Open access

Focusing on the Possible Role of the Cerebellum in Anxiety Disorders

Written By

Meghan D. Caulfield and Richard J. Servatius

Submitted: 24 April 2012 Published: 20 March 2013

DOI: 10.5772/52954

From the Edited Volume

New Insights into Anxiety Disorders

Edited by Federico Durbano

Chapter metrics overview

3,179 Chapter Downloads

View Full Metrics

1. Introduction

The cerebellum is traditionally thought of as the neural structure responsible for motor control, voluntary movement, balance and associative learning. However, there is a growing awareness that the cerebellum plays a role in higher cognitive functions such as sensory processing [1,2], attention [3,4], verbal working memory [5-8] and emotion [9-11]. Converging evidence suggests that the cerebellum may play a role in anxiety disorders. With the greater appreciation that anxiety disorders are best conceptualized by diathesis models of risk, cerebellar activation may represent an endophenotype contributing to anxiety etiology.

This chapter will present the role of a normal functioning cerebellum and outline instances in which abnormal functioning underlies a variety of pathologies including anxiety disorders. We will begin by describing historically accepted roles of the cerebellum in motor control, timing, and learning and memory. We will then present research relating to less appreciated roles such as executive processing and emotional control to demonstrate less recognized cognitive and emotional capacities of the cerebellum.

Key to our theory is that individual differences in cerebellar activity underlie vulnerability to develop anxiety disorders. This argument will be presented by providing an overview of pre-existing vulnerabilities contributing to a diathesis approach of anxiety. We will discuss recent research in which individual differences in cerebellar modulated activities is present, such as during associative learning, avoidance or image processing tasks. Finally, a diathesis model which incorporates cerebellar activation into the etiology and expression of anxiety disorders will be presented with a discussion of its implications and future directions.

Advertisement

2. Historically accepted roles of the cerebellum

The cerebellum is a unique neural structure that accounts for approximately 10% of the total brain volume and contains nearly half of all the neurons of the brain [12,13]. The cerebellum is highly organized, with distinct inputs and outputs. It is made up of an outer region of gray matter (the cerebellar cortex), an inner region of white matter, and three pairs of deep nuclei responsible for cerebellar output; the dentate, the fastigial, and the interposed[13]. The cerebellum is made up of two hemispheres that are structural mirror images, each containing three deep nuclei. The two hemispheres are connected medially by the vermis. For specificity, the cerebellum is segregated into sections: Crus I, Crus II, and lobules I-X ([14].

Motor Functioning. The traditional view of the cerebellum is that of a motor comparator. Muscle movement, especially coordinated and smooth motions, are the product of a feedback loop involving the cerebellum and frontal cortex. Afferent connections via the cortico-pontine-cerebellar tract with the premotor and motor cortex carry a “copy” of motor demands to the cerebellum. The cerebellum then compares feedback from the muscle spindles, joints, and tendons via the cerebellar peduncles to modify motor behavior, maintain coordination and perform skilled movements [15-17].

The essential role of the cerebellum in motor behavior is especially evident following cerebellar insult. Unlike lesions of the motor cortex, a cerebellar lesion does not eliminate movement entirely. Instead, it disrupts initiation, coordination, and timing of movements. Movement deficits following cerebellar lesions can be very precise. Some lesions affect certain muscle groups, but not others, depending on the location, revealing a precise topography in the cerebellum. For example, deterioration of the anterior cerebellum affects the lower limbs, causing a wide staggering gait, while largely sparing arm and hand movements [18-21]. Cerebellar lesions often lead to a lack of coordination, affecting the ability to perform directed movements. Damage to the vestibulocerebellum, which receives input from the vestibular nuclei, affects gross movements, such as standing upright, to fine movements, such as maintaining fixation of gaze. Spinocerebellar lesions disrupt signals from the spinal cord and affect coordination interfering with movement regulation. The spinocerebellum uses a feed-forward process to make on-line updates to ensure accurate coordinated movements. Lesions of the cerebellum cause a variety of movement disorders such as overshooting or undershooting of targets (referred to as dysmetria), poor path correction caused by poorly coordinated joint motions (known as ataxia), tremors at the end of actions [13,18,22]. Finally, insult of the cerebrocerebellum, which has afferents from the cerebral cortex, impairs planned movements and sensory input, affecting reaction time. Individuals with lesions to the cerebrocerebellum report difficulty performing directed actions. Instead of a smooth integration of movements toward a target, their actions take place as a series of several movements strung together, known as decomposition of movement [18]. Altogether, the profound and specific outcomes of cerebellar insult indicate its critical role in coordinated motor behavior, enabling smooth and accurate performance of highly specific fine motor movements.

Timing. Given its role in motor behaviors outlined above, it is not surprising that the cerebellum is essential in motor timing, which produces timed movements by coordinating velocity, acceleration and deceleration [15,23-28]. A simple way of measuring motor timing is through repetitive finger tapping tasks. Participants are asked to tap in time with a pacing device (e.g., metronome). After synchronization, the training device is removed and the individual is asked to continue tapping at the same interval. Variability in timing can then be measured in the inter-tap intervals. This simple task elucidates the essential role of the cerebellum in motor timing. Healthy participants demonstrate a significant increase in cerebellar activity (in addition to other areas related to motor timing such as the supplementary motor area and basal ganglia) during timed finger tapping [28]. Patients with lateral cerebellar lesions demonstrate increased variability when performing rhythmic tapping with the affected (ipsilateral) finger, but not when tapping with the unaffected (contralateral) finger. Interestingly, those with medial cerebellar lesions did not show timing errors, but had a greater number of motor errors, supporting involvement of the cerebellum specifically in timing and not just in producing the behavioral motor output [23].

Timing is also essential in higher cognitive functions such as stimulus processing, expectations, language, and attention. Sensory timing is often measured by duration judgment tasks, which presents two stimuli of either the same or different duration. Here, participants are required to attend to a stimulus, maintain it in working memory, compare it to a second stimulus and make a judgment. Significant increases in cerebellar activity are present during timing tasks in healthy human participants [29,30]. Additionally, the use of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS), which induces inhibition and causes a “temporary lesion” in the stimulated region, of the lateral cerebellum impaired short interval time perception in a similar task (400-600 ms) [31]. Comparable sensory timing deficits are seen in children with Ataxia Telangiectasia, a disease involving cortical degeneration affecting Purkinje and granular cell layers [32]. A similar deficit in duration judgment is seen in patients with cerebellar tumors [33]. Furthermore, the effect of cerebellar lesions on sensory timing is not specific to duration judgment tasks. Patients with cerebellar lesions display deficits in a variety of other tasks requiring sensory processing including interval discrimination [24,34], speed judgments [35,36] and verbal timing [37-41].

Eyeblink conditioning. Although the cerebellum has long been acknowledged as a motor integrator and modulator, associative learning was assumed to be accomplished by higher cortical regions. Over the latter quarter of the 20th century, Thompson and colleagues presented a body of work that the cerebellum is part of the intrinsic circuitry for eyeblink conditioning, a form of new motor learning [42-45]. The foundation of eyeblink conditioning is the simple reflex pathway; the unconditional stimulus (US) produces an unconditional response (UR). Introduction of a second stimulus (conditioned stimulus or CS) that is temporally paired with the US gives rise to a conditioned response (CR), which precedes or significantly modifies the UR. In delay conditioning, the CS precedes and coterminates with the US. Thompson recognized that the simplicity of eyeblink conditioning coupled with the ability to explicitly assess reactivity to the CS, to the US, or its combination under various conditions provided an excellent platform to understand the nature of the engram – the storage and location of a memory trace [42,46,47].

The intrinsic cerebellar circuitry demonstrates why damage to the cerebellar cortex, cerebellar nuclei, or major afferent pathways abolishes or impairs acquisition of the CR during eyeblink conditioning [48-53]. Using rats and rabbits, the neurobiology of eyeblink conditioning has been reduced to two pathways that converge in the cerebellum (For detailed reviews see [45,54]). The basic essential pathway is presented in Figure 1. Simplified, the CS pathway transmits auditory, visual, and somatosensory information via the pontine nuclei to the cerebellar cortex and interpositus nucleus via mossy fiber connections. The US pathway takes two routes from the trigeminal nucleus: a reflexive route that bypasses the cerebellum and a learning route that integrates the relationship between the CS and US. From there, climbing fibers synapse at the cerebellar cortex and interpositus nucleus. The CS and US pathways converge in the cerebellar cortex and anterior interpositus. It is here where the memory trace is stored by changes in the firing patterns of purkinje cells during the development of the CR [47,55-57]. The CR is produced by release of inhibition of the interpositus, which increases activity to the red nucleus, in turn causing the cranial motor nuclei to induce an eye blink response [58,59].

Figure 1.

Intrinsic delay eyeblink conditioning pathway. Adapted from Christian & Thompson, 2003.

Another benefit of the eyeblink conditioning paradigm is that the same parameters can be used across animal species, in humans, and even in early infancy. Consistent with the animal literature, intact cerebellar structures are necessary for the acquisition of the CR in eyeblink conditioning in humans [48-50,60]. Furthermore, imaging studies indicate that activity in the cerebellum is significantly greater during eyeblink conditioning in humans [61-65].

