of emergent reading and brain imaging evidence related to reading acquisition and aims to elucidate our understanding of emergent reading experience and its relationship with brain development.

Advertisement

2. Emergent reading

2.1. What is emergent reading?

The term “emergent reading” is derived from “emergent literacy” and is used to advocate that the development of reading starts early in a child’s life instead of school years. The emergent literacy includes both reading and writing components. The concept “emergent reading” emphasizes the developmental continuum aspect in learning to read, rather than an all-or-none phenomenon that begins only when a child starts school, suggesting there is a boundary between reading and pre-reading. For example, over the years, educators focused on identifying what skills a child needs to understand before he/she can learn to read through a formal reading curriculum. In contrast, an emergent reading perspective views reading-related behaviors occurring before school as essential aspects of reading. Besides, an emergent reading perspective views that language and reading develop concurrently and interdependently from an early age when children were exposed to social interactions in which reading is a component, and no formal instruction was involved.

Emergent reading consists of the skills, knowledge, and attitudes that are presumed to be developmental precursors to conventional forms of reading [1, 3] and the environments supporting these developments (e.g., home literacy environment, shared book reading, etc.).

2.2. Components of emergent reading

Based on the literature, the main components of emergent reading include vocabulary knowledge, decontextualized language skills, conventions of print, knowledge of letters, linguistic awareness, and phoneme-grapheme correspondence.

Vocabulary knowledge is important in emergent reading. Reading requires decoding of visual inputs into meaning. In the earliest stages, a child decodes a word letter by letter, links each letter into its corresponding sound, and combines all the letter-sounds to a single word. For example, in the beginning, a child decodes a word “cat” by sounding out /k/ … /æ/ … /t/. The next stage is to extract the meaning of the word, which is important since it motivates the child. If a child knows individual letters but does not know the meaning, he/she is unlikely enjoying the reading process since the child has no semantic representation through which a child decodes the phonological information. Research studies have shown that semantic and syntactic abilities play important roles in acquiring reading skills when the child is reading for meaning [4, 5]. A recent study investigated the relationship between semantic knowledge and word reading in 27 6-year-old children [4]. General semantic knowledge was assessed using standardized tasks in which children defined words and made judgments about the relationships between words. They have provided strong evidence that variation in semantic knowledge is associated with variation in word-reading performance.

Decontextualized language skills refer to the language used in story narratives and other written forms of communications to convey novel information to readers [3]. Conventions of print in English include the left-to-right and top-to-bottom direction of print, the sequence in which the print progresses from front to back across pages, the difference between pictures and print on a page, and the meaning of elements of punctuation. Knowing these conventions helps a child learn to read [3]. Decontextualized language skills in children are related to conventional reading skills including decoding, understanding story narratives, and print production [6].

Knowledge of letters is critical to learning the sounds associated with the letters. However, only teaching letter names may only increase surface letter knowledge and may not improve the abilities to learn to read [7]. Linguistic awareness involves the ability to take language as a cognitive object and to understand how language is constructed and to use language as a way of communication. Linguistic awareness develops over time, and a child may be aware of some rules (e.g., that words are formed from phonemes) without being aware of other rules (e.g., two words rhyme). Many studies have suggested that children good at detecting syllables and rhymes are better readers [8].

Linguistic awareness involves the ability to take language as a cognitive object and to possess information about the syntax. Most research on linguistic awareness has focused on phonological skills (e.g., phoneme isolation, phoneme deletion, etc.). The relation appears to be reciprocal. Better phonological skills led to quicker learning to read [9, 10, 11, 12], while learning to read improves phonological skills [13, 14].

Phoneme-grapheme correspondence represents the links between phonemes and alphabet letters. A child requires to understand both how individual letter sounds and how combined letters sound. This ability has been related to higher levels of reading achievement [10, 15].

Children learn these main components of emergent reading before formal schooling. These components are the building blocks that a child needs to learn to read. Becoming a fluent reader requires all these components, which can be divided into two interdependent sets of skills and processes. They are the process of decoding and comprehension. The process of decoding needs children’s knowledge of rules for translating letters to sounds and sounds to words, while the process of comprehension needs children to find meanings for the words. Both are essential processes for reading. Difficulties in either process can lead to reading impairments.