Given the advanced understanding of neurosubstrates and its amenability for cross species comparisons, eyeblink conditioning has been a platform for understanding clinical abnormalities and cerebellar dysfunction. Therefore, a more detailed review will be presented for eyeblink conditioning, as well as a selection of clinical examples in which a cerebellar role is revealed by eyeblink conditioning.

Cerebellar abnormalities and eyeblink conditioning. The cerebellum is particularly affected by ethanol alcohol, with alcohol-related diseases causing serious damage to its development and cells. For example, impaired delay eyeblink conditioning has been observed in Korsakoff patients, recovered alcoholics, and children with Fetal Alcohol Syndrome [66-69]. However, not all disorders cause deficits in eyeblink conditioning. For example, individuals with autism acquire eyeblink conditioning faster than matched controls, although the form of the CR is altered [70,71]. Schizophrenia also alters cerebellar functioning, with facilitated eyeblink conditioning observed in schizophrenics compared to healthy controls [72]. Some interventions can also rescue or improve cerebellar functioning. For example, improved performance in eyeblink conditioning has been observed in mice following an antioxidant rich diet over a standard diet [73].

Regardless of etiology, cerebellar abnormalities affect eyeblink conditioning. The well-documented pathways, substrates, and lesion studies makes eyeblink conditioning a simple, yet sensitive tool to understand the cerebellar role in various neuropathologies.

Advertisement

3. Higher cognitive and emotional capacities

Recently, the cerebellum has garnered greater attention for its higher cognitive capabilities. Reviews such as those from Courchesne and colleagues [3,74], Schmahmann and colleagues [75,76] and others [77-79] establish the cognitive role of the cerebellum, which will be briefly summarized here.

Anatomy. In order to have a role in higher cognitive processing the cerebellum must maintain connections with neural structures known to influence cognition. As such, cerebellar efferents have been traced to both motor and non-motor areas of the frontal cortex [80-85]. Tract-tracing studies with primates indicate that cerebellar output to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) places it in a position to modulate higher cognitive processing. Transneuronal retrograde virus tracers injected into multiple areas of the DLPFC (Brodmann areas 9, 46 and 12) labeled neurons in the dentate nucleus, indicating that the dentate has output channels to prefrontal regions [84]. The DLPFC plays an important role in many aspects of executive functioning including organization [86,87], behavioral control [87] working memory [88,89], reasoning and decision making [90], reward and expectancy [91], and emotion and motivation [92]. Follow up studies were able to pinpoint lateral dentate projections to the prefrontal cortex (PFC), with separate dorsal dentate projections terminating in the motor and premotor regions, suggesting a topographic organization of the dentate nucleus with both motor and non-motor output to the cortex [93].

Functional connectivity. Cerebellar connectivity to non-motor cognitive areas in human imaging research reflects pathways implicated in primate studies. Functional connectivity MRI correlates signal fluctuations in one brain area with activity in another, implying a relationship between the two areas. Using this method, Allen et al. [94] found that activity in the dentate nucleus of the cerebellum correlated with changes in activity in non-motor regions such as the limbic system, parietal lobes, and prefrontal cortex. Connectivity between the cerebellum and anterior cingulate cortex, a region typically associated with error detection, anticipation, attention, and emotional responses, has also been reported in resting state paradigms [95]. Furthermore, there is evidence that the cerebellum contributes to the intrinsic connectivity networks, a series of brain structures that correspond to basic functions such as vision, audition, language, episodic memory, executive functioning, and salience detection [11]. Distinct contributions of the neocerebellum to the default mode network, the executive network, and the salience network substantiate the assertion that there is functional connectivity between the cerebellum and non-motor cognitive regions.

Clinical support for a cerebellar role in non-motor cognitive processes is established by the work of Schmahmann and colleagues. Schmahmann recognized that not all patients with cerebellar strokes present with motor deficits. By assessing motor impairments alongside stroke location, he found that individuals with posterior lobe lesions presented with minor if at any motor impairments. Instead, they suffered from behavioral changes affecting executive functioning, verbal fluency, working memory, abstract reasoning, spatial memory, personality, and language deficits; recently coined as cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome [96,97].

Loss of function in lesions is supported by activational studies in healthy humans. Using functional MRI, significant changes in cerebellar activity is present during tasks that are considered largely cognitive or to involve executive processing. Significant increases in cerebellar activity have been recorded during sensory timing [29,30], spatial attention [98-101], and verbal working memory tasks [5,6,102].

Anatomical and functional connectivity, specific activation during executive processing tasks, and impairments concomitant with lesions is convincing evidence that the cerebellum plays a critical role in higher cognitive processing.

Emotions. In addition to connections with prefrontal and frontal cortex, the cerebellum also has direct anatomical connections to the amygdala, the brain region typically associated with emotion and fear [103]. Functional support for this connectivity comes from imaging studies that demonstrate judging emotional intonation, feeling empathy, experiencing sadness, and viewing emotional pictures all correlate with increased activity in the cerebellum [9,76,104-106].

If the cerebellum has important connections to the limbic system, then it follows that stimulation of the cerebellum should result in changes of emotional behaviors. As such, electrical stimulation of the cerebellum in animals demonstrates that it is an important modulator of behaviors classically attributed to limbic functioning including grooming, eating, and sham rage [107-109]. Bernston et al. [107] reported that stimulating the cerebellum of cats induced grooming and eating behaviors, in addition to similar findings with rats [108,109]. The cerebellum, specifically the vermis, plays a role in fear and avoidant behaviors. For example, lesioning the vermis alters fear responses by decreasing freezing and increasing open field exploration [110]. On other other hand, stimulating the vermis induces fear responses, such as increased amplitude of the acoustic startle response [111], indicating cerebellar modulation of species-specific behaviors beyond coordination of muscle movements.

Reports from the clinical literature also support cerebellar modulation of emotion. Attempts to treat severe seizure disorders by stimulating the cerebellum provide unique case reports of observations about cerebellar functioning. Heath et al. [112] placed electrodes in the fastigial nucleus of an emotionally disturbed patient and observed increased activity in the region when the patient reported being angry or fearful. Descriptions of unpleasant sensations and the feeling of being scared were reported following stimulation of the dentate nucleus [113]. In a larger study of cerebellar stimulation as a treatment for chronic epilepsy, Cooper et al. [114] reported marked behavioral changes from sullen mood, dangerous, and aggressive behaviors to open, pleasant, responsive, and sociable affect in patients. More recently, descriptions of highly specific lesions to the cerebellar vermis includes personality changes, especially emotional effects such as flattening of affect [97,115]. Observations from these case studies suggest that the cerebellum may utilize its reciprocal connections with the prefrontal cortex and limbic system to modulate emotional processing.

Cerebellum and Anxiety Disorders

Anxiety. Anxiety is the most prevalent disorder in the United States with one quarter of the population estimated to develop an anxiety disorder at some time in their lives [116,117]. On the other hand, three quarters of the population does not suffer from clinical anxiety, raising the question what is it about an individual that makes them more likely to develop an anxiety disorder? Unfortunately, there is no single vulnerability increasing risk for anxiety. Instead, anxiety disorders are best represented by diathesis models, that is, preexisting conditions enhance risk such that individuals are vulnerable to environmental insults or challenges. A stress-diathesis model for anxiety disorders emphasizes changes in stress reactivity from the convergence of a variety of factors such as genetics, biology, sex, and prior experience [118]. Current research efforts heavily focus on the higher cortical areas (e.g., prefrontal cortex, cingulate cortex, hippocampus, amygdala) as areas critical to development of anxiety. However, the cerebellum is also intimately involved in emotional processing, learning and memory – all of which are represented as risk factors in diathesis models. The following sections will describe how cerebellar activity is related to the signs and symptoms of anxiety and provide often overlooked evidence of cerebellar involvement from imaging research. This will form the basis for speculations regarding individual differences in cerebellar activity as a risk factor for anxiety disorders.

Avoidance. Avoidance is the core feature in the otherwise varied symptomology of anxiety disorders [119]. Therefore, it is essential to understand the role abnormal expressions of avoidance plays in the development and maintenance of anxiety. First, avoidance is acquired and reinforced over time. The essence of anxiety is concern over a potential threatening event in the future, typically one which the individual feels they have no control over and could not cope with. Rather than deal with uncontrollable events, anxious individuals choose to exert their control by substituting other negative thoughts or feelings that are avoidable, providing short term relief and a feeling of temporary control. Avoidance can either be active or passive. In active avoidance, the individual learns to control their environment by alleviating or removing a noxious stimulus. In passive avoidance, the individual learns not to place themselves in a situation that previously contained a noxious stimulus. In anxiety, both forms of avoidance are present, and over time, become pervasive and uncontrollable such that normal functioning becomes impossible.

Avoidance is a learned process. Therefore, it is possible to measure the differences in acquisition of the negative reinforcement learning seen in active-avoidance. Differences in the speed and strength of acquisition in active-avoidance may contribute to risk or resiliency. Some individuals may be more susceptible to acquire and repeatedly express active-avoidance behaviors, leading to development of behavioral and cognitive avoidance symptoms associated with anxiety disorders.