2.3. Environments supporting emergent reading

Home literacy environment has been suggested to positively correlate to preschooler’s language abilities [16, 17]. Home literacy environment characterizes the literacy-related interactions and resources at home, including shared book reading between parents and children (e.g., frequency, duration) and exposure to literacy materials (e.g., how many books at home, types of books). The American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP) advocates reading aloud to children every day, beginning from birth [18]. The AAP early literacy policy released in June 2014 urges pediatricians and policymakers to ensure that books are available to all families, particularly those with low income [18]. High et al. recommends that parents focus on the following activity: read together, rhyme and play with words, set consistent routines, reward with praise, and develop a strong relationship with the child [18]. Shared reading between parents and children can strengthen bonding and improve language skills and vocabulary knowledge. Dialogic reading, known as a shared picture book reading intervention for preschoolers, has been suggested to boost the preschooler’s language abilities [19, 20, 21]. Moreover, the new understanding of brain development through neuroimaging studies has also suggested that the first 1000 days are the crucial developmental stage for later cognitive development.

Children’s daycare and preschool environments are important for children’s emergent reading experience [3]. Studies have identified that aspects of the curriculum, the environment, teach-child interactions, and teaching practices within the classroom are related to the cognitive ability and achievement of children [22]. When controlling for home literacy environment, children’s daycare and preschool environments still predict children’s cognitive and academic achievement scores.

2.4. Socioeconomic status

School readiness refers to a mismatch between what many children bring to their first school experience and what schools expect of them if they are to succeed and is strongly linked to family income [3]. Socioeconomic status (SES) is one of the strongest predictors of performance differences in children at the beginning of first grade [23]. Differences in SES could lead to differences in emergent reading experiences (e.g., language exposure at home, family stress, cognitive stimulation) that likely shape the early development of brain regions that are crucial for becoming a skilled reader [24, 25]. Children from low SES are at risk for DD and are also more likely to be slow in learning to read [26]. Moreover, Matthew effects in reading demonstrated that a child is a disadvantaged organism because of the low SES and genotype provided by the child’s parents [27]. Many students with low SES entering school are significantly behind their more advantaged peers with high SES, and the academic performance gap widens over the course of elementary school [28, 29]. Children from families with different SES exposure to different experiences that support the development of emergent reading skills. Mothers from lower SES groups engaged in fewer teaching behaviors during shared reading than mothers from middle-class groups [30, 31].

2.5. Interventions to enhance emergent reading experiences

Various interventions targeting one or more components of emergent reading have been developed including dialogic reading, little books, phonological sensitivity training, and whole language instruction.

Dialogic reading is a program of shared picture book reading intervention for preschoolers, and it can substantially improve a child’s language skills in preschool [19, 32, 33, 34]. Dialogic reading is different from the conventional shared reading during which the adult reads and the child listens. During dialogic reading, the child learns to become a storyteller, while the adult acts as an active listener, asking questions and prompting the child to increase the sophistication of descriptions of the material in the picture book.

Little books are small, easy-to-read books that contain simple words, simple illustrations, and repetitive text. Studies have shown that giving free little books to children from family with low and middle incomes facilitates better emergent reading experience and supports better reading outcomes [35, 36, 37].

Phonological sensitivity training is to teach children phonological sensitivity, which is one of the strongest predictors of later reading achievement. Interventions on phonological sensitivity training have been shown to be effective in beginning readers [38, 39, 40].

Whole language instruction focuses on the reading components including language units (e.g., words), semantic units (e.g., concepts), and contextual units (e.g., narrative) [41, 42]. Whole language approach advocates that there are strong parallel between the reading acquisition and oral language acquisition and believes that reading acquisition would occur as easily and naturally as language acquisition if the meaning and purpose of the text were emphasized. However, there is ongoing debate on whether whole language emphasis is effective approach [43]. More research is necessary to resolve this debate. Whole language is currently controversial approach to teach reading.

Advertisement

3. Behavior and brain connection

If cognitive behaviors are the immediate results of our brain states, then the most effective way of uncovering a cognitive behavior is to understand the brain states that would lead to it. Brain states are determined by the organization of synaptic connections between neurons that generate various patterns of activations. Thus, brain imaging can provide insights into the neural basis that would lead to the certain cognitive behavior.

When a child learns to read, he/she is more likely to show reading-related activity in the region of occipitotemporal cortex [44, 45, 46, 47]. Two decades ago, brain research has suggested that the socioeconomic status (SES) modulates brain-behavior relationships in reading [25]. Specifically, as SES levels decreased, the relationship between the phonological language skill and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data was stronger, whereas as SES levels increased, these brain-behavior relationships were attenuated [25]. Thus, a child’s background and life experiences, as determined by SES, can systematically influence the relationship between emergent reading skills and reading-related brain activity. To better understand the importance of emergent reading experience, brain imaging evidence will be used to demonstrate the underlying neural basis supporting the developmental continuum aspect of learning to read.

Advertisement

4. Brain imaging evidence

Recent advances in neuroimaging techniques make it possible to identify the brain-based factors that facilitate successful reading outcomes. Importantly, brain imaging may provide innovative solutions to improve education curriculums and lead to improvements in reading results in young children.

Over the last decades, neuroimaging studies focused on identifying brain markers that are the cause of dyslexia (see reviews: [48, 49]). Although researchers are far from concluding that the brain markers causing dyslexia, we have learned about the neural basis of reading acquisition. For instance, a left-lateralized brain network, including temporoparietal and occipitotemporal cortices, is critical to facilitate skilled reading [50, 51] (see Figure 1). High white matter integrity in accurate fasciculus (AF) predicts better reading outcomes in children at risk for dyslexia [52]. AF is a tract connecting Broca’s area and Wernicke’s area, related to reading ability [53, 54, 55] (see Figure 1). If neuroimaging measures can identify children at risk for reading difficulties before they even start to learn to read in school, early emergent reading interventions can be applied to help them overcome the risk of developing reading difficulties in school years. Only a limited number of studies have specifically investigated the relationship between emergent reading environments and neuroimaging data.

Figure 1.

Brain regions and white matter tracts related to reading on a 3D rendered brain. Red: accurate fasciculus (AF), green: superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF).

Hutton et al. used StimQ-P questionnaire [56] to quantify the cognitive simulation at home and identified that functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data during a storying comprehension task presented stronger activity for those children with higher StimQ-P Reading scores [57]. They reported that higher StimQ Reading scores were associated with stronger activation in occipital cortices, including lateral occipital gyrus and precuneus, which can be attributed to mental imagery evoked during story listening [58]. Their study sample includes nineteen 3- to 5-year-old children from a longitudinal study of healthy brain development. In preschool children listening to stories, greater home reading exposure was positively related to activation of left posterior occipital fusiform, lateral occipital, posterior inferior temporal, posterior middle temporal, posterior cingulate, and angular gyri and left precuneus (household income is controlled). Their finding suggests that brain-based markers exist as a result of parent-child reading in early childhood. Thus, emergent reading shall be promoted and may help shape the developing brain and better prepare a child for formal reading instructions in school.

Developmental dyslexia (DD) has strong genetic basis [59], and family history of DD can increase a child’s chance to develop reading difficulties by 34–56% [60, 61, 62]. In order to identify children at risk for DD, familial risk can be used as a good indicator. One group led by Dr. Nadine Gaab in Boston Children’s Hospital has done pioneer work in this research field [48, 52, 63, 64, 65, 66, 67, 68, 69, 70]. For the first time, they examined the relationship between home literacy environment (HLE) and the neural basis of phonological processing in beginning readers with family history of DD (n = 29, first-degree relative who has reading difficulties) and without family history of DD (n = 21) [67]. This study aimed to identify brain mechanism of how HLE affects reading development in beginning readers. SES was controlled in this study in order to isolate the effects only by HLE. In reading-related brain regions (e.g., left inferior/middle frontal and right fusiform gyri), stronger correlations between HLE composite scores and brain activations were present in children without familial risk than those with familial risk. In the nonreading-related brain region (e.g., right precentral gyrus), stronger correlations existed in children with familial risk than those without familial risk. These findings suggest that genetic predisposition for DD alters contributions of HLE to brain activation. Specifically, typically developing children can benefit more from better HLE than children with familial risk for DD. Therefore, enhanced HLE is especially important for children with familial risk for DD to have the same impact as for typically developing children.