Although the cerebellum is typically associated with associative learning using classical conditioning protocols, a cerebellar role in operant learning such as avoidance has also been suggested. For example, lever press avoidance paradigms places a rat in an operant chamber and presents a stimulus (e.g., tone) that precedes and overlaps with an aversive stimulus (e.g., a shock). Over time, the rat learns to make a lever press response to the tone, avoiding the shock. Lesioning the cerebellum prevents acquisition of the avoidance response in this task [120] and in other measures of active-avoidance [121]. Furthermore, cerebellar involvement may play a role in human avoidance as well [122].

Neuropharmacology. Given the role of the cerebellum and associative learning in anxiety vulnerability, it would be useful to consider treatment approaches that target the cerebellum. Among others, the cerebellum maintains a large density of corticotrophin-releasing hormone (CRH) receptors and cannabinoid receptors. Here, we will outline how these receptors relate to anxiety and eyeblink conditioning.

The influence of CRH on various behavioral markers of anxiety demonstrates its role in modulating stress reactivity. CRH has anxiogenic properties, with a dysregulation of CRH systems playing a role in anxiety disorders. The cerebellum contains a high density of CRH1 receptors, the receptor linked to stress responding, anxious behavior and cognitive functioning [123]. The effects of CRH receptor activation have been thoroughly outlined using animal models, including its influence on anxiety (for a review see [124]). For example, an injection of corticotropin releasing factor (CRF), which induces corticosterone release (the animal analog of cortisol), has been shown to decrease open field exploration, time spent in open arms in the elevated plus maze, exploration in novel environments, and social interaction in rats at certain doses. Furthermore, injections of CRF increase startle amplitude, and improve acquisition in both active and passive avoidance paradigms. Additionally, CRH receptors are adaptive to environmental demands, with a variety of stressors upregulating CRH1 receptors specifically, suggesting a relationship to chronic stress that may feed forward into anxiety disorders [125,126]. Eyeblink conditioning is also influenced by CRH, with studies demonstrating facilitated acquisition in trace paradigms of both humans and rats [127-129]. Humans treated with metyrapone, which decreases initial cortisol response to stress (although not long term effects of stress [130], acquired trace eyeblink conditioning faster than placebo treated controls. While there were no acquisition differences between the groups in delay-type conditioning, metyrapone treated individuals were significantly slower to extinguish, a difference not seen in the trace group [128]. Altogether, it appears that stress reactivity in the brain impacts cerebellar functioning and may play a role in modulating learning and memory, feeding into anxious behavior and increased vulnerability to anxiety disorders.

Cannabinoid receptors, which have their highest densities in the frontal cortex and cerebellum, have also been linked to anxiety [131-133]. Low doses of cannabinoid compounds induce anxiolitic effects, with high doses causing anxiety-like reactions in laboratory rats, suggesting interplay between cannabinoid receptor activity and anxiety [134-136]. These findings are in conjunction with subjective reports that exposure to cannabis derivatives can induce feelings of placid relaxation or panic [137]. For example, low doses of a cannabinoid synthetic reduces behaviors linked to stress in rats with high doses of the same drug causing the opposite pattern, inducing anxiety to novelty and increasing corticosterone [135]. Aside from synthetic activation, endogenous cannabinoid receptor activity is related to anxiety as well. Pharmacological blockage of the CB1 cannabinoid receptor increased anxiety-like behaviors in rats including reduced open arm exploration in the elevated plus maze and increased withdrawal-related behaviors [132]. Cannabinoids influence anxiety and have a high density of receptors in the cerebellum, suggesting that cannabinoid receptor activation would influence eyeblink conditioning as well. As such, animal models have demonstrated that CB1 knockout mice demonstrate disrupted eyeblink conditioning [138] In conjunction, humans who report chronic cannabis use (but not at the time of the study) exhibit fewer and poorly timed CRs during delay eyeblink conditioning compared to non-users [139].

Temperament differences contributes to anxiety vulnerability

Diathesis models suggest that the interplay between risk factors increases vulnerability to develop anxiety disorders. Personality is among the many risk factors suggested to play a role in anxiety, with certain personality types at increased risk to develop anxiety disorders. Support for a personality risk factor in anxiety is supported by the low success rates in treating anxiety disorders, which would require the alteration of stable character traits. Of the few studies that have assessed long-term treatment outcomes of anxiety disorders, 30%-50% still have moderate to severe anxiety six years post treatment [140,141].

An understanding of how personality interacts with anxiety is essential. Here, we will discuss an innate feature of personality known as temperament. Temperament is a core feature of personality, often evident early in childhood and remains stable throughout the lifespan. By measuring temperaments related to anxiety such as behavioral inhibition (BI) and trait anxiety, we are able to differentiate at-risk individuals and assess individual differences on cerebellar modulated tasks.

Behavioral inhibition. Similar to anxiety disorders, a core feature of behavioral inhibition is avoidance. Additionally, the behavioral and physiological functioning of an individual with behavioral inhibition is comparable to that seen in anxiety including withdrawal, apprehension, and slow latency to approach unfamiliar people or objects [142]. Kagan and colleagues have provided an extensive behavioral profile of BI using longitudinal methods, reporting that children classified as inhibited at 21-months demonstrate avoidance of social interactions [143], reported more phobias, and had a higher incidence of anxiety disorders [144-147]. As with anxiety disorders, it appears that inhibited temperament is a heritable trait [148]. Parents and siblings of those children classified as inhibited were more likely to have anxiety disorders, social phobia, avoidant and overanxious disorders compared to the families of uninhibited children [149-151].

So far, we have provided evidence supporting that cerebellar differences underlie higher cognitive processes including anxiety disorders. We have outlined the essential role avoidance has in the development and maintenance of anxiety disorders and how learning processes may underlie increased avoidance. We then introduced behaviorally inhibited temperament, a risk factor with many similarities to anxiety. In the next section we will combine individual differences in cerebellar functioning, avoidance, learning, and temperament to provide a cerebellar diathesis theory of anxiety vulnerability.

As described above, avoidance in the development of anxiety disorders is a feed-forward process, such that the expression of avoidance reduces stress in the present while simultaneously increasing the aversiveness of the undesired stimulus or state in the future, increasing the likelihood of continued avoidance behaviors. Both adaptive and pathological avoidance can be described in terms of the degree and rigidity of expression, the sensitivity to acquire stimulus to stimulus associations, and inflexibility to change. Multiple processes underlie avoidance acquisition, making it difficult to tease out the essential factors in anxiety. It is possible that increased sensitivity to the cues and contingencies in the environment are learned faster in anxiety, resulting in better performance on avoidance tasks. One way to measure these associations is through the classically conditioned eyeblink response. The use of eyeblink conditioning allows multiple measures to be taken into account including reactivity, acquisition of the relationship between the CS and US, and rate of extinction.

Learning. The inbred Wistar-Kyoto rat (WKY) provides a model of inherent anxiousness and vulnerability to stress, similar to what is seen in a behaviorally inhibited personality profile [152-160]. Furthermore, the WKY demonstrates enhanced active avoidance in lever-press paradigms, reinforcing the relationship between anxiety vulnerability and avoidance [161,162]. Comparisons of WKY male rats to outbred Sprague-Dawley male rats demonstrate significantly faster acquisition and greater asymptotic performance of the WKY [163,164]. Moreover, avoidance perseverates in WKY during extinction training in the presence of safety signals [159] or avoidance acquisition with more intense stressors [165]. As reviewed by Jiao [166], the WKY provides an animal model of inhibited temperament, faster associative learning, enhanced sensitivity to acquire avoidance, and resistance to extinction. Moreover, the reactivity increases in the face of avoidance acquisition, reminiscent of increased reactivity in PTSD [167].

Striking parallels are evident between rat models of anxiety vulnerable temperament and humans with self-reported inhibited temperament, suggesting a common neural substrate. One way to assess at-risk temperament is through self-report scales such as those that measure behavioral inhibition [168,169] or trait anxiety [170]. Using these measures, our lab has found that at-risk individuals acquire the relationship between the CS and US the faster, demonstrating more CRs earlier in the training period than those who are low scoring [171,172]. For example, a recent study with a large sample of 117 healthy college-age students found that those scoring high on the Adult Measure of Behavioural Inhibition [169] and Trait Anxiety [170] acquired standard delay eyeblink conditioning faster than those who scored below the median on these measures (see Figure 2). Considering the intimate relationship between associative learning of cues as predictors of aversive events, enhanced classical conditioning may reflect increased sensivity to acquire avoidance responses.

These and other similar results [171,173,174] suggest that individual differences in acquisition of learning tasks may reflect processes underlying increased risk for anxiety disorders.

Figure 2.

A comparison of temperament on delay eyeblink acquisition of healthy college-aged students. Those who score above the median on the AMBI and STAI-Trait are considered high scorers, those below are considered low scorers Anxiety vulnerable individuals acquired eyeblink conditioning faster and to a greater degree over the 45 trial training period (blocks 1-9). There were no observed differences in extinction (E1-E3).

Heart Rate. In addition to higher cortical pathways, the cerebellum also has direct reciprocal connections to the hypothalamus. Studies in rats and primates show projections from the deep cerebellar nuclei to the lateral hypothalamus, posterior hypothalamic area, dorsal hypothalamic area, the paraventricular nucleus, and the dorsomedial hypothalamic nucleus (For a review see [175]), some of which may be related to heart rate reactivity.