Shared parent-child reading is one of the important factors in emergent reading. A recent study demonstrated increased activation and functional connectivity in children who are more deeply engaged during shared reading in 22 mother-daughter pairs [71]. The same group also associated shared reading quality scores with brain activation, and they found a positive correlation between shared reading quality scores with activation in left-hemispheric regions supporting expressive and complex language, social-emotional integration, and working memory in 22 healthy, 4-year-old girls from low SES [72]. Their findings suggest that the use of shared parent-child reading is crucial for emergent reading experience, but the quality of this experience has also a strong impact on brain development. Especially for those at-risk families, improvements of the quality of shared reading can promote healthy brain development and better prepare a child for future success in school.

Morken et al. [73] used a longitudinal study design to examine the differences of cortical connectivity in the brain during reading tasks between children with dyslexia and children with typical reading development through dynamic causal modeling (DCM) [74]. They included five regions (inferior frontal gyrus, precentral gyrus, superior temporal gyrus, inferior parietal lobule, and occipitotemporal cortex) in their effective brain connectivity model [74]. They found that effective connectivity between the inferior frontal gyrus and the occipitotemporal cortex during reading tasks changes during reading acquisition. In addition, the group readers with dyslexia presented different developmental trajectory than the control group. The control group actually seemed to downregulate or stabilize connection strength over time, whereas the dyslexia group started out at a level well below the control group, followed by an increase in connectivity from 6 to 8 years and then a downregulation from 8 to 12 years. The general downregulation of connectivity in the control group might reflect that they need these connections to establish reading skills initially, and then, the connections are no longer needed after later automaticity is established. The dyslexia group showed late development of some connections in occipitotemporal cortices. However, they seem to show overcompensation around age 8, followed by normalization before age 12. Importantly, the dyslexia group was clearly lagging behind in the development of the brain networks at the age of 8 (emergent reading stage), suggesting emergent reading stage is critical.

Younger et al. also used a longitudinal study design and found decreases in connectivity for most connections from the first (T1) to the second (T2) time point about 2–3 years apart, regardless of changes in reading skill in 59 typical developing children [75]. But they found a significant decrease in the dorsal, decoding processing pathway from fusiform gyrus (FG) to inferior parietal lobule (IPL) for the group who improved more from the first to the second time point, suggesting that the improvements in reading skills lead to a decreased reliance on the dorsal pathway (decoding processing pathway) in the brain. The high and low improving groups did not differ in behavioral performance at T1, and high improvers showed greater connectivity between FG and IPL at T1 compared to the low improvers. The dorsal pathway facilitates phonological processing, which is necessary for development of the ventral pathway supporting automatic processing of visual word forms. However, there is no sequential relationship between the two routes. They may develop simultaneously.

Yu et al. studied 28 children over three stages (pre-reading, beginning reading, and emergent reading) and found decreases in neural activation in the left inferior parietal cortex (LIPC) during an audiovisual phonological processing task [69]. Seed-based brain network analysis revealed increases in connection strength in the brain network of children with above-average gains in phonological processing but decreases in connection strength in the brain network of children with below-average gains in phonological processing measured by Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing (CTOPP). Moreover, the connection strength between LIPC and the left posterior occipitotemporal cortex (LpOTC, BA 18) at the pre-reading stage significantly predicted reading skills at the emergent reading stage.

Advertisement

5. Discussion

This chapter demonstrates the view of emergent reading and brain imaging evidence supporting advocacy for emergent reading. Emergent reading emphasizes the developmental continuum aspect of learning to read and the importance of reading-related behaviors occurring before school.

Both behavioral and imaging studies on DD suggest that early reading skills are essential to the later development of reading. Most children start formal reading curriculum in kindergarten; however, at that time, many factors (genetic, SES, HLE, etc.) have already affected the future reading development. Moreover, early interventions work more effectively.