Research in behaviorally inhibited children indicate that a high and stable heart rate (compared to uninhibited children) is indicative of long-term inhibited temperament. Reduced resting heart rate variability has been revealed as a feature of perceived stress [176] and anxiety disorders such as PTSD [177]. The presentation of novel or negative stimuli in healthy populations results in large bradycardic response, with greater bradycardia to more negatively valenced images [178,179]. While there appears to be a relationship between heart rate and anxiety, few studies have looked at heart rate reactivity in behaviorally inhibited adults. Studies that manipulate heart rate typically do so with negatively valenced pictures, assessing reactivity to extreme stimuli (i.e., trauma images for a PTSD patient). In order to disentangle individual reactivity from heart rate changes during high-arousal image processing, which can cause large responses in everyone, a recent study from our lab assessed heart rate change in high and low BI individuals when viewing images that were low in arousal across positive negative and neutral valence. Using this design, we could better understand how BI influences reactivity to everyday stimuli normally encountered in the environment to see if inhibition is related to aberrant parasympathetic or sympathetic activation. Recordings of 6 seconds before, 6 seconds during, and 6 seconds after image presentation suggest a sustained bradycardia in inhibited individuals compared to their non-inhibited counterparts. It is possible that greater vagal tone in high BI could also be related to the enhanced eyeblink acquisition seen in behaviorally inhibited individuals in across studies in Veterans, high school aged students, as well as college aged individuals (See Figure 3).

Figure 3.

Heart rate change from baseline for positive, neutral, and negative images in high and low behavioral inhibition. Each block represents 20 trials. Behaviorally inhibited individuals showed sutained bradycardia over the neutral picture viewing session. Bradycardia lasted only through the first block of 20 trials in the positive condition, and appeared in the second block (trials 21-40) in the negative condition.

Cerebellar reactivity. Despite being largely ignored and generally not discussed, imaging studies repeatedly indicate significant changes in cerebellar activity of patients with anxiety disorders compared to healthy controls. Close examination of the reported data reveals significant changes in the cerebellum during resting state and anxiety-provoking tasks in social anxiety disorder [180-182], post-traumatic stress disorder [183-187], obsessive compulsive disorder [188] and generalized anxiety disorder [189,190].

Individual differences in cerebellar reactivity have recently been extended to include anxiety vulnerability. Numerous studies assess the correlation between measures of anxiety vulnerability, most often trait anxiety, and brain activity [191]. Mostly, these studies report that individual differences in amygdala and PFC activity underlies trait anxiety, modulating stimulus processing and increasing hypervigilance [192-195]. What is often overlooked is that reciprocal connections between the cerebellum, prefrontal cortex, and amygdala position the cerebellum to modulate reactivity in anxiety vulnerable individuals. In the only published study to date to our knowledge that discusses cerebellar activity and temperament, Blackford and colleagues [10] compared behaviorally inhibited to uninhibited individuals when viewing familiar and novel faces and found significant increases in BOLD activation in the right cerebellum of the inhibited individuals when viewing novel faces. Specifically, they reported significant increases in the right Crus 1/Lobule VI region of the cerebellum, which may be related to processing the valence of emotional cues, salience detection, and in sensory processing and expectation; especially pain-related processes like fear and startle reactions [2,11,76].

The cerebellar differences found in the Blackford study were the result of a full-brain analysis; importantly, standard imaging procedures often incompletely image the cerebellum, so it is possible that the entire structure is not included in typical analyses. Recent research in our lab has explored the relationship of cerebellar activity and anxious temperament as measured by behavioral inhibition and trait anxiety. To extend the Blackford study we again used familiar faces and novel faces. Additionally, we used familiar and novel scenes, allowing us to differentiate the effect of social stimuli and novelty. Furthermore, we used the cerebellum as our region of interest, ensuring complete coverage during imaging. Finally, participants underwent eyeblink conditioning in addition to imaging (outside of the scanner). Given what is known about the behavioral profile of behaviorally inhibited individuals and in light of previous research, we hypothesized that high behavioral inhibition would correlate with changes in cerebellar activity, with the strongest differences occurring to novel faces. We found that the group with higher scores on measures of behavioral inhibition [168,169] had greater cerebellar reactivity to the novel faces compared to baseline than those with lower scores, a difference not seen with familiar faces. Additionally, we observed greater activity of the high BI group when viewing novel scenes, suggesting that the cerebellum may be sensitive to novel stimuli in general. Differences in percent signal change and BOLD signal activations can be seen in figure 4. In eyeblink conditioning, individuals with high BI scores acquired delay eyeblink faster than those with low scores, replicating previous work in our lab.

Figure 4.

Increased cerebellar reactivity to novel stimuli in anxiety vulnerable individuals. Healthy, college-aged students who scored high on measures of behavioral inhibition demonstrated increased reactivity to multiple areas of the cerebellum in response to novel faces compared to baseline. A similar differential increase in activity was seen for novel scenes. Significant differences in cerebellar activity from baseline were not seen in the familiar face or familiar scene conditions. Left is Right.

We have demonstrated individual differences in cerebellar reactivity and behavior in cerebellar-modulated tasks related to anxiety and anxiety vulnerability. By modulating the signal from higher cortical areas, the cerebellum may be involved in processes related to emotion and anxiety. Figure 5 outlines the cerebrocerebellar and corticopontinecerebellar circuitry as well as the cerebellar outputs for eyeblink conditioning, heart rate responsivity, and higher cognitive process. Cerebellar outputs to prefrontal regions such as the DLPFC and ACC would allow it to modulate incoming signals to these areas regarding higher cognitive functioning including emotion and anxiety. The anatomical pathways, functional connectivity, and individual differences observed of both clinical anxiety and anxiety vulnerable individuals suggest a cerebellar role in anxiety disorders. We propose that cerebellar functioning is another risk factor that needs to be added to the diathesis of anxiety vulnerability. Continued research of individual differences in both cerebellar-modulated tasks (e.g., eyeblink) and the cerebellar role in higher cognitive tasks (e.g., stimulus processing, attention; emotional regulation) will shed light on the interplay of vulnerabilities contributing to the development of anxiety disorders.

Figure 5.

Cerebellar functional connectivity. Reciprocal connectivity with the cortex puts the cerebellum in a position to modulate higher cognitive processes via connections with the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC). Many functions altered by at-risk temperament may be modulated by the cerebellum including eyeblink conditioning, heart rate reactivity, and executive functioning such as emotional regulation, motivation and avoidance.

References

  1. 1. Gao H. J. Parsons L. M. Bower J. M. Xiong J. Li J. Fox P. T. 1996 Cerebellum implicated in sensory acquisition and discrimination rather than motor control. Science.272 5261 545 7
  2. 2. Parsons L. M. Denton D. Egan G. Mc Kinley M. Shade R. Lancaster J. et al. 2000 Neuroimaging evidence implicating cerebellum in support of sensory/cognitive processes associated with thirst. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 97 5 2332 36
  3. 3. Akshoomoff N. A. Courchesne E. 1992 A new role for the cerebellum in cognitive operations. Behavioral Neuroscience 106 5 731 8
  4. 4. Moberget T. Karns C. M. Deouell L. Y. Lindgren M. Knight R. T. Ivry R. B. 2008 Detecting violations of sensory expectancies following cerebellar degeneration: A mismatch negativity study. Neuropsychologia 46 2569 79
  5. 5. Hayter A. L. Langdon D. W. 2007 Cerebellar contributions to working memory. Neuroimage 36 3 943 54
  6. 6. Kirschen M. P. Annabel Chen. S. H. Desmond J. E. 2010 Modality specific cerebro-cerebellar activations in verbal working memory: An fMRI study. Behavioural Neurology 23 1-2 51 63
  7. 7. Marvel C. L. Desmond J. E. 2010 topography of the cerebellum in verbal working memory. Neuropsychology Review 20 3 271 9
  8. 8. Marvel C. L. Desmond J. E. 2010 The contributions of cerebro-cerebellar circuitry to executive verbal working memory. Cortex 46 7 880 95
  9. 9. Liotti M. Mayberg H. S. Brannan S. K. Mc Ginnis S. 2000 Differential limbic-cortical correlates of sadness and anxiety in healthy subjects: implications for affective disorders. Biological Psychiatry 48 1 30 42
  10. 10. Blackford J. U. Avery S. N. Shelton R. C. Zald D. H. 2009 Amygdala temporal dynamics: temperamental differences in the timing of amygdala response to familiar and novel faces. BMC Neuroscience 10 1 145
  11. 11. Habas C. Kamdar N. Nguyen D. Prater K. Beckmann C. F. Menon V. et al. 2009 Distinct cerebellar contributions to intrinsic connectivity networks. Journal of Neuroscience 29 26 8586 94
  12. 12. Ellis R. S. 1920 A quantitative study of the purkinje cells in human cerebella. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 51 6 576
  13. 13. Ghez C. Thach W. T. The cerebellum. Kandel ER, Schwartz JH, Jessel TM, editors. Principles of Neural Science. 4th ed. New York: McGraw-HIll 832 852
  14. 14. Apps R. Hawkes R. 2009 Cerebellar cortical organization: a one-map hypothesis Nature Reviews Neuroscience 10 9 670 81
  15. 15. Brooks V. B. Thach T. W. 2011 Cerebellar control of posture and movement. Comprehensive Physiology
  16. 16. Ito M. 1984 The cerebellum and neural motor control. New York: Raven
  17. 17. Houk J. C. Keifer J. Barto A. G. 1993 Distributed motor commands in the limb premotor network. Trends in Neurosciences 16 1 27 33
  18. 18. Holmes G. 1939 The cerebellum of man Brain 62 1 1 30
  19. 19. Victor M. Adams R. D. 1959 A restricted form of cerebellar cortical degeneration occurring in alcoholic patients. Archives of Neurology 1 6 579 688
  20. 20. Dichgans J. 1996 Cerebellar and spinocerebellar gait disorders. In Bronstein AM, Brant T, Woollacott M, eds: Clinical Disorders of Balance Posture and Gait. London: Hodder Arnold Publishers 147 155
  21. 21. Morton S. M. Bastian A. J. 2004 Cerebellar control of balance and locomotion. Neuroscientist 10 3 247 59
  22. 22. Fredericks C. M. 1996 Disorders of the cerebellum and its connections. In Fredericks CM, Saladin LK. eds: Pathophysiology of the Motor Systems: Principles and Clinical Presentations. FA Davis Co
  23. 23. Ivry R. Keele S. 1988 Dissociation of the lateral and medial cerebellum in movement timing and movement execution. Exp Brain Res 73 167
  24. 24. Ivry R. Keele S. 1989 Timing functions of the cerebellum. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 1 2 136 52
  25. 25. Bloedel J. R. Bracha V. Larson P. S. 1993 Real time operations of the cerebellar cortex. Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences 20 3 S7 18
  26. 26. Raymond J. L. Lisberger S. G. 1996 The cerebellum: a neuronal learning machine? Science 272 1126 1131
  27. 27. Mauk M. D. Medina J. F. Nores W. L. Ohyama T. 2000 Cerebellar function: coordination, learning or timing? Current Biology 10 14 R522 5
  28. 28. Witt S. T. Laird A. R. Meyerand M. E. 2008 Functional neuroimaging correlates of finger-tapping task variations: an ALE meta-analysis. Neuroimage 42 1 343 56
  29. 29. Jueptner M. Rijntjes M. Weiller C. Faiss J. H. Timmann D. Mueller S. P. et al. 1995 Localization of a cerebellar timing process using PET. Neurology 45 8 1540 5
  30. 30. O’Reilly J. X. Mesulam M. M. Nobre A. C. 2008 The cerebellum predicts the timing of perceptual events. The Journal of Neuroscience 28 9 2252 60
  31. 31. Koch G. Oliveri M. Torriero S. Salerno S. Gerfo Lo. E. Caltagirone C. 2007 Repetitive TMS of cerebellum interferes with millisecond time processing. Experimental Brain Research 179 2 291 9
  32. 32. Mostofsky S. H. Kunze J. C. Cutting L. E. Lederman H. M. Denckla M. B. 2000 Judgment of duration in individuals with ataxia-telangiectasia. Developmental Neuropsychology 17 1 63 74
  33. 33. Hetherington R. Dennis M. Spiegler B. 2000 Perception and estimation of time in long-term survivors of childhood posterior fossa tumors. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 6 6 682 692
  34. 34. Malapani C. Dubois B. Rancurel G. Gibbon J. 1998 Cerebellar dysfunctions of temporal processing in the seconds range in humans. Neuroreport 9 17 3907 12
  35. 35. Ivry R. B. 1991 Impaired velocity perception in patients with lesions of the cerebellum. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 3 4 355 66
  36. 36. Nawrot M. Rizzo M. 1995 Motion perception deficits from midline cerebellar lesions in human Vision Research 35 5 723 31
  37. 37. Kent R. D. Netsell R. Abbs J. H. 1979 Acoustic characteristics of dysarthria associated with cerebellar disease. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 22 3 627 48
  38. 38. Gentil M. 1990 Dysarthria in Friedreich disease. Brain and Language 38 3 438 48
  39. 39. Gentil M. 1990 EMG analysis of speech production of patients with Friedreich disease Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 4 2 107 20
  40. 40. Ackermann H. 1993 Dysarthria in Friedreich’s ataxia: timing of speech segments. Clinical linguistics & Phonetics 7 1 75 91
  41. 41. Ackermann H. Hertrich I. Daum I. Scharf G. Spieker S. 1997 Kinematic analysis of articulatory movements in central motor disorders. Movement Disorders 12 6 1019 27
  42. 42. Swain R. A. Thompson R. F. 1993 In search of engrams. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 702 27 39
  43. 43. Thompson R. F. Kim J. J. 1996 Memory systems in the brain and localization of a memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 93 24 13438 44
  44. 44. Thompson R. F. Bao S. Chen L. Cipriano B. D. Grethe J. S. Kim J. J. et al. 1997 Associative learning. International Review of Neurobiology 41 151 89
  45. 45. Christian K. M. Thompson R. F. 2003 Neural Substrates of Eyeblink Conditioning: Acquisition and Retention. Learning & Memory 10 6 427 55
  46. 46. Thompson R. F. 1976 The search for the engram. American Psychologist 31 3 209 27
  47. 47. Mc Cormick D. A. 1981 The engram found? Role of the cerebellum in classical conditioning of nictitating membrane and eyelid responses Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 18 32 3 103 5
  48. 48. Solomon P. Stowe G. Pendlebeury W. W. 1989 Disrupted eyelid conditioning in a patient with damage to cerebellar afferents. Behavioral Neuroscience 103 4 898 902
  49. 49. Daum I. Schugens M. Ackermann H. Lutzenberger W. Dichgans J. Birbaumer N. 1993 Classical conditioning after cerebellar lesions in humans. Behavioral Neuroscience 107 5 748 56
  50. 50. Topka H. Valls-Solé J. Massaquoi S. Hallett M. 1993 Deficit in classical conditioning in patients with cerebellar degeneration. Brain 116 961 9
  51. 51. Sears L. L. Steinmetz J. E. 1990 Acquisition of classically conditioned-related activity in the hippocampus is affected by lesions of the cerebellar interpositus nucleus. Behavioral Neuroscience 104 5 681 92
  52. 52. Steinmetz J. E. Lavond D. G. Ivkovich D. Logan C. G. Thompson R. F. 1992 Disruption of classical eyelid conditioning after cerebellar lesions: damage to a memory trace system or a simple performance deficit? The Journal of Neuroscience 12 11 4403 26
  53. 53. Ivkovich D. Lockard J. M. Thompson R. F. 1993 Interpositus lesion abolition of the eyeblink conditioned response is not due to effects on performance. Behavioral Neuroscience 107 3 530 2
  54. 54. Steinmetz J. E. 2000 Brain substrates of classical eyeblink conditioning: a highly localized but also distributed system. Behavioural Brain Research 110 13 24
  55. 55. Mc Cormick D. A. Clark G. A. Lavond D. G. Thompson R. F. 1982 Initial localization of the memory trace for a basic form of learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 79 8 2731 5
  56. 56. Mc Cormick D. A. Thompson R. F. 1984 Cerebellum: essential involvement in the classically conditioned eyelid response. Science 223 4633 296 9