Brain regions (left inferior/middle frontal gyrus, bilateral fusiform gyri, and right anterior superior temporal gyrus) were identified to be especially sensitive to differences of early language/literacy exposure in beginning readers [67]. A richer HLE corresponded to increased brain activations during a phonological processing task [67] and increased brain activations related to high reading proficiency [76] demonstrated the underlying neural basis of reading. Among the children with a familial risk for DD, only around 50% of them will develop DD. The imaging evidence implies that a rich HLE might be one of the protective factors in reading development especially for children with a familial risk for DD. Future longitudinal studies are needed to examine how HLE contributes to the development of reading networks in the brain and its role as a protective factor in general.

Advocating emergent reading can benefit all children who are learning to read and especially those who are also at risk for DD. It is clear that aspects of HLE (e.g., shared reading) before a child entering kindergarten or preschool benefit the later reading development.

Advertisement

6. Conclusions

Emergent reading experience is crucial since it affects the development of reading. The formal reading curriculum usually starts in kindergarten. Before kindergarten, genetic and environmental factors have already affected the starting point for children. Research studies on DD have provided a rich body of evidence that reading acquisition is influenced by complex genetic and environmental interactions [48]. Recent studies started to focus on the importance of home literacy environment and emergent reading stage using brain imaging evidence.

Advertisement

7. Future directions

There are still a limited number of longitudinal imaging studies on emergent reading. In the future, research shall focus on studying which intervention approaches in emergent reading stage work the best using both behavioral and brain imaging data. In addition, how brain imaging evidence can be used in designing optimized interventions targeting emergent reading stage.

Advertisement

Acknowledgments

Thanks to start-up fund from the Department of Special Education and Communication Disorders and Office of Research & Economic at the University of Nebraska-Lincoln.

Advertisement

Conflict of interest

No conflict of interest declaration.

References

  1. 1. Teale WH, Sulzby E. Emergent Literacy: Writing and Reading. Writing Research: Multidisciplinary Inquiries into the Nature of Writing Series. ERIC. Norwood: Ablex Publishing Corporation; 1986
  2. 2. Koppenhaver DA, Coleman PP, Kalman SL, Yoder DE. The implications of emergent literacy research for children with developmental disabilities. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology. 1991;1(1):38-44
  3. 3. Whitehurst GJ, Lonigan CJ. Child development and emergent literacy. Child Development. 1998;69(3):848-872
  4. 4. Ricketts J, Davies R, Masterson J, Stuart M, Duff FJ. Evidence for semantic involvement in regular and exception word reading in emergent readers of English. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2016;150:330-345
  5. 5. Nation K, Snowling MJ. Beyond phonological skills: Broader language skills contribute to the development of reading. Journal of Research in Reading. 2004;27(4):342-356
  6. 6. Dickinson DK, Snow CE. Interrelationships among prereading and oral language skills in kindergartners from two social classes. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 1987;2(1):1-25
  7. 7. Adams M. Beginning to Read: Thinking and Learning About Print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1990
  8. 8. Bowey JA. Phonological sensitivity in novice readers and nonreaders. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1994;58(1):134-159
  9. 9. Goswami U, Bryant P. Essays in Developmental Psychology Series. Phonological Skills and Learning to Read. Hillsdale, NJ, USA: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc; 1990
  10. 10. Byrne B, Fielding-Barnsley R, Ashley L. Effects of preschool phoneme identity training after six years: Outcome level distinguished from rate of response. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2000;92(4):659
  11. 11. Melby-Lervåg M, Lyster S-AH, Hulme C. Phonological skills and their role in learning to read: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin. 2012;138(2):322
  12. 12. Wagner RK, Torgesen JK, Rashotte CA, Hecht SA, Barker TA, Burgess SR, et al. Changing relations between phonological processing abilities and word-level reading as children develop from beginning to skilled readers: A 5-year longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology. 1997;33(3):468
  13. 13. Bryant P, Goswami U. Phonological Skills and Learning to Read. London, United Kingdom: Routledge; 2016
  14. 14. Perfetti CA, Beck I, Bell LC, Hughes C. Phonemic knowledge and learning to read are reciprocal: A longitudinal study of first grade children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly (1982). 1987;33:283-319
  15. 15. Newman EH, Tardif T, Huang J, Shu H. Phonemes matter: The role of phoneme-level awareness in emergent Chinese readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2011;108(2):242-259
  16. 16. Carroll JM, Holliman AJ, Weir F, Baroody AE. Literacy interest, home literacy environment and emergent literacy skills in preschoolers. Journal of Research in Reading. 2018. Nov(epub)
  17. 17. Liu C, Georgiou GK, Manolitsis G. Modeling the relationships of parents’ expectations, family’s SES, and home literacy environment with emergent literacy skills and word reading in Chinese. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2018;43:1-10
  18. 18. Council on Early Childhood, High PC, Klass P. Literacy promotion: An essential component of primary care pediatric practice. Pediatrics. 2014;134(2):404-409
  19. 19. Morgan PL, Meier CR. Dialogic reading’s potential to improve children’s emergent literacy skills and behavior. Preventing School Failure: Alternative Education for Children and Youth. 2008;52(4):11-16
  20. 20. Hamilton LG, Hayiou-Thomas ME, Hulme C, Snowling MJ. The home literacy environment as a predictor of the early literacy development of children at family-risk of dyslexia. Scientific Studies of Reading. 2016;20(5):401-419
  21. 21. Simsek ZC, Erdogan NI. Effects of the dialogic and traditional reading techniques on children’s language development. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences. 2015;197:754-758
  22. 22. Harms T, Clifford R. Early childhood environmental rating scale New York. Teachers College Press its relationship to early reading. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1980;86:221-223
  23. 23. Sirin SR. Socioeconomic status and academic achievement: A meta-analytic review of research. Review of Educational Research. 2005;75(3):417-453
  24. 24. Noble KG, Mccandliss BD. Reading development and impairment: Behavioral, social, and neurobiological factors. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics. 2005;26(5):370-378
  25. 25. Noble KG, Wolmetz ME, Ochs LG, Farah MJ, McCandliss BD. Brain–behavior relationships in reading acquisition are modulated by socioeconomic factors. Developmental Science. 2006;9(6):642-654
  26. 26. Juel C. Learning to read and write: A longitudinal study of 54 children from first through fourth grades. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1988;80(4):437