  57. 57. Thompson R. F. 1986 The neurobiology of learning and memory. Science 233 4767 941 47
  58. 58. Rosenfield M. E. Moore J. W. 1983 Red nucleus lesions disrupt the classically conditioned nictitating membrane response in rabbits. Behavioural Brain Research 10 2-3 393 8
  59. 59. Rosenfield M. E. Moore J. W. 1985 Red nucleus lesions impair acquisition of the classically conditioned nictitating membrane response but not eye-to-eye savings or unconditioned response amplitude. Behavioural Brain Research 17 1 77 81
  60. 60. Schugens M. M. Egerter R. Daum I. Schepelmann K. Kockgether T. Loschmann P. A. 1997 The NMDA antagonist memantine impairs classical eyeblink conditioning in humans. Neuroscience Letters 224 57 60
  61. 61. Logan C. G. Grafton S. T. 1995 Functional anatomy of human eyeblink conditioning determined with regional cerebral glucose metabolism and positron-emission tomography. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 92 16 7500 4
  62. 62. Blaxton T. A. Zeffiro T. A. Gabrieli J. D. E. Bookheimer S. Y. Carrillo M. C. Theodore W. H. et al. 1996 Functional mapping of human learning: A positron emission tomography activation study of eyeblink conditioning. The Journal of Neuroscience 16 12 4032 40
  63. 63. Knuttinen M. G. Parrish T. B. Weiss C. La Bar K. S. Gitelman D. R. Power J. M. et al. 2002 Electromyography as a recording system for eyeblink conditioning with functional magnetic resonance imaging Neuroimage 17 2 977 87
  64. 64. Knight D. C. Cheng D. T. Smith C. N. Stein E. A. Helmstetter F. J. 2004 Neural substrates mediating human delay and trace fear conditioning. The Journal of Neuroscience 24 1 218 28
  65. 65. Cheng D. T. Disterhoft J. F. Power J. M. Ellis D. A. Desmond J. E. 2008 Neural substrates underlying human delay and trace eyeblink conditioning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 105 23 8108 13
  66. 66. Mc Glinchey-Berroth R. Cermak L. S. Carrillo M. C. Armfield S. JD Gabrieli Disterhoft. J. F. 1995 Impaired delay eyeblink conditioning in amnesic Korsakoff’s patients and recovered alcoholics Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research 19 5 1127 32
  67. 67. Coffin J. M. Baroody S. Schneider K. O’Neill J. 2005 Impaired cerebellar learning in children with prenatal alcohol exposure: a comparative study of eyeblink conditioning in children with ADHD and dyslexia. Cortex 41 3 389 98
  68. 68. Jacobson S. W. Stanton M. E. Molteno C. D. Burden M. J. Fuller D. S. Hoyme H. E. et al. 2008 Impaired eyeblink conditioning in children with fetal alcohol syndrome. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research 32 2 365 72
  69. 69. Jacobson S. W. Stanton M. E. Dodge N. C. Pienaar M. Fuller D. S. Molteno C. D. et al. 2011 Impaired delay and trace eyeblink conditioning in school-age children with fetal alcohol syndrome. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research 35 2 250 64
  70. 70. Sears L. L. Finn P. R. Steinmetz J. E. 1994 Abnormal classical eye-blink conditioning in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 24 6 737 51
  71. 71. Steinmetz L. 2000 Classical eyeblink conditioning in normal and autistic children. Eyeblink classical conditioning I143 162
  72. 72. Sears L. L. Andreasen N. C. O’Leary D. S. 2000 Cerebellar functional abnormalities in schizophrenia are suggested by classical eyeblink conditioning. Biological Psychiatry 48 3 204 9
  73. 73. Cartford M. C. Gemma C. 2002 Eighteen-month-old Fischer 344 rats fed a spinach-enriched diet show improved delay classical eyeblink conditioning and reduced expression of tumor necrosis. The Journal of Neuroscience 22 14 5813 6
  74. 74. Courchesne E. Allen G. 1997 Prediction and preparation, fundamental functions of the cerebellum. Learning & Memory 4 1 1 35
  75. 75. Schmahmann J. D. 1996 From Movement to Thought: Anatomic Substrates of the Cerebellar Contribution to Cognitive Processing. Human Brain Mapping 4 174 98
  76. 76. Stoodley C. Schmahmann J. 2009 Functional topography in the human cerebellum: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies Neuroimage 44 2 489 501
  77. 77. Strick P. L. Dum R. P. Fiez J. A. 2009 Cerebellum and nonmotor function. Annual Review of Neuroscience 32 413 34
  78. 78. Desmond J. E. Fiez J. A. 1998 Neuroimaging studies of the cerebellum: Language, learning and memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2 9 355 62
  79. 79. Steinlin M. 2007 The cerebellum in cognitive processes: Supporting studies in children. The cerebellum 6 3 237 41
  80. 80. Brodal P. Bjaalie J. G. Aas J. E. 1991 Organization of cingulo-ponto-cerebellar connections in the cat. Anatomy and Embryology 184 3 245 54
  81. 81. Middleton F. A. Strick P. L. 1994 Anatomical evidence for cerebellar and basal ganglia involvement in higher cognitive function. Science 266 5184 458 61
  82. 82. Schmahmann J. D. Pandya D. N. 1995 Prefrontal cortex projections to the basilar pons in rhesus monkey: implications for the cerebellar contribution to higher function. Neuroscience Letters 199 175 8
  83. 83. Middleton F. A. Strick P. L. 1997 Cerebellar output channels. International Review of Neurobiology 41 61 82
  84. 84. Middleton F. A. Strick P. L. 2001 Cerebellar projections to the prefrontal cortex of the primate. The Journal of Neuroscience 21 2 700 12
  85. 85. Kelly R. M. Strick P. L. 2003 Cerebellar loops with motor cortex and prefrontal cortex of a nonhuman primate. The Journal of Neuroscience 23 23 8432 44
  86. 86. Fuster J. M. 2000 Executive frontal functions. Experimental Brain Research 133 66 70
  87. 87. Bor D. Duncan J. Wiseman R. J. Owen A. M. 2003 Encoding strategies dissociate prefrontal activity from working memory demand. Neuron 37 2 361 7
  88. 88. Goldman-Rakic P. S. 1987 Circuitry of primate prefrontal cortex and regulation of behavior by representational memory. CComprehensive Physiology
  89. 89. Postle B. R. Berger J. S. Taich A. M. D’Esposito M. 2000 Activity in human frontal cortex associated with spatial working memory and saccadic behavior. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2 2 14
  90. 90. Damasio A. R. 1995 On Some Functions of the Human Prefrontal Cortex. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 769 241 52
  91. 91. Li J. Delgado M. R. Phelps E. A. 2011 How instructed knowledge modulates the neural systems of reward learning . Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 108 1 55 60
  92. 92. Hikosaka K. Watanabe M. 2004 Long- and short-range reward expectancy in the primate orbitofrontal cortex European Journal of Neuroscience 19 4 1046 54
  93. 93. Dum R. P. Strick P. L. 2002 An Unfolded Map of the Cerebellar Dentate Nucleus and its Projections to the Cerebral Cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology 89 1 634 9
  94. 94. Allen G. Mc Coll R. Barnard H. Ringe W. K. Fleckenstein J. Cullum C. M. 2005 Magnetic resonance imaging of cerebellar-prefrontal and cerebellar-parietal functional connectivity. Neuroimage 28 39 48
  95. 95. Yan H. Zuo-N X. Wang D. Wang J. Zhu C. Milham M. P. et al. 2009 Hemispheric asymmetry in cognitive division of anterior cingulate cortex: a resting-state functional connectivity study Neuroimage 47 4 1579 89
  96. 96. Schmahmann J. D. Macmore J Vangel M. 2009 Cerebellar stroke without motor deficit: clinical evidence for motor and non-motor domains within the human cerebellum Neuroscience 162 3 852 61
  97. 97. Schmahmann J. D. Sherman J. C. 1998 The cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome. Brain 121 Pt 4 561 79
  98. 98. Nobre A. C. Sebestyen G. N. Gitelman D. R. 1997 Functional localization of the system for visuospatial attention using positron emission tomography. Brain 120 3 515 33
  99. 99. Coull J. T. Nobre A. C. 1998 Where and when to pay attention: the neural systems for directing attention to spatial locations and to time intervals as revealed by both PET and fMRI. The Journal of Neuroscience 18 18 7426 35
  100. 100. Kim Y. H. Gitelman D. R. Nobre A. C. Parrish T. B. 1999 The large-scale neural network for spatial attention displays multifunctional overlap but differential asymmetry. Neuroimage 9 3 269 77
  101. 101. La Bar K. S. Gitelman D. R. Parrish T. B. Mesulam M. 1999 Neuroanatomic overlap of working memory and spatial attention networks: a functional MRI comparison within subjects. Neuroimage 10 6 695 704
  102. 102. Marvel C. L. Desmond J. E. 2010 The contributions of cerebro-cerebellar circuitry to executive verbal working memory. Cortex 46 7 880 95
  103. 103. Heath R. G. Harper J. W. 1974 Ascending projections of the cerebellar fastigial nucleus to the hippocampus, amygdala, and other temporal lobe sites: evoked potential and histological studies in monkeys and cats. Experimental NeurolologyJun. 13; 45 268 87
  104. 104. Lee G. P. Meador K. J. Loring D. W. Allison J. D. Brown W. S. Paul L. K. et al. 2004 Neural substrates of emotion as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Cognitive Behavioral Neurology 17 9 17
  105. 105. Bermpohl F. Pascual-Leone A. Amedi A. Merabet L. B. Fregni F. Gaab N. et al. 2006 Dissociable networks for the expectancy and perception of emotional stimuli in the human brain Neuroimage 30 588 600