  27. 27. Stanovich K. Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual reading and language development in preschool classrooms. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1986;93(2):243-250
  28. 28. Stanovich KE. Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Journal of Education. 2009;189(1-2):23-55
  29. 29. Pfost M, Hattie J, Dörfler T, Artelt C. Individual differences in reading development: A review of 25 years of empirical research on Matthew effects in reading. Review of Educational Research. 2014;84(2):203-244
  30. 30. Ninio A. Picture-book reading in mother-infant dyads belonging to two subgroups in Israel. Child Development. 1980;51(2):587-590
  31. 31. Raz IS, Bryant P. Social background, phonological awareness and children’s reading. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 1990;8(3):209-225
  32. 32. Zevenbergen AA, Whitehurst GJ. Dialogic Reading: A Shared Picture Book Reading Intervention for Preschoolers. In: Van Kleeck A, Stahl SA, Bauer EB, editors. On Reading Books to Children: Parents and Teachers. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 2003. pp. 177-200
  33. 33. Arnold DS, Whitehurst GJ. Accelerating language development through picture book reading: A summary of dialogic reading and its effect. In: Diskinson DK editor. Bridges to literacy; Children, families, and schools. Cambridge, England: Blackwell; 1994:103-128
  34. 34. Whitehurst GJ, Arnold DS, Epstein JN, Angell AL, Smith M, Fischel JE. A picture book reading intervention in day care and home for children from low-income families. Developmental Psychology. 1994;30(5):679
  35. 35. McCormick C, Mason JM. Use of little books at home: A minimal intervention strategy that fosters early reading. In: Center for the Study of Reading Technical Report, No. 388; 1986
  36. 36. Pikulski JJ. Preventing reading failure: A review of five effective programs. The Reading Teacher. 1994;48(1):30-39
  37. 37. Gallimore R, Goldenberg C. Activity settings of early literacy: Home and school factors in children’s emergent literacy. In: Forman EA, Minick N, Stone CA, editors. Contexts for Learning: Sociocultural Dynamics in Children’s Development. New York: Oxford University Press; 1993. pp. 315-335
  38. 38. De Jong PF, Seveke M-J, van Veen M. Phonological sensitivity and the acquisition of new words in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2000;76(4):275-301
  39. 39. Lonigan CJ, Driscoll K, Phillips BM, Cantor BG, Anthony JL, Goldstein H. A computer-assisted instruction phonological sensitivity program for preschool children at-risk for reading problems. Journal of Early Intervention. 2003;25(4):248-262
  40. 40. Bowey JA. Reflections on onset-rime and phoneme sensitivity as predictors of beginning word reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2002;82(1):29-40
  41. 41. Anderson GS. A Whole Language Approach to Reading: ERIC. Lanham, MD: University Press of America; 1984
  42. 42. Stahl SA, Miller PD. Whole language and language experience approaches for beginning reading: A quantitative research synthesis. Review of Educational Research. 1989;59(1):87-116
  43. 43. Foorman BR. Research on "the Great Debate": Code-oriented versus whole language approaches to reading instruction. School Psychology Review. 1995;24:276-292
  44. 44. Shaywitz BA, Shaywitz SE, Pugh KR, Mencl WE, Fulbright RK, Skudlarski P, et al. Disruption of posterior brain systems for reading in children with developmental dyslexia. Biological Psychiatry. 2002;52(2):101-110
  45. 45. Brunswick N, McCrory E, Price CJ, Frith CD, Frith U. Explicit and implicit processing of words and pseudowords by adult developmental dyslexics: A search for Wernicke’s Wortschatz? Brain. 1999;122(Pt 10):1901-1917
  46. 46. Cohen L, Dehaene S, Naccache L, Lehéricy S, Dehaene-Lambertz G, Hénaff M-A, et al. The visual word form area: Spatial and temporal characterization of an initial stage of reading in normal subjects and posterior split-brain patients. Brain. 2000;123(2):291-307
  47. 47. Paulesu E, Demonet JF, Fazio F, McCrory E, Chanoine V, Brunswick N, et al. Dyslexia: Cultural diversity and biological unity. Science. 2001;291(5511):2165-2167
  48. 48. Ozernov-Palchik O, Yu X, Wang Y, Gaab N. Lessons to be learned: How a comprehensive neurobiological framework of atypical reading development can inform educational practice. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 2016;10:45-58
  49. 49. Norton ES, Beach SD, Gabrieli JD. Neurobiology of dyslexia. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2015;30:73-78
  50. 50. Kovelman I, Norton ES, Christodoulou JA, Gaab N, Lieberman DA, Triantafyllou C, et al. Brain basis of phonological awareness for spoken language in children and its disruption in dyslexia. Cerebral Cortex. 2011;22(4):754-764
  51. 51. Raschle NM, Stering PL, Meissner SN, Gaab N. Altered neuronal response during rapid auditory processing and its relation to phonological processing in prereading children at familial risk for dyslexia. Cerebral Cortex. 2014;24(9):2489-2501
  52. 52. Wang Y, Mauer MV, Raney T, Peysakhovich B, Becker BLC, Sliva DD, et al. Development of tract-specific white matter pathways during early reading development in at-risk children and typical controls. Cerebral Cortex. 2017;27(4):2469-2485
  53. 53. Andrews JS, Ben-Shachar M, Yeatman JD, Flom LL, Luna B, Feldman HM. Reading performance correlates with white-matter properties in preterm and term children. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 2010;52(6):e94-e100
  54. 54. Yeatman JD, Dougherty RF, Rykhlevskaia E, Sherbondy AJ, Deutsch GK, Wandell BA, et al. Anatomical properties of the arcuate fasciculus predict phonological and reading skills in children. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2011;23(11):3304-3317
  55. 55. Yeatman JD, Dougherty RF, Ben-Shachar M, Wandell BA. Development of white matter and reading skills. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2012;109(44):E3045-E3053
  56. 56. The Bellevue Project for Early Language L, Education Success (BELLE), STIMQ Cognitive Home Environment. 2014. http://pediatrics.med.nyu.edu/developmental/research/the-belle-project/stimq-cognitivehome-environment
  57. 57. Hutton JS, Horowitz-Kraus T, Mendelsohn AL, DeWitt T, Holland SK. Home reading environment and brain activation in preschool children listening to stories. Pediatrics. 2015;136(3):466-478
  58. 58. Schmithorst VJ, Holland SK, Plante E. Cognitive modules utilized for narrative comprehension in children: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. NeuroImage. 2006;29(1):254-266
  59. 59. Galaburda AM, LoTurco J, Ramus F, Fitch RH, Rosen GD. From genes to behavior in developmental dyslexia. Nature Neuroscience. 2006;9(10):1213-1217
  60. 60. Pennington BF, Lefly DL. Early reading development in children at family risk for dyslexia. Child Development. 2001;72(3):816-833
  61. 61. Snowling MJ, Gallagher A, Frith U. Family risk of dyslexia is continuous: Individual differences in the precursors of reading skill. Child Development. 2003;74(2):358-373
  62. 62. Smith SD, Pennington BF, Kimberling WJ, Ing PS. Familial dyslexia: Use of genetic linkage data to define subtypes. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1990;29(2):204-213
  63. 63. Raschle NM, Chang M, Gaab N. Structural brain alterations associated with dyslexia predate reading onset. NeuroImage. 2011;57(3):742-749
  64. 64. Raschle N, Zuk J, Ortiz-Mantilla S, Sliva DD, Franceschi A, Grant PE, et al. Pediatric neuroimaging in early childhood and infancy: Challenges and practical guidelines. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2012;1252:43-50
  65. 65. Raschle NM, Zuk J, Gaab N. Functional characteristics of developmental dyslexia in left-hemispheric posterior brain regions predate reading onset. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2012;109(6):2156-2161
  66. 66. Saygin ZM, Norton ES, Osher DE, Beach SD, Cyr AB, Ozernov-Palchik O, et al. Tracking the roots of reading ability: White matter volume and integrity correlate with phonological awareness in prereading and early-reading kindergarten children. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33(33):13251-13258
  67. 67. Powers SJ, Wang Y, Beach SD, Sideridis GD, Gaab N. Examining the relationship between home literacy environment and neural correlates of phonological processing in beginning readers with and without a familial risk for dyslexia: An fMRI study. Annals of Dyslexia. 2016;66(3):337-360
  68. 68. Raschle NM, Becker BL, Smith S, Fehlbaum LV, Wang Y, Gaab N. Investigating the influences of language delay and/or familial risk for dyslexia on brain structure in 5-year-olds. Cerebral Cortex. 2017;27(1):764-776
  69. 69. Yu X, Raney T, Perdue MV, Zuk J, Ozernov-Palchik O, Becker BLC, et al. Emergence of the neural network underlying phonological processing from the prereading to the emergent reading stage: A longitudinal study. Human Brain Mapping. 2018;39(5):2047-2063
  70. 70. Zuk J, Gaab N. Evaluating predisposition and training in shaping the musician’s brain: The need for a developmental perspective. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2018. epub
  71. 71. Hutton JS, Phelan K, Horowitz-Kraus T, Dudley J, Altaye M, DeWitt T, et al. Story time turbocharger? Child engagement during shared reading and cerebellar activation and connectivity in preschool-age children listening to stories. PLoS One. 2017;12(5):e0177398
  72. 72. Hutton JS, Phelan K, Horowitz-Kraus T, Dudley J, Altaye M, DeWitt T, et al. Shared reading quality and brain activation during story listening in preschool-age children. The Journal of Pediatrics. 2017;191:204-211. e201
  73. 73. Morken F, Helland T, Hugdahl K, Specht K. Reading in dyslexia across literacy development: A longitudinal study of effective connectivity. NeuroImage. 2017;144(Pt A):92-100
  74. 74. Friston KJ, Harrison L, Penny W. Dynamic causal modelling. NeuroImage. 2003;19(4):1273-1302
  75. 75. Wise Younger J, Tucker-Drob E, Booth JR. Longitudinal changes in reading network connectivity related to skill improvement. NeuroImage. 2017;158:90-98
  76. 76. Turkeltaub PE, Gareau L, Flowers DL, Zeffiro TA, Eden GF. Development of neural mechanisms for reading. Nature Neuroscience. 2003;6(7):767-773

Written By

Yingying Wang

Submitted: 21 October 2018 Reviewed: 06 November 2018 Published: 05 December 2018