  106. 106. Hofer A. Siedentopf C. M. Ischebeck A. Rettenbacher M. A. Verius M. Felber S. et al. 2007 Sex differences in brain activation patterns during processing of positively and negatively valenced emotional words. Psychological Medicine 37 1 109 19
  107. 107. Berntson G. G. Potolicchio S. J. Miller N. E. 1973 Evidence for higher functions of the cerebellum: eating and grooming elicited by cerebellar stimulation in cats. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 70 9 2497 9
  108. 108. Ball G. G. Micco D. J. Berntson G. G. 1974 Cerebellar stimulation in the rat: complex stimulation-bound oral behaviors and self-stimulation. Physiology & Behavior 13 1 123 7
  109. 109. Watson P. J. 1978 Nonmotor Functions of the Cerebellum. Psychological Bulletin 85 5 944 67
  110. 110. Supple W. F. Leaton R. N. Fanselow M. S. 1987 Effects of cerebellar vermal lesions on species-specific fear responses, neophobia, and taste-aversion learning in rats. Physiology & Behavior 39 5 579 86
  111. 111. Leaton R. N. Supple W. F. 1986 Cerebellar vermis: essential for long-term habituation of the acoustic startle response. Science 232 4749 513 5
  112. 112. Heath R. G. Cox A. W. Lustick L. S. 1974 Brain activity during emotional states. American Journal of Psychiatry 131 8 858 62
  113. 113. Blaine S. Nadhold J. Slaughter D. G. 1969 Effects of stimulating or destroying the deep cerebellar regions in man Journal of Neurosurgery 31 172 86
  114. 114. Cooper I. S. Amin I. Riklan M. Waltz J. M. Tung Pui. Poon M. D. 1976 Chronic cerebellar stimulation in epilepsy: clinical and anatomical studies. Archives of Neurology 33 8 559 70
  115. 115. Rapoport M. van Reekum R. Mayberg H. 2000 The role of the cerebellum in cognition and behavior: a selective review. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences 12 2 193 8
  116. 116. Kessler R. C. Berglund P. Demler O. Jin R. Merikangas K. R. Walters E. E. 2005 Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry 62 6 593 602
  117. 117. Kessler R. C. Chiu W. T. Demler O. Merikangas K. R. Walters E. E. 2005 Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry 62 6 617 27
  118. 118. Mineka S. Zinbarg R. 2006 A contemporary learning theory perspective on the etiology of anxiety disorders: It’s not what you thought it was. American Psychologist 61 1 10 26
  119. 119. American Psychiatric Association 2000 Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM IV-TR. 4th ed. American Psychiatric Association Washington, DC
  120. 120. Steinmetz J. E. Logue S. F. Miller D. P. 1993 Using signaled barpressing tasks to study the neural substrates of appetitive and aversive learning in rats: behavioral manipulations and cerebellar lesions. Behavioral Neuroscience 107 6 941 54
  121. 121. Dahhaoui M. Caston J. Auvray N. 1990 Role of the cerebellum in an avoidance conditioning task in the rat. Physiology & Behavior 47 1175 80
  122. 122. Schlund M. W. Cataldo M. F. 2010 Amygdala involvement in human avoidance, escape and approach behavior. Neuroimage 53 2 769 76
  123. 123. Holsboer F. 1999 The rationale for corticotropin-releasing hormone receptor (CRH-R) antagonists to treat depression and anxiety. Journal of Psychiatric Research 33 3 181 214
  124. 124. Dunn A. J. Berridge C. W. 1990 Physiological and behavioral responses to corticotropin-releasing factor administration: is CRF a mediator of anxiety or stress responses. Brain Research Reviews 15 71 100
  125. 125. Lacroix S. Rivest S. 1996 Role of cyclo-oxygenase pathways in the stimulatory influence of immune challenge on the transcription of a specific CRF receptor subtype in the rat brain. Journal of Chemical Neuroanatomy 10 1 53 71
  126. 126. Giardino L. Puglisi-Allegra S. Ceccatelli S. 1996 CRH-R1 mRNA expression in two strains of inbred mice and its regulation after repeated restraint stress. Molecular Brain Research 40 2 310 14
  127. 127. Servatius R. J. Beck K. D. Moldow R. L. Salameh G. 2005 A stress-induced anxious state in male rats: corticotropin-releasing hormone induces persistent changes in associative learning and startle reactivity. Biological Psychiatry 57 1 865 72
  128. 128. Nees F. Richter S. Lass-Hennemann J. Blumenthal T. D. Schächinger H. 2008 Inhibition of cortisol production by metyrapone enhances trace, but not delay, eyeblink conditioning Psychopharmacology199 2 183 90
  129. 129. Kuehl L. K. Lass-Hennemann J. Richter S. Blumenthal T. D. Oitzl M. Schächinger H. 2010 Accelerated trace eyeblink conditioning after cortisol IV-infusion Neurobiology of Learning and Memory94 4 547 53
  130. 130. Brennan F. X. Ottenweller J. E. Servatius R. J. 2001 Pharmacological suppression of corticosterone secretion in response to a physical stressor does not prevent the delayed persistent increase in circulating basal Stress 4 2 137 141
  131. 131. Herkenham M. Lynn A. B. Little M. D. Johnson M. R. Melvin L. S. De Costa B. R. et al. 1990 Cannabinoid receptor localization in brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 87 5 932
  132. 132. Navarro M. Hernández E. Muñoz R. M. del Arco I. Villanúa MA Carrera M. R. A. et al. 1997 Acute administration of the CB1 cannabinoid receptor antagonist SR 141716A induces anxiety-like responses in the rat. Neuroreport 8 2 491 6
  133. 133. Haller J. Bakos N. Szirmay M. Ledent C. Freund T. F. 2002 The effects of genetic and pharmacological blockade of the CB1 cannabinoid receptor on anxiety. European Journal of Neuroscience16 7 1395 8
  134. 134. Onaivi E. S. Green M. R. Martin B. R. 1990 Pharmacological characterization of cannabinoids in the elevated plus maze. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics253 3 1002 1009
  135. 135. de Fonseca F. R. Rubio P. Menzaghi F. Merlo-Pich E. Rivier J. Koob G. F. Navarro M. 1996 Corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) antagonist [D-Phe12, Nle21, 38, C alpha MeLeu37] CRF attenuates the acute actions of the highly potent cannabinoid receptor agonist HU-210 on defensive-withdrawal behavior in rats. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics27615664
  136. 136. Navarro M. Fernández-Ruiz J. J. De Miguel R. Hernández M. L. Cebeira M. Ramos J. A. 1993 An acute dose of delta 9-tetrahydrocannabinol affects behavioral and neurochemical indices of mesolimbic dopaminergic activity. Behavioural Brain Research57 1 37 46
  137. 137. Hollister L. E. 1986 Health aspects of cannabis. Pharmacological Reviews38 1 1 20
  138. 138. Kishimoto Y. Kano M. 2006 Endogenous cannabinoid signaling through the CB1 receptor is essential for cerebellum-dependent discrete motor learning. The Journal of Neuroscience26 34 8829 37
  139. 139. Skosnik P. D. Edwards C. R. O’Donnell B. F. Steffen A. Steinmetz J. E. Hetrick W. P. 2008 Cannabis use disrupts eyeblink conditioning: evidence for cannabinoid modulation of cerebellar-dependent learning Neuropsychopharmacology 33 6 1432 40
  140. 140. Noyes R. Jr. Clancy J. Hoenk P. R. 1980 The prognosis of anxiety neurosis. Archives of General Psychiatry 37 2 173
  141. 141. Kendall P. C. Brady E. U. Verduin T. L. 2001 Comorbidity in childhood anxiety disorders and treatment outcome. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 40 7 787 94
  142. 142. Garcia Coll C. Kagan J. 1984 Behavioral inhibition in young children. Child Development 55 3 1005 19
  143. 143. Kagan J. Moss H. A. 1962 Birth to maturity: A study in psychological development. Yale University Press
  144. 144. Hirshfeld D. Rosenbaum J. Biederman J. 1992 Stable Behavioral Inhibition and Its Association with Anxiety Disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 31 1 103 11
  145. 145. Schwartz C. E. Snidman N. 1999 Adolescent social anxiety as an outcome of inhibited temperament in childhood. of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 38 8 1008 15
  146. 146. Biederman J. Rosenbaum J. F. Hirshfeld D. R. Faraone S. V. Bolduc E. A. Gersten M. et al. 1990 Psychiatric correlates of behavioral inhibition in young children of parents with and without psychiatric disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry 47 1 21
  147. 147. Biederman J. Hirshfeld-Becker D. 2001 Further evidence of association between behavioral inhibition and social anxiety in children. American journal of Psychiatry 158 10 1673 79
  148. 148. Robinson J. Kagan J. Reznick J. S. 1992 The heritability of inhibited and uninhibited behavior: A twin study. Developmental Psychology 28 6 1030 37
  149. 149. Rosenbaum J. Biederman J. Hirshfeld D. 1991 Further evidence of an association between behavioral inhibition and anxiety disorders: Results from a family study of children from a non-clinical sample. Journal of Psychiatric Research 25 1 49 65
  150. 150. Rosenbaum J. F. Biederman J. 1992 Comorbidity of parental anxiety disorders as risk for childhood-onset anxiety in inhibited children. The American Journal of Psychiatry 149 4 475 81
  151. 151. Rosenbaum J. F. Biederman J. Hirshfeld-Becker D. R. Kagan J. Snidman N. Friedman D. et al. 2000 A controlled study of behavioral inhibition in children of parents with panic disorder and depression. American Journal of Psychiatry 157 12 2002 10
  152. 152. Paré W. P. 1989 Strain, age, but not gender, influence ulcer severity induced by water-restraint stress. Physiology & Behavior 45 3 627 32
  153. 153. Paré W. P. 1989 Stress ulcer susceptibility and depression in Wistar Kyoto (WKY) rats. Physiology & Behavior 46 6 993 8
  154. 154. Paré W. P. 1993 Passive-avoidance behavior in Wistar-Kyoto (WKY), Wistar, and Fischer-344 rats. Physiology & Behavior 54 5 845 52
  155. 155. Paré W. P. 1994 Open field, learned helplessness, conditioned defensive burying, and forced-swim tests in WKY rats. Physiology & Behavior 55 3 433 9
  156. 156. Redei E. Paré W. P. Aird F. Kluczynski J. 1994 Strain differences in hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal activity and stress ulcer. American Journal of Physiology-Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology 266 2 R353 60
  157. 157. Armario A. Gavaldà A. Martí J. 1995 Comparison of the behavioural and endocrine response to forced swimming stress in five inbred strains of rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology 20 8 879 90
  158. 158. Rittenhouse P. A. López-Rubalcava C. Stanwood G. D. Lucki I. 2002 Amplified behavioral and endocrine responses to forced swim stress in the Wistar-Kyoto rat Psychoneuroendocrinology 27 3 303 18
  159. 159. Servatius R. J. Jiao X. Beck K. D. Pang K. C. H. Minor T. R. 2008 Rapid avoidance acquisition in Wistar-Kyoto rats. Behavioural Brain Research 192 2 191 7
  160. 160. McAuley J. D. Stewart A. L. Webber E. S. Cromwell H. C. Servatius R. J. Pang K. C. H. 2009 Wistar-Kyoto rats as an animal model of anxiety vulnerability: support for a hypervigilance hypothesis. Behavioural Brain Research 204 1 162 8
  161. 161. Servatius R. J. Jiao X. Beck K. D. Pang K. 2008 Rapid avoidance acquisition in Wistar-Kyoto rats Behavioural Brain Research 192 2 191 97
  162. 162. Beck K. D. Jiao X. Pang K. C. H. Servatius R. J. 2010 Vulnerability factors in anxiety determined through differences in active-avoidance behavior Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 34 6 852 60
  163. 163. Ricart T. M. Jiao X. Pang K. C. H. Beck K. D. Servatius R. J. 2011 Classical and instrumental conditioning of eyeblink responses in Wistar-Kyoto and Sprague-Dawley rats Behavioural Brain Research 216 1 414 8
  164. 164. Ricart T. M. De Niear M. A. Jiao X. Pang K. C. H. Beck K. D. Servatius R. J. 2011 Deficient proactive interference of eyeblink conditioning in Wistar-Kyoto rats. Behavioural Brain Research 216 1 59 65
  165. 165. Jiao X. Pang K. C. H. Beck K. D. Minor T. R. Servatius R. J. 2011 Avoidance perseveration during extinction training in Wistar-Kyoto rats: an interaction of innate vulnerability and stressor intensity Behavioural Brain Research 221 1 98 107
  166. 166. Jiao X. Beck K. D. Pang K. C. H. Servatius R. J. 2011 Animal Models of Anxiety Vulnerability- The Wistar Kyoto Rat. Selek S, editor. Different View of Anxiety Disorders. Rijeka:InTech
  167. 167. Ricart T. M. Servatius R. J. Beck K. D. 2012 Acquisition of Active Avoidance Behavior as a Precursor to Changes in General Arousal in an Animal Model of PTSD. Ovuga E, editor. Post Traumatic Stress Disorders in a Global Context. Rijeka: InTech
  168. 168. Reznick J. S. Hegeman I. M. Kaufman E. R. Woods S. W. Jacobs M. 1992 Retrospective and concurrent self-report of behavioral inhibition and their relation to adult mental health Development and Psychopatholoy 4 301 21
  169. 169. Gladstone G. Parker G. 2005 Measuring a behaviorally inhibited temperament style: Development and initial validation of new self-report measures. Psychiatry Research 135 133 43
  170. 170. Spielberger C. Gorsuch R. Lushene R. 1970 Spielberger: Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press
  171. 171. Myers C. E. Van Meenen K. M. Mc Auley J. D. Beck K. D. Pang K. C. H. Servatius R. J. 2011 Behaviorally inhibited temperament is associated with severity of post-traumatic stress disorder symptoms and faster eyeblink conditioning in veterans. Stress 15 1 31 44
  172. 172. Myers C. E. Van Meenen K. M. Servatius R. J. 2012 Behavioral inhibition and PTSD symptoms in veterans. Psychiatry Research 196 2 271 6
  173. 173. Farber I. E. Spence K. W. 1953 Complex learning and conditioning as a function of anxiety. Journal of Psychology 45 2 120 5
  174. 174. Spence K. W. Beecroft R. S. 1954 Differential conditioning and level of anxiety. Journal of Experimental Psychology 48 5 399 403
  175. 175. Zhu-N J. Yung-H W. Kwok-Chong Chow. B. Chan-S Y. Wang-J J. 2006 The cerebellar-hypothalamic circuits: potential pathways underlying cerebellar involvement in somatic-visceral integration Brain Research Reviews 52 1 93 106
  176. 176. Dishman R. K. Nakamura Y. Garcia M. E. Thompson R. W. Dunn A. L. Blair S. N. 2000 Heart rate variability, trait anxiety, and perceived stress among physically fit men and women. International Journal of Psychophysiology 37 121 33
  177. 177. Cohen H. Benjamin J. Geva A. B. Matar M. A. Kaplan Z. Kotler M. 2000 Autonomic dysregulation in panic disorder and in post-traumatic stress disorder: application of power spectrum analysis of heart rate variability at rest and in response to recollection of trauma or panic attacks. Psychiatry Research 96 1 1 13
  178. 178. Bohlin G. Graham F. K. Silverstein L. D. Hackley S. A. 1981 Cardiac orinting and startle blink modification in novel and signal situations. Psychophysiology 18 5 603 11
  179. 179. Bradley M. M. Lang P. J. Cuthbert B. N. 1993 Emotion, Novelty, and the Startle Reflex: Habituation in Humans. Behavioral neuroscience 107 6 970 80
  180. 180. Kilts C. D. Kelsey J. E. Knight B. Ely T. D. Bowman F. D. Gross R. E. et al. 2006 The neural correlates of social anxiety disorder and response to pharmacotherapy. Neuropsychopharmacology 31 10 2243 53
  181. 181. Evans K. C. Wright C. I. Wedig M. M. Gold A. L. Pollack M. H. Rauch S. L. 2008 A functional MRI study of amygdala responses to angry schematic faces in social anxiety disorder. Depression and Anxiety 25 6 496 505
  182. 182. Warwick J. M. Carey P. Jordaan G. P. Dupont P. Stein D. J. 2008 Resting brain perfusion in social anxiety disorder: a voxel-wise whole brain comparison with healthy control subjects Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 32 5 1251 6
  183. 183. Shin L. M. Mc Nally R. J. Kosslyn S. M. Thompson W. L. Rauch S. L. Alpert N. M. et al. 1999 Regional Cerebral Blood Flow During Script-Driven Imagery in Childhood Sexual Abuse-Related PTSD: A PET Investigation. American Journal of Psychiatry 156 575 84
  184. 184. Bremner J. D. Narayan M. Staib L. H. Southwick S. M. Mc Glashan T. Charney D. S. 1999 Neural Correlates of Memories of Childhood Sexual Abuse in Women With and Without Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. The American journal of psychiatry 156 1787 95
  185. 185. Bremner J. D. Vythilingam M. Vermetten E. Southwick S. M. Mc Glashan T. Staib L. H. et al. 2003 Neural correlates of declarative memory for emotionally valenced words in women with posttraumatic stress disorder related to early childhood sexual abuse. Biological Psychiatry 53 10 879 89
  186. 186. Bonne O. Gilboa A. Louzoun Y. Brandes D. Yona I. Lester H. et al. 2003 Resting regional cerebral perfusion in recent posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry 54 10 1077 86
  187. 187. Yang P. Wu T. M. Hsu-C C. Ker-H J. 2004 Evidence of early neurobiological alternations in adolescents with posttraumatic stress disorder: a functional MRI study. Neuroscience Letters 370 1 13 8
  188. 188. Menzies L. Achard S. Chamberlain S. R. Fineberg N. Chen-H C. del Campo N. et al. 2007 Neurocognitive endophenotypes of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Brain 130 12 3223 36
  189. 189. Blair K. Shaywitz J. Smith B. W. et al. 2008 Response to emotional expressions in generalized social phobia and generalized anxiety disorder: evidence for separate disorders American Journal of Psychiatry 165 9 1193 1202
  190. 190. Chen J. 2011 A review of neuroimaging studies of anxiety disorders in China Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment 7 241 249
  191. 191. Schwartz C. E. Rauch S. L. 2004 Temperament and its implications for neuroimaging of anxiety disorders. CNS Spectrums 9 4 284 91
  192. 192. Etkin A. Klemenhagen K. C. Dudman J. T. Rogan M. T. Hen R. Kandel E. R. et al. 2004 Individual differences in trait anxiety predict the response of the basolateral amygdala to unconsciously processed fearful faces. Neuron 44 1043 55
  193. 193. Bishop S. J. 2008 Trait anxiety and impoverished prefrontal control of attention. Nature Neuroscience 12 1 92 98
  194. 194. Schienle A. Schäfer A. Stark R. Walter B. Vaitl D. 2005 Relationship between disgust sensitivity, trait anxiety and brain activity during disgust induction Neuropsychobiology 51 2 86 92
  195. 195. Blackford J. U. Avery S. N. Cowan R. L. Shelton R. C. Zald D. H. 2010 Sustained amygdala response to both novel and newly familiar faces characterizes inhibited temperament. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 6 5 621 9

Written By

Meghan D. Caulfield and Richard J. Servatius

Submitted: 24 April 2012 Published: 20 March 2013